Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-7c8c6479df-27gpq Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-03-27T00:10:23.444Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

4 - Olfaction and Memory

from Section I - Neurology, Neurophysiology and Neuropsychology: Olfactory Clues to Brain Development and Disorder

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  17 August 2009

Warrick J. Brewer
Affiliation:
Mental Health Research Institute of Victoria, Melbourne
David Castle
Affiliation:
University of Melbourne
Christos Pantelis
Affiliation:
University of Melbourne
Get access

Summary

The scent was so heavenly fine that tears welled into Baldini's eyes. He did not have to test it; he simply stood at the table in front of the mixing bottle and breathed. The perfume was glorious. It was to Amour and Psyche as a symphony is to the scratching of a lonely violin. And it was more. Baldini closed his eyes and watched as the most sublime memories were awakened within him. He saw himself as a young man walking through the evening gardens of Naples; he saw himself lying in the arms of a woman with dark curly hair and saw the silhouette of a bouquet of roses on the windowsill as the night wind passed by; he heard the random song of birds and the distant music from a harbor tavern; he heard whispering at his ear, he heard I-love-you and felt his hair ruffle with bliss, now! Now at this very moment! He forced open his eyes and groaned with pleasure. This perfume was not like any perfume known before. It was not a scent that made things smell better, not some sachet, some toiletry. It was something completely new, capable of creating a whole world, a magical rich world, and in an instant you forgot all the loathsomeness around you and felt so rich, so at ease, so free, so fine …

-from Perfume by Patrick Süskind

Introduction

As Süskind has reminded us in his evocative description of the power of scent, the slightest hint of perfume can transform the present into the past; it can re-create entire sensory experiences by providing an emotional link between past events initially experienced through separate senses; it can make memories seem real and tangible.

Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2006

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Abraham, A. & Mathai, K. V. (1983) The effect of right temporal lobe lesions on matching of smells. Neuropsychologia, 21, 277–81.Google Scholar
Alaoui-Ismaili, O., Vernet-Maury, E., Dittmar, A., et al. (1997) Odor hedonics: connection with emotional response estimated by autonomic parameters. Chem Senses, 22, 237–48.Google Scholar
Arnold, S. E. & Trojanowski, J. Q. (1996) Human fetal hippocampal development: I. Cytoarchitecture, myeloarchitecture, and neuronal morphologic features. J Comp Neurol, 367, 274–92.Google Scholar
Ayabe-Kanamura, S., Saito, S., Distel, H., et al. (1998) Differences and similarities in the perception of everyday odors. A Japanese-German cross-cultural study, Ann N Y Acad Sci, 855, 694–700.Google Scholar
Baylis, L. L., Rolls, E. T. & Baylis, G. C. (1995) Afferent connections of the caudolateral orbitofrontal cortex taste area of the primate. Neuroscience, 64, 801–12.Google Scholar
Buchanan, T. W., Tranel, D., & Adolphs, R. (2003) A specific role for the human amygdala in olfactory memory. Learn Mem, 10, 319–25.Google Scholar
Bensafi, M., Rouby, C., Farget, V., et al. (2002a) Autonomic nervous system responses to odours: the role of pleasantness and arousal. Chem Senses, 27, 703–9.Google Scholar
Bensafi, M., Rouby, C., Farget, V., et al. (2002b) Psychophysiological correlates of affects in human olfaction. Neurophysiol Clin, 32, 326–32.Google Scholar
Buchanan, T. W., Denburg, N. L., Tranel, D., et al. (2001) Verbal and nonverbal emotional memory following unilateral amygdala damage. Learn Mem, 8, 326–35.Google Scholar
Cain, W. (1979) To know with the nose: keys to odor identification. Science, 203, 467–70.Google Scholar
Cain, W. S., Schiet, F. T., Olsson, M. J., et al. (1995) Comparison of models of odor interaction. Chem Senses, 20, 625–37.Google Scholar
Carmichael, S. T. & Price, J. L. (1994) Architectonic subdivision of the orbital and medial prefrontal cortex in the macaque monkey. J Comp Neurol, 346, 366–402.Google Scholar
Carmichael, S. T. & Price, J. L. (1995) Sensory and premotor connections of the orbital and medial prefrontal cortex of macaque monkeys. J Comp Neurol, 363, 642–64.Google Scholar
Carmichael, S. T. & Price, J. L. (1996) Connectional networks within the orbital and medial prefrontal cortex of macaque monkeys. J Comp Neurol, 371, 179–207.Google Scholar
Cavada, C., Company, T., Tejedor, J., et al. (2000) The anatomical connections of the macaque monkey orbitofrontal cortex. A review. Cereb Cortex, 10, 220–42.Google Scholar
Chiavaras, M. M., LeGoualher, G., Evans, A., et al. (2001) Three-dimensional probabilistic atlas of the human orbitofrontal sulci in standardized stereotaxic space. Neuroimage, 13, 479–96.Google Scholar
Chu, S. & Downess, J. J. (2000) Odour-evoked autobiographical memories: psychological investigations of proustian phenomena. Chem Senses, 25, 111–16.Google Scholar
Chu, S., & Downess, J. J. (2002) Proust nose best: odors are better cues of autobiographical memory. Mem Cognit, 30, 511–18.Google Scholar
Dade, L. A., Zatorre, R. J., Evans, A. C., et al. (2001) Working memory in another dimension: functional imaging of human olfactory working memory. Neuroimage, 14, 650–60.Google Scholar
Dade, L. A., Zatorre, R. J. & Jones-Gotman, M. (2002) Olfactory learning: convergent findings from lesion and brain imaging studies in humans. Brain, 125, 86–101.Google Scholar
Dalton, P. (1996) Odor perception and beliefs about risk. Chem Senses, 21, 447–58.Google Scholar
Dalton, P. (2000) Psychophysical and behavioral characteristics of olfactory adaptation. Chem Senses, 25, 487–92.Google Scholar
Wijk, R. A. & Cain, W. S. (1994) Odor quality: discrimination versus free and cued identification. Percept Psychophys, 56, 12–18.Google Scholar
Distel, H. & Hudson, R. (2001) Judgement of odor intensity is influenced by subjects' knowledge of the odor source. Chem Senses, 26, 247–51.Google Scholar
Distel, H., Ayabe-Kanamura, S., Martinez-Gomez, M., et al. (1999) Perception of everyday odors: correlation between intensity, familiarity and strength of hedonic judgement. Chem Senses, 24, 191–9.Google Scholar
Ehrlichman, H., Brown, S., Zhu, J., et al. (1995) Startle reflex modulation during exposure to pleasant and unpleasant odors. Psychophysiology, 32, 150–4.Google Scholar
Eichenbaum, H. (1998) Using olfaction to study memory. Ann N Y Acad Sci, 855, 657–69.Google Scholar
Eichenbaum, H., Clegg, R. A. & Feeley, A. (1983) Reexamination of functional subdivisions of the rodent prefrontal cortex. Exp Neurol, 79, 434–51.Google Scholar
Engen, T. (1983) The human uses of olfaction. Am J Otolaryngol, 4, 250–1.Google Scholar
Engen, T. (1987) Remembering odors and their names. Am Scientist, 75, 497–503.Google Scholar
Engen, T. & Ross, B. M. (1973) Long-term memory of odors with and without verbal descriptions. J Exp Psychol, 100, 221–7.Google Scholar
Frey, S. & Petrides, M. (2000) Orbitofrontal cortex: A key prefrontal region for encoding information. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA, 97, 8723–7.Google Scholar
Fulbright, R. K., Skudlarski, P., Lacadie, C. M., et al. (1998) Functional MR imaging of regional brain responses to pleasant and unpleasant odors. Am J Neuroradiol, 19, 1721–6.Google Scholar
Fuster, J. M. (1997) Network memory. Trends Neurosci, 20, 451–9.Google Scholar
Goldman-Rakic, P. S. (1995) Architecture of the prefrontal cortex and the central executive. Ann NY Acad Sci, 769, 71–83.Google Scholar
Gottfried, J. A. & Dolan, R. J. (2003) The nose smells what the eye sees: crossmodal visual facilitation of human olfactory perception. Neuron, 39, 375–86.Google Scholar
Gottfried, J. A., Smith, A. P., Rugg, M. D., et al. (2004) Remembrance of odors past: human olfactory cortex in cross-modal recognition memory. Neuron, 42, 687–95.Google Scholar
Herz, R. S. & Cupchik, G. C. (1995) The emotional distinctiveness of odor-evoked memories. Chem Senses, 20, 517–28.Google Scholar
Herz, R. S., Eliassen, J., Beland, S., et al. (2004) Neuroimaging evidence for the emotional potency of odor-evoked memory. Neuropsychologia, 42, 371–8.Google Scholar
Holmes, D. D. (1970) Differential changes in affective intensity and forgetting of unpleasant personal experiences. J Personality Soc Psychol, 15, 234.Google Scholar
Hummel, T. & Kobal, G. (1992) Differences in human evoked potentials related to olfactory or trigeminal chemosensory activation. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol, 84, 84–9.Google Scholar
Jinks, A., Laing, D. G., Hutchinson, I., et al. (1998) Temporal processing of odor mixtures reveals that identification of components takes precedence over temporal information in olfactory memory. Ann NY Acad Sci, 855, 834–6.Google Scholar
Jones-Gotman, M. & Zatorre, R. J. (1988) Olfactory identification deficits in patients with focal cerebral excision. Neuropsychologia, 26, 387–400.Google Scholar
Koenig, O., Bourron, G. & Royet, J. P. (2000) Evidence for separate perceptive and semantic memories for odours: a priming experiment. Chem Senses, 25, 703–8.Google Scholar
Kosslyn, S. M., Chabris, C. F., Marsolek, C. J., et al. (1992) Categorical versus coordinate spatial relations: computational analyses and computer simulations. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform, 18, 562–77.Google Scholar
Lehrner, J. P., Walla, P., Laska, M., et al. (1999) Different forms of human odor memory: a developmental study. Neurosci Letters, 272, 17–20.Google Scholar
Levy, L. M., Henkin, R. I., Lin, C. S., et al. (1999) Odor memory induces brain activation as measured by functional MRI. J Comp Assisted Tom, 23, 487–98.Google Scholar
Levy, D. A., Manns, J. R., Hopkins, R. O., et al. (2003) Impaired visual and odor recognition memory span in patients with hippocampal lesions. Learn Mem, 10, 531–6.Google Scholar
Lipton, P. A., Alvarez, P. & Eichenbaum, H. (1999) Crossmodal associative memory representations in rodent orbitofrontal cortex. Neuron, 22, 349–59.Google Scholar
Lorig, T. S. (1999) On the similarity of odor and language perception. Neurosci Biobehav Rev, 23, 391–8.Google Scholar
Lorig, T. S., Elmes, D. G. & Yoerg, V. L. (1998) Chemosensory alteration of information processing. Ann NY Acad Sci, 855, 591–7.Google Scholar
Maylor, E. A., Carter, S. M. & Hallett, E. L. (2002) Preserved olfactory cuing of autobiographical memories in old age. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci, 57, 41–6.Google Scholar
McNamara, T. P., Halpin, J. A. & Hardy, J. K. (1992) The representation and integration in memory of spatial and nonspatial information. Mem Cognit, 20, 519–32.Google Scholar
Melcher, J. M. & Schooler, J. W. (1996) The misremembrance of wines past: Verbal and perceptual expertise differentially mediate verbal overshadowing of taste memory. J Mem Language, 35, 231–45.Google Scholar
Mohr, C., Röhrenbach, C. M., Landis, T., et al. (2001) Associations to smell are more pleasant than to sound. J Clin Exp Neuropsychology, 23, 484–9.Google Scholar
Mouly, A. M. & Gervais, R. (2002) Polysynaptic potentiation at different levels of rat olfactory pathways following learning. Learn Mem, 9, 66–75.Google Scholar
Mouly, A. M., Fort, A., Ben-Boutayab, N., et al. (2001) Olfactory learning induces differential long-lasting changes in rat central olfactory pathways. Neuroscience, 102, 11–21.Google Scholar
Murphy, C. & Cain, W. S. (1986) Odor identification: the blind are better. Physiol Behav, 37, 177–180.Google Scholar
O'Doherty, J. P., Deichmann, R., Critchley, H. D., et al. (2002) Neural responses during anticipation of a primary taste reward. Neuron, 33, 815–26.Google Scholar
Olsson, M. J. (1999) Implicit testing of odor memory: instances of positive and negative repetition priming. Chem Senses, 24, 347–50.Google Scholar
Olsson, M. J. & Cain, W. S. (2003) Implicit and explicit memory for odors: hemispheric differences. Mem Cognit, 31, 44–50.Google Scholar
Olsson, M. J. & Friden, M. (2001) Evidence of odor priming: edibility judgements are primed differently between the hemispheres. Chem Senses, 26, 117–23.Google Scholar
Parr, W. V., Heatherbell, D. & White, K. G. (2002) Demystifying wine expertise: olfactory threshold, perceptual skill and semantic memory in expert and novice wine judges. Chem Senses, 27, 747–55.Google Scholar
Petrides, M. (1995) Functional organization of the human frontal cortex for mnemonic processing. Evidence from neuroimaging studies. Ann NY Acad Sci, 769, 85–96.Google Scholar
Powell, T. P., Cowan, W. M. & Raisman, G. (1965) The central olfactory connexions. J Anat, 99, 791–813.Google Scholar
Price, J. L., Carmichael, S. T. & Drevets, W. C. (1996) Networks related to the orbital and medial prefrontal cortex: a substrate for emotional behavior?Prog Brain Res, 107, 523–36.Google Scholar
Rausch, R., Serafetinides, E. A. & Crandall, P. H. (1977) Olfactory memory in patients with anterior temporal lobectomy. Cortex, 13, 445–52.Google Scholar
Rolls, E. T. (2000) Memory systems in the brain. Ann Rev Psychol, 51, 599–630.Google Scholar
Rolls, E. T. & Baylis, L. L. (1994) Gustatory, olfactory and visual convergence within the primate orbitofrontal cortex. J Neuroscience, 14, 5437–52.Google Scholar
Rolls, E. T., Critchley, H. D. & Treves, A. (1996) Representation of olfactory information in the primate orbitofrontal cortex. J Neurophysiol, 75, 1982–96.Google Scholar
Rosenbluth, R., Grossman, E. S. & Kaitz, M. (2000) Performance of early-blind and sighted children on olfactory tasks. Perception, 29, 101–10.Google Scholar
Rosenkilde, C. E., Bauer, R. H. & Fuster, J. M. (1981) Single cell activity in ventral prefrontal cortex of behaving monkeys. Brain Res, 209, 375–94.Google Scholar
Royet, J. P., Paugam-Moisy, H., Rouby, C., et al. (1996) Is short-term odour recognition predictable from odour profile?Chem Senses, 21, 553–66.Google Scholar
Royet, J. P., Koenig, O., Gregoire, M. C., et al. (1999) Functional anatomy of perceptual and semantic processing for odors. J Cog Neurosci, 11, 94–109.Google Scholar
Royet, J. P., Zald, D. H., Versace, R., et al. (2000) Emotional responses to pleasant and unpleasant olfactory, visual, and auditory stimuli: a positron emission tomography study. J Neuroscience, 15, 7752–9.Google Scholar
Rubin, D. C., Groth, E. & Goldsmith, D. J. (1984) Olfactory cuing of autobiographical memory. Am J Psychol, 97, 493–507.Google Scholar
Sanides, F. & Hoffmann, J. (1969) Cyto- and myeloarchitecture of the visual cortex of the cat and of the surrounding integration cortices. J Hirnforsch, 11, 79–104.Google Scholar
Savage, R., Combs, D. R., Pinkston, J. B., et al. (2002) The role of temporal lobe and orbitofrontal cortices in olfactory memory function. Arch Clin Neuropsychology, 17, 305–18.Google Scholar
Savic, I. (2001) Processing of odorous signals in humans. Brain Res Bull, 54, 307–12.Google Scholar
Savic, I. & Berglund, H. (2004) Passive perception of odors and semantic circuits. Hum Brain Mapp, 21, 271–8.Google Scholar
Savic, I., Gulyas, B., Larsson, M., et al. (2000) Olfactory functions are mediated by parallel and hierarchical processing. Neuron, 26, 735–45.Google Scholar
Schoenbaum, G., Chiba, A. A. & Gallagher, M. (1998) Orbitofrontal cortex and basolateral amygdala encode expected outcomes during learning. Nat Neurosci, 1, 155–9.Google Scholar
Süskind, P. (1976) Perfume. Penguin: London.
Thorpe, S. J., Rolls, E. T. & Maddison, S. (1983) The orbitofrontal cortex: neuronal activity in the behaving monkey. Exp Brain Res, 49, 93–115.Google Scholar
Tremblay, L. & Schultz, W. (1999) Relative reward preference in primate orbitofrontal cortex. Nature, 398, 704–8.Google Scholar
Tulving, E. & Schacter, D. L. (1990) Priming and human memory systems. Science, 247, 301–6.Google Scholar
Vasterling, J. J., Brailey, K. & Sutker, P. B. (2000) Olfactory identification in combat-related posttraumatic stress disorder. J Trauma Stress, 13, 241–53.Google Scholar
Vermetten, E. & Bremner, J. D. (2003) Olfaction as a traumatic reminder in posttraumatic stress disorder: case reports and review. J Clin Psychiatry, 64, 202–7.Google Scholar
Wakefield, C. E., Homewood, J. & Taylor, A. J. (2004) Cognitive compensations for blindness in children: an investigation using odour naming. Perception, 33, 429–42.Google Scholar
Walla, P., Hufnagl, B., Lehrner, J., et al. (2003) Olfaction and depth of word processing: a magnetoencephalographic study. Neuroimage, 18, 104–16.Google Scholar
Watanabe, M. (1996) Reward expectancy in primate prefrontal neurons. Nature, 382, 629–32.Google Scholar
Watanabe, M. (1998) Cognitive and motivational operations in primate prefrontal neurons. Rev Neurosci, 9, 225–41.Google Scholar
White, I. M. & Wise, S. P. (1999) Rule-dependent neuronal activity in the prefrontal cortex. Exp Brain Res, 126, 315–35.Google Scholar
White, T. L. (1998) Olfactory memory: The long and short of it. Chem Senses, 23, 433–41.Google Scholar
Yamamoto, T., Oomura, Y., Nishino, H., et al. (1984) Monkey orbitofrontal neuron activity during emotional and feeding behaviorsBrain Res Bull, 12, 441–3.Google Scholar
Zajonc, R. B. (1968) Attitudinal effects of mere exposure. J Personality Soc Psy, 9, 1.Google Scholar
Zald, D. H. & Pardo, J. (2000) Functional neuroimaging of the olfactory system in humans. Int J Psychophysiol, 36, 165–81.Google Scholar
Zald, D. H. & Pardo, J. V. (1997) Emotion, olfaction, and the human amygdala: amygdala activation during aversive olfactory stimulation. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA, 94, 4119–24.Google Scholar
Zald, D. H. & Kim, S. W. (1996a) Anatomy and function of the orbital frontal cortex. II. Function and relevance to obsessive-compulsive disorder. J Neuropsychiatry, 8, 249–61.Google Scholar
Zald, D. H. & Kim, S. W. (1996b) Anatomy and function of the orbital frontal cortex, I: anatomy, neurocircuitry, and obsessive-compulsive disorder. J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci, 8, 125–38.Google Scholar
Zatorre, R. & Jones-Gotman, M. (1991) Human olfactory discrimination after unilateral frontal or temporal lobectomy. Brain, 114, 71–84.Google Scholar
Zatorre, R., Jones-Gotman, M. & Rouby, C. (2000) Neural mechanisms involved in odor pleasantness and intensity judgements. Neuroreport, 11, 2711–16.Google Scholar
Zellner, D. A. & Kautz, M. A. (1990) Color affects perceived odor intensity. J Exp Psychol Hum PerceptPerform, 16, 391–7.Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×