Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-848d4c4894-4hhp2 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-05-06T18:10:01.533Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

22 - Self-consciousness: an integrative approach from philosophy, psychopathology and the neurosciences

from Part III - Disturbances of the self: the case of schizophrenia

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  18 December 2009

Tilo Kircher
Affiliation:
Eberhard-Karls-Universität Tübingen, Germany
Anthony David
Affiliation:
Institute of Psychiatry, London
Get access

Summary

Abstract

In this chapter, we want to try and integrate the divergent lines introduced in the other parts of this book. We propose a model of self-consciousness derived from phenomenology, philosophy, the cognitive and neurosciences. We will then give an overview of research data on self-processing from various fields and link it to our model. Some aspects of the disturbances of the self in pathological states such as brain lesions and schizophrenia will be discussed. Finally, the clinically important concept of insight into a disease and its neurocognitive origin will be introduced. We argue that self-consciousness is a valid construct and, as shown in this chapter, it is possible that it can be studied with the instruments of cognitive neuroscience.

Introduction

The self as an entity distinct from the other has entered western thought through Greek philosophy (see chapter 1, this volume, for details). Throughout history, a myriad of different notions, starting from theology, philosophy, psychoanalysis, to early psychological concepts, psychopathology, the social sciences, and, more recently, cognitive psychology, neurology and the neurosciences have been developed. With the advent of scientific interest in consciousness towards the end of the twentieth century, self-consciousness has also become a topic taken up by the neuroscientific community. As a first phenomenological approximation based on commonly shared experience, we know that we are the same person across time, that we are the author of our thoughts/actions, and that we are distinct from the environment.

Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2003

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Allport, G. W. (1955). Becoming. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press
Amador, X. F. & David, A. S. (1997). Insight and Psychosis. New York: Oxford University Press
Amador, X. F., Strauss, D. H., Yale, S. A. et al. (1993). Assessment of insight in psychosis. American Journal of Psychiatry, 150, 873–9Google Scholar
Anderson, J. R. & Reder, L. M. (1979). An elaborative processing explanation of depth of processing. In Levels of Processing in Human Memory, ed. L. S. Cermak & F. I. M. Craik, pp. 385–403. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum
Armstrong, D. (1980). The Nature of Mind and Other Essays. Ithaca, NY: Cornell University Press
Bentall, R. P., Kinderman, P. & Kaney, S. (1994). The self, attributional processes and abnormal beliefs: towards a model of persecutory delusions. Behavioral Research and Therapy, 32, 331–41Google Scholar
Bermudez, J. L., Marcel, A. & Eilan, N. (1995). The Body and the Self. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press
Berze, J. (1914). Die primäre Insuffizienz der psychischen Aktivität. Leipzig: Franz Deuticke
Blakemore, S. J., Goodbody, S. J. & Wolpert, D. M. (1998a). Predicting the consequences of our own actions: the role of sensorimotor context estimation. Journal of Neuroscience, 18, 7511–18Google Scholar
Blakemore, S. J., Wolpert, D. M. & Frith, C. D. (1998b). Central cancellation of self-produced tickle sensation. Nature and Neuroscience, 1, 635–40Google Scholar
Bleuler, E. (1916). Lehrbuch der Psychiatrie. (Reprinted 1983.) Berlin: Springer
Block, N., Flanagan, O. & Güzeldere, G. (1997). The Nature of Consciousness. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press
Chung, K. F., Chen, E. Y. H., Lam, L. C. W., Chen, R. Y. L. & Chan, C. K. Y. (1997). How are psychotic symptoms perceived? A comparison between patients, relatives and the general public. Australian and New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry, 31, 756–61Google Scholar
Churchland, P. (1995). The Engine of Reason, the Seat of the Soul: A Philosophical Journey into the Brain. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press
Collins, A. A., Remington, G. J., Coulter, K. & Birkett, K. (1997). Insight, neurocognitive function and symptom clusters in chronic schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Research, 27, 37–44Google Scholar
Cooley, C. H. (1902). Human Nature and the Social Order. New York: Scribner
Craik, F. I. M., Moroz, T. M., Moscovitch, M. et al. (1999). In search of the self: a positron emission tomography study. Psychological Science, 10, 26–34Google Scholar
Cuesta, M. J. & Peralta, V. (1994). Lack of insight in schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 20, 359–66Google Scholar
Cuesta, M. J., Peralta, V., Caro, F. & Leon, J. (1995). Is poor insight in psychotic disorders associated with poor performance on the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test? American Journal of Psychiatry, 152, 1380–2Google Scholar
Daprati, E., Franck, N., Georgieff, N. et al. (1997). Looking for the agent: an investigation into consciousness of action and self-consciousness in schizophrenic patients. Cognition, 65, 71–86Google Scholar
David, A. S. (1990). Insight and psychosis. British Journal of Psychiatry, 156, 798–808Google Scholar
David, A., Os, J., Jones, P. et al. (1995). Insight and psychotic illness. Cross-sectional and longitudinal associations. British Journal of Psychiatry, 167, 621–8Google Scholar
David, A., Kemp, R., Smith, L. & Fahy, T. (1996). Multiple personality and schizophrenia. In Methods in Madness: Case Studies in Cognitive Neuropsychiatry, ed. P. Halligan & J. Marshall, pp. 123–46. East Sussex: Lawrence Erlbaum
Dehaene, S. & Naccache, L. (2001). Towards a cognitive neuroscience of consciousness: basic evidence and a workspace framework. Cognition, 79, 1–37Google Scholar
Dennett, D. (1991). Consciousness Explained. Boston: Little, Brown
Dickerson, F. B., Boronow, J. J., Ringel, N. & Parente, F. (1997). Lack of insight among outpatients with schizophrenia. Psychiatric Services, 48, 195–9Google Scholar
Eysenck, M. W. & Eysenck, M. C. (1979). Processing depth, elaboration of encoding, memory stores, and expended processing capacity. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning and Memory, 5, 472–84Google Scholar
Feinberg, I. (1978). Efference copy and corollary discharge: implications for thinking and its disorders. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 4, 636–40Google Scholar
Ferguson, T. J., Rule, G. R. & Carlson, D. (1983). Memory for personally relevant information. Journal of Personal and Social Psychology, 44, 251–61Google Scholar
Fletcher, P. C., Frith, C. D., Grasby, P. M. et al. (1995). Brain systems for encoding and retrieval of auditory-verbal memory. An in vivo study in humans. Brain, 118, 401–16Google Scholar
Fodor, J. (1983). The Modularity of Mind. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press Bradford Books
Fourneret, P. & Jeannerod, M. (1998). Limited conscious monitoring of motor performance in normal subjects. Neuropsychologia, 36, 1133–40Google Scholar
Franck, N., Farrer, C., Georgieff, N. et al. (2001). Defective recognition of one's own actions in patients with schizophrenia. American Journal of Psychiatry, 158, 454–9Google Scholar
Frith, C. D. (1992). The Cognitive Neuropsychology of Schizophrenia. Hove, UK: Lawrence Erlbaum
Frith, C. D. & Done, D. J. (1989). Experiences of alien control in schizophrenia reflect a disorder in the central monitoring of action. Psychological Medicine, 19, 359–63Google Scholar
Gallois, P., Ovelacq, E., Hautecoeur, P. & Dereux, J. F. (1988). Disconnexion et reconnaissance des visages. [Disconnection and recognition of faces. A case with lesions of the left visual cortex and the splenium.] Revue Neurologique de Paris, 144, 113–19Google Scholar
Gallup, G. G. (1970). Chimpanzees: self-recognition. Science, 167, 86–7Google Scholar
Garety, P. A. & Hemsley, D. R. (1987). Characteristics of delusional experience. European Archives of Psychiatry and Neurological Science, 236, 294–8Google Scholar
Gruhle, H. W. (1932). Die Schizophrenie: Allgemeine Symptomatologie. Die Psychopathologie. In: Handbuch der Geisteskrankheiten, ed. O. Bumke, pp. 135–210. Berlin: Springer Verlag
Hattie, J. (1992). Self-concept. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum
Henry, M. (1963). L'essence de la manifestation. Paris: PUF
Henry, M. (1965). Philosophie et phénoménologie du corps. Paris: PFU
Higgins, E. T. (1987). Self-discrepancy: a theory relating self and affect. Psychological Review, 94, 319–40Google Scholar
Hodges, J. R. & McCarthy, R. A. (1993). Autobiographical amnesia resulting from bilateral paramedian thalamic infarction. A case study in cognitive neurobiology. Brain, 116, 40Google Scholar
Hong, C. C., Gillin, J. C., Dow, B. M., Wu, J. & Buchsbaum, M. S. (1995). Localized and lateralized cerebral glucose metabolism associated with eye movements during REM sleep and wakefulness: a positron emission tomography (PET) study. Sleep, 18, 570–80Google Scholar
Husserl, E. (1922). Ideen zu einer reinen Phänomenologie. Augemeine Einf[w2]uhrung in die reine Phänomenologie, vol. 1, 2nd edn, p. ⅹ–323. Halle an der Saale: Max Niemeyer
Indefrey, P. & Levelt, W. J. M. (2000). The neural correlates of language production. In The New Cognitive Sciences, ed. M. S. Gazzaniga, Boston, MA: pp. 845–64. MIT Press
James, W. (1950). Principles of Psychology. New York: Dover Publications
Jaspers, K. (1913). Allgemeine Psychopathologie. Berlin: Springer
Jaspers, K. (1963). General Psychopathology. Manchester: Manchester University Press
Jeannerod, M. (1999). The 25th Bartlett lecture: to act or not to act: perspectives on the representation of actions. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 52, A, 1–29Google Scholar
Kapur, S., Craik, F. I., Jones, C. et al. (1995). Functional role of the prefrontal cortex in retrieval of memories: a PET study. Neuroreport, 6, 1880–4Google Scholar
Keefe, R. S. E. (1998). The neurobiology of disturbances of self. In Insight and Psychosis, ed. X. F. Amador & A. S. David, pp. 142–73. New York: Oxford University Press
Keenan, J. M. (1993). An exemplar model can explain Klein and Loftus' results. In Advances in Social Cognition, ed. T. K. Srull & R. S. Wyer, pp. 69–78. New York: Erlbaum
Keenan, J. M. & Baillet, S. D. (1983). Memory for personally and socially significant events. In Attention and Performance, ed. R. S. Nickerson, pp. 651–69. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum
Keith, L. K. & Bracken, B. A. (1996). Self-concept instrumentation: a historical and evaluative review. In Handbook of Self-Concept, ed. B. A. Bracken, pp. 91–171. New York: John Wiley
Kihlstrom, J. F. (1993). What does the self look like? In Advances in Social Cognition, ed. T. K. Srull & R. S. Wyer, pp. 79–90. New York: Erlbaum
Kihlstrom, J. F. & Cantor, N. (1984). Mental representations of the self. Advances in Experimantal and Social Psychology, 17, 1–47Google Scholar
Kinderman, P. & Bentall, R. P. (1996). Self-discrepancies and persecutory delusions: evidence for a model of paranoid ideation. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 105, 106–13Google Scholar
Kinney, H. C., Brody, B. A., Kloman, A. S. & Gilles, F. H. (1988). Sequence of central nervous system myelination in human infancy. II. Patterns of myelination in autopsied infants. Journal of Neuropathology and Experimental Neurology, 47, 217–34Google Scholar
Kinsbourne, M. (1995). Awareness of one's own body: an attentional theory of its nature, development and brain basis. In The Body and the Self, ed. J. L. Bermudez, A. Marcel & N. Eilan, pp. 205–23. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press
Kircher, T. T. J., Brammer, M. J., Simmons, A. & McGuire, P. K. (2000). Pausing for thought: Activation of left superior temporal sulcus when subjects pause during speech. submitted
Kircher, T. T. J., Liddle, P. F., Brammer, M. J. et al. (2001a). Neural correlates of formal thought disorder in schizophrenia. Archives of General Psychiatry, 58, 769–74Google Scholar
Kircher, T., Baar, S., Plewnia, C. & Bartels, M. (2002a). Impaiment of facial self recognition in schizophrenia. in preparation
Kircher, T. T. J., Brammer, M. J., Bullmore, E. et al. (2002b). The neural correlates of intentional and incidental self processing. Neuropsychologia, 40, 683–92Google Scholar
Kircher, T. T. J., Senior, C., Phillips, M. L. et al. (2001b). Recognising one's own face. Cognition, 78, B1–15Google Scholar
Klatzky, R. L. & Forrest, F. H. (1984). Recognizing familiar and unfamiliar faces. Memory and Cognition, 12, 60–70Google Scholar
Klein, S. B. & Loftus, J. (1988). The nature of self-referent encoding: the contributions of elaborative and organizational processes. Journal of Personal and Social Psychology, 55, 5–11Google Scholar
Klein, S. B. & Loftus, J. (1993). The mental representation of trait and autobiographical knowledge about the self. In The Mental Representation of Trait and Autobiographical Knowledge about the Self. Advances in Social Cognition, ed, T. K. Srull & R. S. Wyer, pp. 1–49. Hillsdale, USA: Lawrence Erlbaum
Knoblich, G. & Kircher, T. (2002). Deceiving oneself about control: sensitivity for mapping changes between movements and their consequences. submitted
Kraepelin, E. (1913). Ein Lehrbuch für Studierende und Ärzte. Leipzig: Barth
Krause, B. J., Schmidt, D., Mottaghy, F. M. et al. (1999). Episodic retrieval activates the precuneus irrespective of the imagery content of word pair associates. A PET study. Brain, 122, 255–63Google Scholar
Kronfeld, A. (1922). Ueber schizophrene Veränderungen des Bewutseins der Aktivität. Zeitschrift für die gesamte Neurologie und Psychiatrie, 74, 15–68Google Scholar
Lane, R. D., Reiman, E. M., Bradley, M. M. et al. (1997). Neuroanatomical correlates of pleasant and unpleasant emotion. Neuropsychologia, 35, 1437–44Google Scholar
Leicester, J., Sidman, M., Stoddard, L. T. & Mohr, J. P. (1969). Some determinants of visual neglect. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery and Psychiatry, 32, 580–7Google Scholar
Lewis, A. (1934). The psychopathology of insight. Br. J. Med. Psychol., 14, 332–48Google Scholar
Linville, P. W. (1987). Self-complexity as a cognitive buffer against stress-related illness and depression. Journal of Personal and Social Psychology, 52, 663–76Google Scholar
Locke, J. (1959). An Essay Concerning Human Understanding. New York: Dover Publications
Lord, C. G. (1980). Schemas and images as memory aids: two models of processing social information. Journal of Personal and Social Psychology, 38, 257–69Google Scholar
Luria, A. R. (1969). Frontal lobe syndromes. In Handbook of Clinical Neurology, ed. P. J. Vinken & G. W. Bruyn, pp. 725–57. Amsterdam: North Holland
Lycan, W. G. (1997). Consciousness and Experience. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press
Lysaker, P. H., Bell, M. D., Bryson, G. & Kaplan, E. (1998). Neurocognitive function and insight in schizophrenia: support for an association with impairments in executive function but not with impairments in global function. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 97, 297–301Google Scholar
Maki, R. H. & Carlson, A. K. (1993). Knowledge of the self: is it special? In Advances in Social Cognition, ed. T. K. Srull & R. S. Wyer, pp. 101–110. New York: Erlbaum
Markus, H. (1977). Self-schemata and processing information about the self. Journal of Personal and Social Psychology, 35, 63–78Google Scholar
Marsh, H. W. & Hattie, J. (1996). Theoretical perspectives on the structure of self-concept. In Handbook of Self-Concept, ed. B. A. Bracken. New York: John Wiley
McGlynn, S. M. & Schacter, D. L. (1989). Unawareness of deficits in neuropsychological syndromes. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 11, 143–205Google Scholar
McGuire, P. K., Silbersweig, D. A., Wright, I. et al. (1995). Abnormal monitoring of inner speech: a physiological basis for auditory hallucinations. Lancet, 346, 596–600Google Scholar
Mead, G. H. (1934). Mind, Self and Society. Chicago: University of Chicago Press
Merleau-Ponty, M. (1965). Phänomenologie der Wahrnehmung. Berlin: Walter de Gruyter
Metzinger, T. (1995). Conscious Experience. Paderborn: Schöningh-Verlag
Miller, L. A. (1992). Impulsivity, risk-taking, and the ability to synthesize fragmented information after frontal lobectomy. Neuropsychologia, 30, 69–79Google Scholar
Mohamed, S., Fleming, S., Penn, D. L. & Spaulding, W. (1999). Insight in schizophrenia: its relationship to measures of executive functions. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 187, 525–31Google Scholar
Neisser, U. (1988). Five kinds of self-knowledge. Philosophical Psychology, 1, 35–59Google Scholar
Parker, S. T., Mitchell, R. W. & Boccia, M. C. (1994). Self Awareness in Animals and Humans. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press
Parnas, J. (2000). The self and intentionality in the pre-psychotic stages of schizophrenia. In Exploring the Self, ed. D. Zahavi, pp. 115–47. Amsterdam: John Benjamin
Phillips, M. L., Young, A. W., Senior, C. et al. (1997). A specific neural substrate for perceiving facial expressions of disgust. Nature, 389, 495–8Google Scholar
Pilowsky, T., Yirmiya, N., Arbelle, S. & Mozes, T. (2000). Theory of mind abilities of children with schizophrenia, children with autism, and normally developing children. Schizophrenia Research, 42, 145–55Google Scholar
Povinelli, D. J. & Preuss, T. M. (1995). Theory of mind: evolutionary history of a cognitive specialization. Trends in Neuroscience, 18, 418–24Google Scholar
Preilowski, B. (1979). Consciousness after complete surgical section of the forebrain commissures in man. In Structure and Function of Cerebral Commissures, ed. I. S. Russell, M. W. van Hoff & G. Berlucchi, pp. 411–20. London: Macmillan Press
Rees, G. (2001). Neuroimaging of visual awareness in patients and normal subjects. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 11, 150–6Google Scholar
Rogers, T. B., Kuiper, N. A. & Kirker, W. S. (1977). Self-reference and the encoding of personal information. Journal of Personal and Social Psychology, 35, 677–88Google Scholar
Sanz, M., Constable, G., Lopez-Ibor, I., Kemp, R. & David, A. (1997). A comparative study of insight scales and their relationship to psychopathological and clinical variables. Psychological Medicine, 28, 437–46Google Scholar
Sarfati, Y. & Hardy Bayle, M. C. (1999). How do people with schizophrenia explain the behaviour of others? A study of theory of mind and its relationship to thought and speech disorganization in schizophrenia. Psychological Medicine, 29, 613–20Google Scholar
Scharfetter, C. (1980). General Psychopathology. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press
Scharfetter, C. (1981). Ego-psychopathology: the concept and its empirical evaluation. Psychological Medicine, 11, 273–80Google Scholar
Schneider, K. (1967). Klinische Psychopathologie, 8th edn. Stuttgart: Thieme Verlag
Shallice, T., Fletcher, P., Frith, C. D. et al. (1994). Brain regions associated with acquisition and retrieval of verbal episodic memory. Nature, 368, 633–5Google Scholar
Sims, A. (1988). Symptoms in the Mind. An Introduction to Descriptive Psychopathology. London: WB Saunders
Sirigu, A., Daprati, E., Pradat Diehl, P., Franck, N. & Jeannerod, M. (1999). Perception of self-generated movement following left parietal lesion. Brain, 122, 74Google Scholar
Slachevsky, A., Pillon, B., Fourneret, P. et al. (2001). Preserved adjustment but impaired awareness in a sensory-motor conflict following prefrontal lesions. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 13, 332–40Google Scholar
Smith, T. E., Hull, J. W., Israel, L. M. & Willson, D. F. (2000). Insight, symptoms, and neurocognition in schizophrenia and schizoaffective disorder. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 26, 193–200Google Scholar
Snygg, D. & Combs, A. W. (1949). Individual Behaviour. New York: Harper & Row
Spence, S. A., Brooks, D. J., Hirsch, S. R. et al. (1997). A PET study of voluntary movement in schizophrenic patients experiencing passivity phenomena (delusions of alien control). Brain, 120, 1997–2011Google Scholar
Sperry, R. W. (1950). Neural basis of the spontaneous optokinetic response produced by visual inversion. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 43, 482–9Google Scholar
Sperry, R. W., Zaidel, E. & Zaidel, D. (1979). Self recognition and social awareness in the deconnected minor hemisphere. Neuropsychologia, 17, 153–66Google Scholar
Srinivasan, R., Russell, D. P., Edelman, G. M. & Tononi, G. (1999). Increased synchronization of neuromagnetic responses during conscious perception. Journal of Neuroscience, 19, 5435–48Google Scholar
Stanton, B. & David, A. S. (2000). First person accounts of delusions. Psychiatric Bulletin, 24, 333–6Google Scholar
Startup, M. (1997). Awareness of own and others' schizophrenic illness. Schizophrenia Research, 26, 203–11Google Scholar
Strauss, J. S. (1969). Hallucinations and delusions as points on continua function. Archives of General Psychiatry, 21, 581–6Google Scholar
Swanson, C. L., Freudenreich, N. J., McEvoy, J. P. et al. (1995). Insight in schizophrenia and mania. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 183, 752–5Google Scholar
Symons, C. S. & Johnson, B. T. (1997). The self-reference effect in memory: a meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 121, 371–94Google Scholar
Tulving, E. (1991). Concepts of human memory. In Memory: Organisation and Locus of Change, ed. G. Squire, G. Lynch, N. M. Weinberger & J. L. McGaugh. Oxford University Press, New York
Tulving, E. (1993a). Self-knowledge of an amnesic individual is represented abstractly. In The Mental Representation of Trait and Autobiographical Knowledge about the Self. Advances in social cognition, ed. T. Srull & R. S. Wyer, p. ⅷ. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum
Tulving, E. (1993b). Self-knowledge of an amnesic is represented abstractly. In The Mental Representation of Trait and Autobiographical Knowledge about the Self. Advances in Social Cognition, ed. T. K. Srull & R. S. Wyer, pp. 147–56. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum
Valentine, T. & Bruce, V. (1986). Recognizing familiar faces: the role of distinctiveness and familiarity. Canadian Journal of Psychology, 40, 300–5Google Scholar
Vandenberghe, R., Price, C., Wise, R., Josephs, O. & Frackowiak, R. S. (1996). Functional anatomy of a common semantic system for words and pictures. Nature, 383, 254–6Google Scholar
Holst, E. & Mittelstaedt, H. (1950). Das Reafferenzprinzip (Wechselwirkungen zwischen Zentralnervensystem and Peripherie). Naturwissenschaften, 37, 464–76Google Scholar
Walsh, K. & Darby, D. (1999). Neuropsychology. A Clinical Approach, 4th edn. Edinburgh: Churchill Livingstone
Wiggs, C. L., Weisberg, J. & Martin, A. (1999). Neural correlates of semantic and episodic memory retrieval. Neuropsychologia, 37, 103–18Google Scholar
Wolpert, D. M., Ghahramani, Z. & Jordan, M. I. (1995). An internal model for sensorimotor integration. Science, 269, 1880–2Google Scholar
Wylie, R. C. (1989). Measures of Self-concept. Lincoln, NE: University of Nebraska Press
Yoon, H. U. (2001). Ich selbst oder ein anderer? Vertraut oder fremd? Untersuchungen zur Gesichtererkennung. Doctoral dissertation. University of Tübingen
Young, A. W., Hay, D. C., McWeeny, K. H., Flude, B. M. & Ellis, A. W. (1985). Matching familiar and unfamiliar faces on internal and external features. Perception, 14, 737–46Google Scholar
Young, D. A., Davila, R. & Scher, H. (1993). Unawareness of illness and neuropsychological performance in chronic schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Research, 10, 117–24Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure coreplatform@cambridge.org is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×