Hostname: page-component-8448b6f56d-t5pn6 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-23T14:15:27.778Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Growth patterns and breast milk/infant formula energetic efficiency in healthy infants up to 18 months of life: the COGNIS study

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  19 February 2021

Natalia Sepúlveda-Valbuena
Affiliation:
Nutrition and Biochemistry Department, School of Sciences, Pontificia Universidad Javeriana, Bogotá, Colombia Department of Paediatrics, School of Medicine, University of Granada, Granada, Spain
Ana Nieto-Ruiz
Affiliation:
Department of Paediatrics, School of Medicine, University of Granada, Granada, Spain Instituto de Investigación Biosanitaria ibs, GRANADA, Health Sciences Technological Park, Granada, Spain EURISTIKOS Excellence Centre for Paediatric Research, University of Granada, Granada, Spain Mind, Brain and Behavior Research Center–CIMCYC, University of Granada, Granada, Spain
Estefanía Diéguez
Affiliation:
Department of Paediatrics, School of Medicine, University of Granada, Granada, Spain Instituto de Investigación Biosanitaria ibs, GRANADA, Health Sciences Technological Park, Granada, Spain EURISTIKOS Excellence Centre for Paediatric Research, University of Granada, Granada, Spain
Florian Herrmann
Affiliation:
Department of Paediatrics, School of Medicine, University of Granada, Granada, Spain EURISTIKOS Excellence Centre for Paediatric Research, University of Granada, Granada, Spain
Mireia Escudero-Marín
Affiliation:
Department of Paediatrics, School of Medicine, University of Granada, Granada, Spain Instituto de Investigación Biosanitaria ibs, GRANADA, Health Sciences Technological Park, Granada, Spain EURISTIKOS Excellence Centre for Paediatric Research, University of Granada, Granada, Spain
Roser De-Castellar
Affiliation:
Laboratorios Ordesa, S.L, Barcelona, Spain
María Rodríguez-Palmero
Affiliation:
Laboratorios Ordesa, S.L, Barcelona, Spain
María Teresa Miranda
Affiliation:
Department of Biostatistics, School of Medicine, University of Granada, Granada, Spain
José Antonio García-Santos
Affiliation:
Department of Paediatrics, School of Medicine, University of Granada, Granada, Spain Instituto de Investigación Biosanitaria ibs, GRANADA, Health Sciences Technological Park, Granada, Spain EURISTIKOS Excellence Centre for Paediatric Research, University of Granada, Granada, Spain
Mercedes G. Bermúdez
Affiliation:
Department of Paediatrics, School of Medicine, University of Granada, Granada, Spain Instituto de Investigación Biosanitaria ibs, GRANADA, Health Sciences Technological Park, Granada, Spain EURISTIKOS Excellence Centre for Paediatric Research, University of Granada, Granada, Spain
Cristina Campoy*
Affiliation:
Department of Paediatrics, School of Medicine, University of Granada, Granada, Spain Instituto de Investigación Biosanitaria ibs, GRANADA, Health Sciences Technological Park, Granada, Spain EURISTIKOS Excellence Centre for Paediatric Research, University of Granada, Granada, Spain Spanish Network of Biomedical Research in Epidemiology and Public Health (CIBERESP), Granada’s Node, Institute of Health Carlos III, Madrid, Spain
*
*Corresponding author: Cristina Campoy, email ccampoy@ugr.es
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Type of feeding during early life influences growth trajectory and metabolic risk at later ages. Modifications in infant formula composition have led to evaluate their effects on growth and energetic efficiency (EE) compared with breast-feeding. Main goal was to analyse type of feeding potential effects during first months of life, plus its EE, on growth patterns in healthy formula fed (standard infant formula (SF) vs. experimental infant formula enriched with bioactive nutrients (EF)) and breastfed (BF) infants participating in the COGNIS RCT (http://www.ClinicalTrials.gov, Identifier: NCT02094547) up to 18 months of age. Infants follow-up to 18 months of age (n 141) fed with a SF (n 48), EF(n 56), or BF (n 37), were assessed for growth parameters using WHO standards. Growth velocity (GV) and catch-up were calculated to identify growth patterns. EE of breast milk/infant formula was also estimated. Infants’ growth at 6 months showed higher length and lower head circumference gains in SF and EF infants than BF infants. Both weight-for-length and weight-for-age catch-up growth showed significant differences in formula fed groups compared with the BF. No significant differences in GV or catch-up were found at 6–12 and 12–18 months. Regarding EE, infant formula groups showed significantly lower weight and length gains/g of milk protein, and higher weight and length gains/g of milk lipids, than the BF infants. GV during first 6 months, which may be influenced by feeding, seems to be the main predictor of subsequent growth trajectory. Breast-feeding may have positive effects on growth programming due to its nutrients’ EE.

Type
Full Papers
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BYCreative Common License - NCCreative Common License - ND
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-NoDerivatives licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-nd/4.0/), which permits non-commercial re-use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is unaltered and is properly cited. The written permission of Cambridge University Press must be obtained for commercial re-use or in order to create a derivative work.
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2021. Published by Cambridge University Press on behalf of The Nutrition Society

Type of feeding after birth has been associated with growth velocity (GV) during the first years of life and may determine growth during childhood and adolescence. Both weight gain during the first 2 years of life and differences in weight-for-age z-score (WAZ) indicator could define child growth pattern(Reference Ong, Ahmed and Emmett1,Reference Stettler, Zemel and Kumanyika2) . Different authors have described a rapid growth pattern in infants associated with higher risk of obesity(Reference Ong, Ahmed and Emmett1,Reference Ong and Loos3Reference Azad, Vehling and Chan6) , type 2 diabetes and CVD later on(Reference Perng, Hajj and Belfort7Reference Sutharsan, O’Callaghan and Williams10).

The WHO recommends exclusive breast-feeding (BF) during the first 6 months of life, continuing at least until 24 months of age along with complementary feeding(11,Reference Pecoraro, Agostoni and Pepaj12) . Infant formulas intent to promote an adequate growth, neurodevelopment and health programming in infants, when breast-feeding is not possible(Reference Dipasquale, Serra and Corsello13). However, feeding with infant formulas has been shown to increase weight gain and BMI during the first year of life(Reference Kurniawan, Anggraini and Julia14,Reference Heinig, Nommsen and Peerson15) , compared with breast milk.

During last years, infant formulas have been modified and enriched with bioactive ingredients similar to those present in human milk to provide the necessary amount of energy and nutrients to reach an optimal growth(16). Both the protein and lipid fractions of bovine milk fat globule membrane(Reference Demmelmair and Koletzko17Reference Zavaleta, Kvistgaard and Graverholt20), long-chain PUFAs (arachidonic acid, DHA, etc.)(Reference Jasani, Simmer and Patole21,Reference Hadley, Ryan and Forsyth22) , synbiotics (probiotics and prebiotics)(Reference Mugambi, Musekiwa and Lombard23,Reference Szajewska, Ruszczyński and Szymański24) , sialic acid and milk oligosaccharides(Reference Charbonneau, O’Donnell and Blanton25,Reference Buyken, Karaolis-Danckert and Remer26) have been added to infant formulas as an alternative to narrow the nutritional and functional gap compared with breast milk. Furthermore, protein content modification(Reference Koletzko, von Kries and Closa27) and infant formula supplementation with different nutrients have been studied to identify their effects on energetic efficiency (EE)(Reference Butte, Smith and Garza28,Reference Fleddermann, Demmelmair and Grote29) , growth, development and immune function compared with breast-feeding(Reference Dipasquale, Serra and Corsello13,Reference Tijhuis, Doets and Vonk Noordegraaf-Schouten30) .

To date, numerous studies have been focused on the effects of enriched infant formulas on growth rates, obesity risk associated with protein intake at early ages and benefits of the fatty acid profile or other nutrients in childhood neurodevelopment(Reference Pecoraro, Agostoni and Pepaj12,Reference Belkind-Gerson, Carreon-Rodriguez and Contreras-Ochoa31Reference Braun, Erler and Kiefte-de Jong33) . Nevertheless, there is insufficient scientific evidence about the effect of bioactive compounds-enriched infant formulas on infant growth trajectory(Reference Tijhuis, Doets and Vonk Noordegraaf-Schouten30). Having in mind these considerations, the present study was aimed to analyse the potential effects of the type of feeding during the first months of life, as well as its EE, on growth patterns in healthy formula fed (standard v. enriched infant formula) and breastfed (BF) infants up to 18 months of age.

Methods

The present study was carried out following the updated Declaration of Helsinki Principles(34,35) , the Good Clinical Practice recommendations of the ECC (document 111/3976/88 July 1990) and Spanish Royal Decree 561/1993 on clinical trials. All procedures involved in the COGNIS study were approved by the Research Bioethical Committee from the University of Granada, as well as the Bioethical Committees for Clinical Research of San Cecilio and Mother-Infant Hospitals of Granada. All families were informed about protocols and a signed written consent was obtained from each parent or legal guardian before involving each child in the study.

Study design and subjects

The COGNIS study (registered at www.ClinicalTrials.gov, Identifier: NCT02094547) is a prospective, double-blind randomised clinical trial (RCT) with a nutritional intervention using a new infant formula enriched with bioactive nutrients such as milk fat globule membrane components (10 % of total protein (wt:wt)), long-chain PUFAs (DHA and arachidonic acid), synbiotics (mix of fructooligosaccharide and inulin (ratio 1:1), Bifidobacterium infantis IM1 and Lactobacillus rhamnosus LCS-742), nucleotides, sialic acid and ganglioside-enriched whey proteins.

Inclusion criteria

Eligible infants were healthy term infants (37–41 weeks gestational age), with adequate birth weight for gestational age (between 3–97 percentile), normal APGAR score (7–10) and umbilical pH ≥ 7·10. They must have availability to continue throughout the study period, and parents or legal guardians have signed the informed consent.

Exclusion criteria

The exclusion criteria were defined as follows: infants participating in another study; infants who suffered neurological disorders (hydrocephalus, perinatal hypoxia, intraventricular haemorrhage, neonatal meningitis, septic shock, West syndrome, etc.) or gastrointestinal disturbances (mainly cows’ milk protein allergy/intolerance or lactose intolerance); maternal pathological background (neurological diseases, mental illness, metabolopathies (type 1 diabetes mellitus), chronic diseases (hypothyroidism), maternal malnutrition or prenatal infections (TORCH complex, etc.)), mothers who received during pregnancy or lactation drug treatments potentially harmful for neurodevelopment (anxiolytics, antidepressants, etc.); parents impossibility to continue through the study. A detailed description of the COGNIS study can be found in online Supplementary Methods. Briefly, a total of 220 full-term infants were enrolled, 170 of them were randomised (ratio 1:1) to receive, during their first 18 months of life, either a standard infant formula (SF; n 85) or an experimental infant formula (EF; n 85) enriched with bioactive nutrients. A total of fifty exclusively breastfed (BF) infants for at least 2 months were also included as reference group. The present study involved 171 infants at 6 months (SF = 60; EF = 69; BF = 42); 152 infants at 12 months (SF = 51; EF = 63; BF = 38) and 141 infants at 18 months (SF = 48; EF = 56; BF = 37). A detailed participant flow chart from the baseline visit to 18 months of life is shown in Fig. 1.

Fig. 1. Dropout rates and exclusions from the baseline visit to 18 months of age. Infants who did not show up at the follow-up visits, but remained in COGNIS study for later visits are described as ‘did not attend’. SF, standard infant formula; EF, experimental infant formula; BF, breastfed infants; EE, energetic efficiency. D = drop outs and E = exclusions, as previously reported(Reference Nieto-Ruiz, García-Santos and Bermúdez41). *SF and EF infants were randomised between 0 and 2 months of age; BF infants were included up to 6 months of age.

Demographical and clinical characteristics

Baseline information about maternal and paternal age, anthropometric characteristics (BMI and height), as well as their educational level, place of residence and intelligence quotient (IQ) was obtained at study entry. Prenatal characteristics (weight gain during pregnancy, siblings, type of delivery and smoking during pregnancy) were also recorded. Data regarding weight, length and head circumference (HC) at birth were taken from clinical records.

Evaluation of infant growth patterns

To analyse the potential effects of the type of feeding on infants growth, anthropometric measurements were collected at 1–2, 3, 4, 6, 12 and 18 months of life by a trained paediatrician, following standard procedures(36); measurements were taken in triplicate and their means were calculated. Analyses of growth patterns were performed by a trained nutritionist. A detailed description of growth measurements analyses can be found in online Supplementary Methods.

Anthropometric data included weight (SOEHNLE Multina Comfort 8352·01·001, max 20 kg), length (Harpenden Infantometer Holtain Model 702, max 91·5 cm) and HC (SECA 212, max 59 cm). All anthropometric parameters were assessed according to the current WHO Child Growth Standards by sex and age(37,38) . WAZ, weight-for-length z-score (WLZ), length-for-age z-score (LAZ), BMI-for-age z-score and HC-for-age z-score (HCZ) were calculated using WHO Anthro software package version 3.2.2 (WHO)(37).

GV was calculated according to weight, length and HC gains/d. Weight gain (g/d), length gain (cm/d) and HC (cm/d) were calculated at three different time intervals: (i) from the baseline visit (formula fed infants groups) or from birth (BF infants) to 6 months of life, (ii) from 6 to 12 months of life and (iii) from 12 to 18 months of life. These data were compared using velocity standards for weight, length and HC of the WHO growth standards presented as 6-month intervals and stratified by sex, to obtain standard deviations (sd). GV was classified as: slow (< –1 sd), normal (≥ –1 sd and ≤ + 1 sd) or rapid (> + 1 sd), based on the cut-off points indicated by WHO for the analysis of growth indicators and the identification of growth problems (≥ + 1 sd and ≤ –1 sd)(36). After this, to identify infant growth pattern developed up to 18 months of age, trajectories of weight, length, HC and growth indicators WAZ, LAZ, WLZ and HCZ were analysed up to 18 months, according to weight GV up to 6 months of age.

Catch-up growth is defined as the variation in infant weight gain rates, which shows a significant growth to compensate from intra-uterine restriction or enhancement of growth, during the first 2 years of life(Reference Ong, Ahmed and Emmett1,Reference Ong and Loos3) . Catch-up growth was calculated as WAZ and WLZ. Differences in z-scores were considered as the three time intervals mentioned above. Afterwards, catch-up growth was classified by the differences in z-scores as follows: slow (< –0·67), normal (≥ –0·67 and ≤ + 0·67) and rapid (> + 0·67)(Reference Ong, Ahmed and Emmett1,Reference Ong and Loos3) .

Estimation of breast milk/infant formula energetic efficiency

This parameter is described as the increase in weight and length according to the energy and macronutrient intake (proteins, carbohydrates and lipids) per day from infant formula or breast milk(Reference Butte, Smith and Garza28,Reference Fleddermann, Demmelmair and Grote29) . The average of energy and macronutrients intake was obtained using a 3-d dietary record at 6, 12 and 18 months. For the infant formula groups, the daily volume of milk (ml) was reported as the amount of water (ml) and powdered formula (g) for infant formula reconstitution. Volume of breast milk intake (ml) was estimated according to infant gastric capacity and theoretical estimates for breast milk intake(Reference Dewey and Brown39,Reference Kent40) . Detailed description in this regard is included in online Supplementary Methods. EE was expressed as (a) weight gain (g/d)/4184 kJ (100 kcal) of milk, (b) weight gain (g/d)/g of milk protein, (c) weight gain (g/d)/g of milk carbohydrate, (d) weight gain (g/d)/g of milk lipids, (e) length gain (mm/d)/418·4 kJ (100 kcal) of milk, (f) length gain (mm/d)/g of milk protein, (g) length gain (mm/d)/g of milk carbohydrate and (h) length gain (mm/d)/g of milk lipids(Reference Butte, Smith and Garza28,Reference Fleddermann, Demmelmair and Grote29) .

Statistical analyses

All statistical analyses were conducted using IBM® SPSS Statistics® programme, version 22.0 (SPSS Inc.). Shapiro–Wilk and Kolmogorov–Smirnov tests, as well as visual inspection of histograms, were applied for all variables in the three study groups to check them for normality. Variables with normal distribution were presented as means and standard deviations, while non-normally distributed variables as median and interquartile range. Categorical variables were expressed as frequencies and percentages. Differences among BF, SF and EF groups were tested using ANOVA or Welch and Student t test for two groups, Kruskal–Wallis rank sum test for non-normally distributed variables, and χ 2 or Fisher’s exact tests for categorical variables were used depending on the response variable. Interaction test between weight GV up to 6 months and the study groups was performed to identify effects on infant’s growth. Relevant confounders (maternal age, height and IQ) were included in the ANCOVA and in the adjusted multivariate ANCOVA. Bonferroni corrected post hoc comparisons were used to identify significant group differences in pairs. P < 0·05 values were considered statistically significant.

Sample size was calculated before the beginning of the COGNIS study, as previously described(Reference Nieto-Ruiz, García-Santos and Bermúdez41). Statistical power was calculated for the present study applying the following equation(Reference Martín Andrés and Luna del Castillo42):

$$n = 2{{{{\left( {{\rm{Z\alpha }} + {\rm{Z}}2{\rm{\beta }}} \right)}^2}} \over {\delta '}}.$$

Results

Background characteristics and baseline data of parents and infants participating in the COGNIS study are shown in Table 1. There were statistically significant differences between study groups with respect to maternal age, height and IQ. Mothers of BF infants were older and taller than mothers of EF infants (P = 0·028 and 0·005, respectively). These variables (maternal age and height) were included as confounders in further growth patterns statistical analyses. Furthermore, mothers of BF group showed higher IQ compared with the mothers of the formula fed groups (SF and EF) (P < 0·001); thus, IQ was also included as a confounder since it was identified as a variable that may be directly or indirectly related to growth and development and the type of feeding in infants(Reference Pecoraro, Agostoni and Pepaj12,Reference Der, Batty and Deary43) .

Table 1. Parents and infants baseline characteristics by study group

SF, standard infant formula; EF, experimental infant formula; BF, breastfed infants; IQ, intelligence quotient; WAZ, weight-for-age z-score; WLZ, weight-for-length z-score; LAZ, length-for-age z-score; BAZ, BMI-for-age z-score; HCZ, head circumference-for-age z-score.

* Mean values and standard deviations; † medians and interquartile ranges; ‡ numbers and percentages.

** P-values for overall differences between COGNIS-groups. ANOVA for normally distributed variables, Kruskal–Wallis rank-sum test for non-normal continuous variables, and χ 2 test for categorical variables. Values which do not share the same suffix (ab) were significantly different in Bonferroni post hoc test. Bold: P-value < 0·05.

Regarding newborn’s characteristics, no differences were found between study groups in anthropometrics parameters at birth, including weight, length, HC and their respective z-scores by age (WAZ, WLZ, LAZ, BMI-for-age z-score and HCZ). Breast-feeding days were significantly different between the three study groups (P < 0·001) due to the design of the COGNIS study.

Effects of the type of feeding on anthropometric characteristics and daily growth gain in infants during their first 18 months of life

Results concerning infants’ anthropometric measures are shown in Table 2. HC at 4 and 6 months was bigger in the formula fed groups compared with the BF infants in the confounders (maternal age, height and IQ) adjusted model (P adj = 0·013; P adj = 0·004, respectively). HCZ was also significantly higher at 4 and 6 months in the formula fed infants in comparison with those in the BF group (P adj = 0·018; P adj = 0·008, respectively). Moreover, length at 12 months of age was higher in the SF group than in the BF group (P adj = 0·041) (Table 2). No significant differences were found in WAZ, WLZ, LAZ and BMI-for-age z-score along the COGNIS groups.

Table 2. Effect of the type of feeding on anthropometric data in infants during the first 18 months of life.

SF, standard infant formula; EF, experimental infant formula; BF, breastfed infants; HC, head circumference; WAZ, weight-for-age z-score; WLZ, weight-for-length z-score; LAZ, length-for-age z-score; BAZ, BMI-for-age z-score; HCZ, head circumference-for-age z-score; IQ, intelligence quotient.

* P-value was obtained from ANOVA; P adj was obtained from ANOVA; P adj was obtained from ANCOVA for the group differences using univariate general linear model, including the effects from the following potential confounders: maternal age, height and IQ. Bold: P-value < 0·05.

ab Values not sharing the same suffix were significantly different in Bonferroni post hoc test.

In order to estimate GV in COGNIS infants, we next evaluate weight, length and HC gains in three time intervals: (i) up to 6 months, (ii) between 6 and 12 months and (iii) between 12 and 18 months (Table 3). Infants from the formula fed groups (SF or EF) had significantly higher length gain (cm/d) (P adj = 0·015) and lower HC gain (cm/d) (P adj = 0·001) compared with the BF infants at 6 months of age. Between 6 and 12 months of age, the SF group presented a significantly lower HC gain compared with BF infants (P adj = 0·019) (Table 3).

Table 3. Daily growth gain in infants of the COGNIS study.

SF, standard infant formula; EF, experimental infant formula; BF, breastfed infants; HC, head circumference; IQ, intelligence quotient.

* P-value was obtained from ANOVA; P adj was obtained by ANCOVA for the group differences using univariate general linear model, including the effects of the following potential confounders: maternal age, height and IQ. Bold: P-value < 0·05.

Calculated from the baseline visit (formula fed infants group) or from birth to 6 months of life (breastfed infants). Data were analysed using the WHO growth velocity standards, and presented as 6-month increments from birth to 18 months and stratified by sex(38).

a, b Values not sharing the same suffix were significantly different in Bonferroni post hoc test.

Growth velocity and catch-up growth

Infants’ GV and catch-up indicators were classified as slow, normal or rapid according to the aforementioned criteria in the ‘Methods’ section. At 6 months, the SF group showed a lower proportion of slow length GV when compared with the BF infants (P = 0·028). Formula fed infants showed higher proportion of slow, and lower proportion of rapid HC GV compared with the BF infants (P < 0·001) (Table 4). When analysing the WAZ catch-up until 6 months of life, the formula fed groups (SF or EF) had significantly lower percentage of slow and higher percentage of rapid catch-up growth compared with the BF group (P < 0·001). Regarding WLZ catch-up until 6 months of life, EF and BF groups are characterised by higher proportion of normal catch-up growth compared with the SF group, although not reaching significant difference. On the other hand, the SF group showed a lower percentage of infants classified as slow catch-up growth (P = 0·046). No significant differences were detected in the GV and catch-up growth parameters at 6–12 and 12–18 months of age (Table 4).

Table 4 Growth velocity and catch-up growth in infants during the first 18 months of life by COGNIS study groups.

SF, standard infant formula; EF, experimental infant formula; BF, breastfed infants; HC, head circumference; WAZ, weight-for-age z-score; WLZ, weight-for-length z-score.

* χ 2 test for categorical variables. P-values are comparisons between the COGNIS-groups. Values which do not share the same suffix (a,b) are significantly different in Bonferroni post hoc test. Bold: P-value < 0·05. Growth velocity was classified as: slow: < −1 sd, normal: ≥ −1 sd and ≤ + 1 sd or rapid: > + 1 sd; catch-up growth classified as follows: slow: < −0·67, normal: ≥ −0·67 and ≤ + 0·67 and rapid: > + 0·67 (see ‘Methods’ section for more details).

Calculated from the baseline visit (formula fed infants group) or from birth to 6 months of life (breastfed infants).

Growth patterns in COGNIS infants up to 18 months

Regarding interaction between GV up to 6 months and study groups (SF, EF and BF), no statistically significant results were detected in those test (P-values >0·05) (online Supplementary Table S2). This means that there is no effect of the study group in those growth parameters and indicators evaluated. Same happens to interaction between weight GV and study group. Only one factor has effect on growth: GV up to 6 months of age (P < 0·05), which indicates that this factor may predict subsequent growth trajectory while diet group does not. However, due to the study design, results are presented according to study group.

Growth patterns were evaluated up to 18 months of age according to the classification of weight GV up to 6 months of age visits (Fig. 2(a)–(g) and online Supplementary Table S3).

Fig. 2. Growth patterns up to 18 months of life, according to weight growth velocity up to 6 months of life by feeding group. Data are marginal mean values and standard deviations. P adj was obtained by ANCOVA for the group differences using univariate general linear model, including the effects of the following potential confounders: maternal age, height and IQ. *P adj-values < 0·05 and **P adj-values < 0·01. SGV, slow growth velocity; NGV, normal growth velocity; RGV, rapid growth velocity; SF, standard infant formula; EF, experimental infant formula; BF, breastfed infants; HC, head circumference; WAZ, weight-for-age z-score; LAZ, length-for-age z-score; WLZ, weight-for-length z-score; HCZ, head circumference-for-age z-score; IQ: intelligence quotient. Dotted lines indicate normal growth according to WHO cut-off points(37). , SF; , BF; , EF.

Infants with slow growth velocity

At 6 months of age, LAZ was significantly lower in the SF group (–2·27 (sd 0·59)) compared with the BF group (–0·45 (sd 0·65)) (P adj = 0·036) (Fig. 2(e)), although the signification did not remain at later ages.

Infants with normal growth velocity

At 6 months, weight (kg) was significantly higher in the SF infants (7·56 (sd 0·60)) compared with the BF group (7·11 (sd 0·61)) (P adj = 0·025) (Fig. 2(a)); length (cm) was significantly higher in the SF group compared with the BF group (SF: 66·24 (sd 1·84); BF: 64·95 (sd 1·90); P adj = 0·041) (Fig. 2(b)), while HC (cm) was significantly higher in the EF group compared with the BF group (EF: 43·76 (sd 1·17); BF: 42·63 (sd 1·17); P adj = 0·008) (Fig. 2(c)). Similarly, at 18 months of age, the EF group presented higher HC (cm) compared with the SF group (EF: 48·08 (sd 1·34); SF: 47·14 (sd 1·30); P adj = 0·031) (Fig. 2(c)).

Regarding growth z-scores, at 6 months, HCZ was significantly higher in the EF group compared with the BF group (EF: 0·63 (sd 0·80); BF: 0·00 (sd 0·80); P adj = 0·034) (Fig. 2(g)). And at 18 months, HCZ was higher in the EF group compared with the SF group (EF: 0·74 (sd 0·85); SF: 0·17 (sd 0·82); P adj = 0·038) (Fig. 2(g)).

Infants with rapid growth velocity

Up to 18 months, no differences in weight, length or HC, nor WAZ, LAZ, WLZ or HCZ were found according to the feeding group (Fig. 2(a)–(g) and online Supplementary Table S3).

Energetic efficiency

EE was estimated up to 18 months of age according to energy and macronutrients intake per day from breast milk or infant formula (Fig. 3). For this purpose, we first evaluated if there were differences in infant formula/breast milk volume intake per day (d) at 6, 12 and 18 months. We found that the milk volume intake (ml/d) was significantly different between the study groups at 6 months of age (SF: 683·59 (sd 255·88); EF: 696·19 (sd 217·29); BF: 843·18 (sd 242·37), P = 0·003); thus, this parameter (milk volume intake) was included as an additional confounder for further EE analysis.

Fig. 3. Energetic efficiency analysis up to 18 months of age by feeding group. Data are marginal mean values and standard deviations. P-values were obtained by multivariate ANCOVA for group differences using multivariate general linear model, including the following confounders: maternal age, height and IQ. At 6 months of age, analysis was additionally adjusted by volume intake (ml/d) of infant formula or breast milk. For variables weight gain g/g protein; weight gain g/g lipids and length gain mm/g protein at 12 months of age, and length gain mm/g lipids at 6 and 12 months of age, P adj corresponds to log-transformed for these variables. *P adj-values < 0·05; ** P adj-values < 0·01 and *** P adj-values < 0·001. SF, standard infant formula; EF, experimental infant formula; BF, breastfed infants; IQ, intelligence quotient. , SF; , EF; , BF.

Energetic efficiency and weight gain (g/d)

Weight gain/g of milk protein at 6, 12 and 18 months of age was significantly lower in the formula fed groups (SF or EF) compared with the BF group (6 months, P adj = 0·003; 12 months, P adj < 0·001; 18 months, P adj < 0·001) (Fig. 3(a) and online Supplementary Table S4). Conversely, weight gain/g of milk lipids was significantly higher in the infant formula groups compared with the BF infants (6 months, P adj < 0·001; 12 months, P adj < 0·001; 18 months, P adj = 0·007) (Fig. 3(b) and online Supplementary Table S4). Furthermore, weight gain/418·4 kJ (100 kcal) of milk was significantly higher in the SF group compared with the BF infants at 12 months of age (P adj = 0·049). However, at 6 and 18 months of age there were no significant differences. Regarding weight gain/g of milk carbohydrates, there were no significant differences between feeding groups up to 18 months (online Supplementary Table S4).

Energetic efficiency and length gain (mm/d)

In a similar way, there were no significant differences up to 18 months of age in length gain/418·4 kJ (100 kcal) of milk and length gain/g of milk carbohydrates according to the feeding group (online Supplementary Table S4). However, at 6 months of age, length gain/g of milk protein was lower in the SF group compared with the BF infants (P adj = 0·023); moreover, at 12 and 18 months of age, the formula fed groups showed lower length gain/g of milk protein than the BF group (12 months, P adj < 0·001 and 18 months, P adj < 0·001) (Fig. 3(c) and online Supplementary Table S4). Finally, we found that, at 12 months of age, length gain/g of milk lipids was higher in the infant formula groups compared with the BF group (P adj < 0·001), while at 18 months of age it was higher in SF infants compared with the BF infants (P adj = 0·002) (Fig. 3(d) and online Supplementary Table S4).

Discussion

The present study was aimed to perform an exhaustive analysis of growth patterns up to 18 months of life in COGNIS infants fed with a new bioactive compounds-enriched infant formula, compared with a standard infant formula and BF infants. Additionally, EE of infant formulas and breast milk was estimated. Our results suggest that the type of feeding during the first 6 months of life may determine infant growth patterns; length gain was higher and HC gain was lower in infants from the formula fed groups (SF or EF) compared with the BF infants. These significant differences were not found at 6–12 and 12–18 months. Moreover, the analysis of weight/length catch-up until 6 months of life suggested that EF and BF groups are characterised by higher proportion of normal catch-up growth compared with the SF group. Studies refer that HC may be associated with specific aspects of child development in healthy population(Reference Dupont, Castellanos-Ryan and Séguin44,Reference Whitehouse, Zubrick and Blair45) . Also, higher HC in infants at 4 and 6 months of age might be related to a better visual acuity at 12 months of life(Reference Nieto-Ruiz, García-Santos and Bermúdez41) and language development at 4 years old in those children fed with the enriched bioactive nutrients infant formula(Reference Nieto-Ruiz, Diéguez and Sepúlveda-Valbuena46).

In general, growth is a result of a process depending on the availability of essential nutrients. A harmonious growth results from weight and length gains in appropriate proportion; an imbalance in weight gain might affect linear growth and could negatively impact on cognitive development at later ages(Reference Uauy47). Some studies suggest to analyse weight gain rates and linear growth as independent factors that could affect health and development in adulthood(Reference Horta, Victora and de Mola48,Reference Adair, Fall and Osmond49) . Therefore, infant weight gain is generally known as the primary indicator of healthy growth; higher weight gains between 3 and 12 months of age(Reference Baird, Poole and Robinson50Reference Singhal and Lucas52) have been related to higher risk of obesity and other metabolic disorders(Reference Owen53,Reference Baird, Fisher and Lucas54) .

Growth patterns in infants during the first months of life are a topic of interest in paediatric research; growth characteristics have been described according to the type of feeding in early stages of life, given biological dynamics of human milk compared with infant formulas(Reference Azad, Vehling and Chan6,Reference Young, Levek and Reynolds55,Reference Wallingford and Barber56) . Results from the present study showed that the SF fed group was characterised by a lower proportion of infants with slow GV in length compared with the BF infants. In addition, when analysing the weight/age catch-up until 6 months of life, the formula fed groups (SF or EF) had significantly lower percentage of slow growth infants compared with the BF group; conversely, the BF group had lower proportion of rapid WAZ catch-up. Moreover, the EF and BF groups are characterised by higher proportion of normal WLZ catch-up growth compared with the SF one. Research refers that infant formula intake is a risk factor for self-regulation satiety alteration(Reference Isselmann DiSantis, Collins and Fisher57), higher length gain(Reference Fleddermann, Demmelmair and Grote29), higher weight gain, rapid weight gain velocity, higher WAZ(Reference Patel, Dalrymple and Pasupathy58,Reference Butte, Wong and Hopkinson59) and weight excess at later ages(Reference Mannan60,Reference Giugliani61) compared with the BF infants. Since several years ago, infant formulas have been modified and enriched with bioactive nutrients trying to mimic those present in human milk to satisfy an optimal development and growth, providing the necessary amount of energy and nutrients(Reference Dipasquale, Serra and Corsello13,16) . Some studies have reported that infant formula intake implies an elevated protein intake, leading to increased secretion of insulin, insulin-like growth factor type 1 and adipogenic activity(Reference Koletzko, Broekaert and Demmelmair62,Reference Koletzko, Chourdakis and Grote63) ; these mechanisms could be involved in growth patterns observed in formula fed infants; in this regard, infants fed with low protein content formulas show less risk for obesity during childhood(Reference Koletzko, von Kries and Closa27). Infant formulas tested in the present study follow the guidelines of the ESPGHAN Committee on Nutrition(Reference Koletzko, Baker and Cleghorn64) and international and national recommendations for infant formula composition, and their protein content is within the minimum recommended range. Results from a study with a similar design to the present one indicated a rapid GV in formula fed infants compared with the breastfed ones, with no differences between standard formula or milk fat globule membrane supplemented formula(Reference Timby, Domellof and Hernell18).

The present study also evaluated whether differences in growth patterns between types of feeding with infant formula (SF or EF) or breast-feeding in the COGNIS study could be related to the EE of macronutrients present in infant formula and breast milk as a possible, and partial, explanation of the results obtained. To date, and despite that EE of infant formulas has been described, studies are largely based on short-term infant growth follow-up (until 6 months of life), and there is insufficient evidence to accurately compare EE in infant formula and breast milk(Reference Fleddermann, Demmelmair and Grote29,Reference Fleddermann, Demmelmair and Koletzko65) . The present study reports information about EE up to 18 months of age in formula fed and BF infants. Our data showed that BF infants had higher weight and length gain per g of milk protein compared with the formula fed infants up to 18 months of life, while milk lipids appear to be responsible for increased weight and length gain in those infants who received infant formula. These findings may be related to the nutritional composition of breast milk; in fact, breast milk provides high biological value proteins, optimal whey/casein ratio and adequate supply of amino acids(66Reference Lönnerdal69). Despite its higher content in lipids, breast milk also contains enzymes needed for digestibility and utilisation of fatty acids(Reference Boquien70). Both nutritional characteristics, together with its content in bioactive nutrients, hormonal and immune factors, could determine a better EE of breast milk compared with infant formulas in order to ensure an optimal infant growth and development. It is important to note that data from the present study cannot be attributed to a single specific component of infant formulas (enriched or not), but as a whole product designed to meet infant nutritional needs; moreover, there is likely a synergy among the different bioactive components, which could be associated with bioavailability and biological utilisation of nutrients. Currently, there is insufficient evidence to support the effect of DHA and arachidonic acid enriched formula on growth and adiposity(Reference Tijhuis, Doets and Vonk Noordegraaf-Schouten30,Reference Hoffman, Harris and Wampler71,Reference Ingol, Li and Boone72) , or synbiotic supplementation on infants growth(Reference Mugambi, Musekiwa and Lombard23,Reference Tijhuis, Doets and Vonk Noordegraaf-Schouten30,Reference Braegger, Chmielewska and Decsi73,Reference van den Nieuwboer, Claassen and Morelli74) . Effects on immune system and on microbiota structure and function have been described; these may be factors influencing children growth as well(Reference Mugambi, Musekiwa and Lombard23,Reference Braegger, Chmielewska and Decsi73,Reference van den Nieuwboer, Claassen and Morelli74) . Thus, data obtained may be useful to develop further strategies not only focused on narrowing the nutritional gap but also the functional and metabolic gap, between infant formulas and human milk.

One of the strengths of the present study relies on long-term infant growth follow-up, as consequence of the longitudinal design of the COGNIS study, thus allowing a cross-sectional analyses of infant growth until 18 months of life, compared with other studies with shorter follow-up. Secondly, despite participants dropout at 18 months of life, statistical power was calculated to identify the validity of the results found in the present study. Statistical power reached to detect a minimum difference of 0·6 sd in growth patterns was 80 % at 18 months of age, enough to detect relevant differences in growth patterns between study groups. Additionally, it is also important to highlight that 26 % of infants in the control group continued breast-feeding until 18 months, which allowed to estimate the EE and compare growth patterns between infants who received SF or EF (initiation and follow-on formulas), and those who were breastfed at long-term. Finally, COGNIS design also included data about socio-demographic and anthropometric characteristics both from parents and newborns, which were used as potential confounders in statistical models.

Research about infant growth has been largely focused on the analysis of growth in a transversal way (z-scores). Despite infant weight assessment is considered a global indicator of healthy child growth, our results suggest that it could be worth to analyse in depth how the weight GV and trajectory modify the length and HC, as well as the growth indicators (WAZ, LAZ, WLZ and HCZ) during childhood. Current results have an added value due to the methodology used, based on different techniques with z-scores trajectories, GV using the WHO growth standards and classification according to growth rate (catch-up) between follow-up visits. Thus, this methodology could provide a basis to evaluate long-term potential impact of type of feeding at early age on infant GV and trajectory. Moreover, it can be useful to provide detailed and individualised guidelines for the introduction of complementary feeding, according to nutritional needs and infant growth patterns (especially for those who present rapid or slow growth trajectories), ensuring an adequate and normal growth for age.

However, there are limitations that must be acknowledged. First, due to ethical reasons, breast milk must be offered on demand with no limits in its volume or duration. Consequently, volumes of breast milk intake were obtained from 3-d dietary records as well as theoretical estimates based on previously published findings(Reference Dewey and Brown39,Reference Kent40) . For future studies, we suggest to weight the infant before and after being breastfed to know more accurately breast milk volume intake. Second, breast milk is a nutrient with a varying nutritional composition, both in a time-day basis and over time, to adapt to infants nutritional needs. Our study has not an individual analysis of the breast milk of each infant during the follow-up; therefore, composition in the present study was estimated based on a full mature breast milk composition as reported in the USDA National Nutrient Database for Standard Reference(66). It would be interesting in later studies to analyse the composition of breast milk individually and describe the composition of nutrients during the first 2 years of age. Third, our goal was to estimate EE of infant formulas (SF or EF) and breast milk, and we did not include complementary feeding in the analysis performed; thus, it would be interesting to analyse the complementary feeding in the three COGNIS groups in future studies. Finally, as previously reported(Reference Nieto-Ruiz, García-Santos and Bermúdez41), COGNIS study design implies the non-randomisation of the BF group, in contrast to both SF and EF groups, which could explain differences in maternal age, height and IQ baseline characteristics.

In summary, herewith we suggest that type of feeding received between birth and the first 6 months of life could be responsible for differences in infant GV and catch-up growth. GV during first 6 months, which may be influenced by feeding, seems to be the main predictor of subsequent growth patterns. Moreover, in infants fed with the enriched bioactive nutrients formula, higher HC at 6 months of age was found in the present study, which could be related to better visual acuity(Reference Nieto-Ruiz, García-Santos and Bermúdez41) and language development at 4 years old(Reference Nieto-Ruiz, Diéguez and Sepúlveda-Valbuena46), as previously reported. EE of breast milk nutrients seems to be responsible for its benefits on infant growth. Our results suggest that traditional methodology used for growth analysis may be insufficient to characterise the growth trajectory during childhood. In fact, traditional methodology is based on the parameters and indicators in a cross-sectional way, but not on an evolutionary one. However, analysis of weight, length and HC gains as a function of time and velocity, and how the GV changes, may better define the growth pattern at later stages. Further research based on long-term growth monitoring is necessary to establish the effects of early nutrition (infant formula fed or breast milk and complementary feeding) on body composition as an indicator of metabolic long-term health risks, and children’s growth later in life.

Acknowledgements

The authors want to acknowledge the parents and infants who participated in the COGNIS study; the authors also thank the paediatricians, technicians and researchers of the EURISTIKOS team and the Department of Paediatrics from the University of Granada, Spain. N. S.-V. acknowledges to Pontificia Universidad Javeriana and Fundación Carolina for supporting the development of her Ph.D. thesis. The results of the present study are likely to be included in the Doctoral Thesis of N. S.-V., in the context of the Clinical Medicine and Public Health Doctoral Programme at the University of Granada.

This project has been funded by Laboratorios Ordesa, S. L. Contract University of Granada General Foundation, No. 3349 and SMARTFOODS (CIEN) Contract University of Granada General Foundation, No. 4003, Spanish Ministry of Economy, Industry and Competitiveness, and partially funded by HORIZON 2020 EU DynaHEALTH Project (GA No.633595). N. S-V. has been granted with a scholarship from Fundación Carolina, Madrid, Spain.

C. C. was responsible for conceptualisation; N. S-V., A. N-R., E. D. and F. H. carried out the investigation, and performed data curation; N. S-V., A. N-R., E. D. and M. T. M. were responsible for the methodology developed, analysis and interpretation; N. S-V., A. N-R. and M. T. M. carried out the formal analysis; C. C., M. R-P. and R. D-C. were responsible for funding acquisition; C. C. was responsible for project administration; C. C. carried out supervision; N. S-V. wrote the first draft of the article; J. A. G-S. and M. G. B. discussed and supervised writing of the manuscript; A. N-R., E. D., F. H., M. E-M., R. D-C., M. R-P., M. T. M., J. A. G-S., M. G. B., C. C. reviewed and edited the present paper.

Dr. R. D-C. and Dr. M. R-P. are employees of Laboratorios Ordesa, S.L.

Supplementary material

For supplementary materials referred to in this article, please visit https://doi.org/10.1017/S000711452100057X

References

Ong, KK, Ahmed, ML, Emmett, PM, et al. (2000) Association between postnatal catch-up growth and obesity in childhood: prospective cohort study. BMJ 320, 967971.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stettler, N, Zemel, BS, Kumanyika, S, et al. (2002) Infant weight gain and childhood overweight status in a multicenter, cohort study. Pediatrics 109, 194199.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ong, K & Loos, R (2006) Rapid infancy weight gain and subsequent obesity: systematic reviews and hopeful suggestions. Acta Paediatr 95, 904908.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stettler, N, Kumanyika, SK, Katz, SH, et al. (2003) Rapid weight gain during infancy and obesity in young adulthood in a cohort of African Americans. Am J Clin Nutr 77, 13741378.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Monteiro, POA & Victora, CG (2005) Rapid growth in infancy and childhood and obesity in later life--a systematic review. Obes Rev Off J Int Assoc Study Obes 6, 143154.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Azad, MB, Vehling, L, Chan, D, et al. (2018) Infant feeding and weight gain: separating breast milk from breastfeeding and formula from food. Pediatrics 142, e20181092.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Perng, W, Hajj, H, Belfort, MB, et al. (2016) Birth size, early life weight gain, and midchildhood cardiometabolic health. J Pediatr 173, 122130.e1.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Taal, HR, vd Heijden, AJ, Steegers, EAP, et al. (2013) Small and large size for gestational age at birth, infant growth, and childhood overweight: size at birth, infant growth, and overweight. Obesity 21, 12611268.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Claris, O, Beltrand, J & Levy-Marchal, C (2010) Consequences of intrauterine growth and early neonatal catch-up growth. Semin Perinatol 34, 207210.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sutharsan, R, O’Callaghan, MJ, Williams, G, et al. (2015) Rapid growth in early childhood associated with young adult overweight and obesity–evidence from a community based cohort study. J Health Popul Nutr 33, 19.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
World Health Organization Guideline: Protecting, Promoting and Supporting Breastfeeding in Facilities Providing Maternity and Newborn Services. Geneva: WHO; 2017.Google Scholar
Pecoraro, L, Agostoni, C, Pepaj, O, et al. (2018) Behind human milk and breastfeeding: not only food. Int J Food Sci Nutr 69, 641646.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dipasquale, V, Serra, G, Corsello, G, et al. (2019) Standard and specialized infant formulas in Europe: making, marketing, and health outcomes. Nutr Clin Pract 35, 273281.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kurniawan, J, Anggraini, A & Julia, M (2018) Infant feeding practice on growth velocity in 4–6 month-olds. Paediatr Indones 58, 36.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Heinig, MJ, Nommsen, LA, Peerson, JM, et al. (1993) Energy and protein intakes of breast-fed and formula-fed infants during the first year of life and their association with growth velocity: the DARLING Study. Am J Clin Nutr 58, 152161.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Codex Alimentarius (1987) Standard for infant formula and formulas for special medical purposes intended for infants CODEX STAN 72–1981. [Internet]. 1987. Available from: http://www.fao.org/fao-who-codexalimentarius/sh-proxy/en/?lnk=1&url=https%253A%252F%252Fworkspace.fao.org%252Fsites%252Fcodex%252FStandards%252FCXS%2B72-1981%252FCXS_072e.pdf Google Scholar
Demmelmair, H & Koletzko, B (2018) Lipids in human milk. Best Pract Res Clin Endocrinol Metab 32, 5768.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Timby, N, Domellof, E, Hernell, O, et al. (2014) Neurodevelopment, nutrition, and growth until 12 mo of age in infants fed a low-energy, low-protein formula supplemented with bovine milk fat globule membranes: a randomized controlled trial. Am J Clin Nutr 99, 860868.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Timby, N, Hernell, O, Vaarala, O, et al. (2015) Infections in Infants Fed Formula Supplemented With Bovine Milk Fat Globule Membranes: J Pediatr Gastroenterol Nutr 60, 384389.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zavaleta, N, Kvistgaard, AS, Graverholt, G, et al. (2011) Efficacy of a complementary food enriched with a milk fat globule membrane protein fraction on diarrhea, anemia and micronutrient status in infants: J Pediatr Gastroenterol Nutr 1.Google Scholar
Jasani, B, Simmer, K, Patole, SK, et al. (2017) Long chain polyunsaturated fatty acid supplementation in infants born at term. Cochrane Database Syst Rev 3, CD000376.Google ScholarPubMed
Hadley, K, Ryan, A, Forsyth, S, et al. (2016) The Essentiality of arachidonic acid in infant development. Nutrients 8, 216.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mugambi, MN, Musekiwa, A, Lombard, M, et al. (2012) Synbiotics, probiotics or prebiotics in infant formula for full term infants: a systematic review. Nutr J 11, 81.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Szajewska, H, Ruszczyński, M, Szymański, H, et al. (2017) Effects of infant formula supplemented with prebiotics compared with synbiotics on growth up to the age of 12 month: a randomized controlled trial. Pediatr Res 81, 752758.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Charbonneau, MR, O’Donnell, D, Blanton, LV, et al. (2016) Sialylated milk oligosaccharides promote microbiota-dependent growth in models of infant undernutrition. Cell 164, 859871.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Buyken, AE, Karaolis-Danckert, N, Remer, T, et al. (2008) Effects of breastfeeding on trajectories of body fat and BMI throughout childhood. Obesity 16, 389395.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Koletzko, B, von Kries, R, Closa, R, et al. (2009) Lower protein in infant formula is associated with lower weight up to age 2 years: a randomized clinical trial. Am J Clin Nutr 89, 18361845.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Butte, NF, Smith, EO & Garza, C (1990) Energy utilization of breast-fed and formula-fed infants. Am J Clin Nutr 51, 350358.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fleddermann, M, Demmelmair, H, Grote, V, et al. (2014) Infant formula composition affects energetic efficiency for growth: the BeMIM study, a randomized controlled trial. Clin Nutr 33, 588595.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tijhuis, M, Doets, E & Vonk Noordegraaf-Schouten, M (2014) Extensive literature search and review as preparatory work for the evaluation of the essential composition of infant and follow-on formulae and growing-up milk. EFSA Support Publ 11, 551E.Google Scholar
Belkind-Gerson, J, Carreon-Rodriguez, A, Contreras-Ochoa, CO, et al. (2008) Fatty acids and neurodevelopment. J Pediatr Gastroenterol Nutr 47, S7S9.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Skórka, A, Pieścik-Lech, M, Kołodziej, M, et al. (2018) Infant formulae supplemented with prebiotics: are they better than unsupplemented formulae? An updated systematic review. Br J Nutr 119, 810825.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Braun, KV, Erler, NS, Kiefte-de Jong, JC, et al. (2016) Dietary intake of protein in early childhood is associated with growth trajectories between 1 and 9 years of age. J Nutr 146, 23612367.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
World Medical Association (2008) The World Medical Association Inc Declaration of Helsinki: Ethical Principles for Medical Research Involving Human Subjects. Ferney-Voltaire, Fr. [accessed 2018, Sep 18]. https://www.wma.net/wp-content/uploads/2018/07/DoH-Oct2008.pdf Google Scholar
World Medical Association (2013) World Medical Association Declaration of Helsinki: ethical principles for medical research involving human subjects. JAMA 310, 2191.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
World Health Organization (2008) Training Course on Child Growth Assessment. Geneva: WHO.Google Scholar
World Health Organization (WHO) (2006) The WHO Child Growth Standards. [accessed 2019, Feb 3]. http://www.who.int/childgrowth/standards/en/ Google Scholar
World Health Organization (editor) (2009) WHO Child Growth Standards: Growth Velocity Based on Weight, Length and Head Circumference: Methods and Development. Geneva: World Health Organization & Department of Nutrition for Health and Development.Google Scholar
Dewey, KG & Brown, KH (2003) Update on technical issues concerning complementary feeding of young children in developing countries and implications for intervention programs. Food Nutr Bull 24, 528.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kent, JC (2006) Volume and frequency of breastfeedings and fat content of breast milk throughout the day. Pediatrics 117, e387e395.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nieto-Ruiz, A, García-Santos, JA, Bermúdez, MG, et al. (2019) Cortical visual evoked potentials and growth in infants fed with bioactive compounds-enriched infant formula: results from COGNIS randomized clinical trial. Nutrients 11, 2456.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Martín Andrés, A & Luna del Castillo, J de D (2004) Bioestadística para las ciencias de la salud. Madrid: Norma-Capitel.Google Scholar
Der, G, Batty, GD & Deary, IJ (2006) Effect of breast feeding on intelligence in children: prospective study, sibling pairs analysis, and meta-analysis. BMJ 333, 945.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dupont, C, Castellanos-Ryan, N, Séguin, JR, et al. (2018) The predictive value of head circumference growth during the first year of life on early child traits. Sci Rep 8, 9828.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Whitehouse, AJO, Zubrick, SR, Blair, E, et al. (2012) Fetal head circumference growth in children with specific language impairment. Arch Dis Child 97, 4951.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nieto-Ruiz, A, Diéguez, E, Sepúlveda-Valbuena, N, et al. (2020) Influence of a functional nutrients-enriched infant formula on language development in healthy children at four years old. Nutrients 12, 535.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Uauy, R (2013) Improving linear growth without excess body fat gain in women and children. Food Nutr Bull 34, 257260.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Horta, BL, Victora, CG, de Mola, CL, et al. (2017) Associations of linear growth and relative weight gain in early life with human capital at 30 years of age. J Pediatr 182, 8591.e3.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Adair, LS, Fall, CH, Osmond, C, et al. (2013) Associations of linear growth and relative weight gain during early life with adult health and human capital in countries of low and middle income: findings from five birth cohort studies. Lancet 382, 525534.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baird, J, Poole, J, Robinson, S, et al. (2008) Milk feeding and dietary patterns predict weight and fat gains in infancy. Paediatr Perinat Epidemiol 22, 575586.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dewey, KG (1998) Growth characteristics of breast-fed compared to formula-fed infants. Neonatology 74, 94105.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Singhal, A & Lucas, A (2004) Early origins of cardiovascular disease: is there a unifying hypothesis? Lancet 363, 16421645.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Owen, CG (2005) Effect of infant feeding on the risk of obesity across the life course: a quantitative review of published evidence. Pediatrics 115, 13671377.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baird, J, Fisher, D, Lucas, P, et al. (2005) Being big or growing fast: systematic review of size and growth in infancy and later obesity. BMJ 331, 929.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Young, BE, Levek, C, Reynolds, RM, et al. (2018) Bioactive components in human milk are differentially associated with rates of lean and fat mass deposition in infants of mothers with normal vs. elevated BMI: breast milk and body composition partitioning. Pediatr Obes 13, 598606.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wallingford, JC & Barber, C (2019) A review of studies on the growth of infants fed infant formula. Curr Dev Nutr 3, nzz095.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Isselmann DiSantis, K, Collins, BN, Fisher, JO, et al. (2011) Do infants fed directly from the breast have improved appetite regulation and slower growth during early childhood compared with infants fed from a bottle? Int J Behav Nutr Phys Act 8, 89.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Patel, N, Dalrymple, KV, Pasupathy, D, et al. (2018) Mode of infant feeding, eating behaviour and anthropometry in infants at 6-months of age born to obese women–a secondary analysis of the UPBEAT trial. BMC Pregnancy Childbirth 18, 111.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Butte, NF, Wong, WW, Hopkinson, JM, et al. (2000) Infant feeding mode affects early growth and body composition. Pediatrics 106, 13551366.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mannan, H (2018) Early infant feeding of formula or solid foods and risk of childhood overweight or obesity in a socioeconomically disadvantaged region of Australia: a longitudinal cohort analysis. Int J Environ Res Public Health 15, 1685.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Giugliani, ERJ (2018) Growth in exclusively breastfed infants. J Pediatr 95, S79S84.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Koletzko, B, Broekaert, I, Demmelmair, H, et al. (2005) Protein intake in the first year of life: a risk factor for later obesity? The E.U. childhood obesity project. Adv Exp Med Biol 569, 6979.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Koletzko, B, Chourdakis, M, Grote, V, et al. (2014) Regulation of early human growth: impact on long-term health. Ann Nutr Metab 65, 101109.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Koletzko, B, Baker, S, Cleghorn, G, et al. (2005) Global standard for the composition of infant formula: recommendations of an ESPGHAN Coordinated International Expert Group: J Pediatr Gastroenterol Nutr 41, 584599.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fleddermann, M, Demmelmair, H & Koletzko, B (2014) Energetic efficiency of infant formulae: a review. Ann Nutr Metab 64, 276283.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
United States Department of Agriculture (2018) USDA Food Composition Databases. [accessed 2018, Jul 14]. https://fdc.nal.usda.gov/fdc-app.html#/food-details/171279/nutrients Google Scholar
Anatolitou, F (2012) Human milk benefits and breastfeeding. J Pediatr Neonatal Individ Med 1118.Google Scholar
Ballard, O & Morrow, AL (2013) Human milk composition. Pediatr Clin North Am 60, 4974.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lönnerdal, B (2003) Nutritional and physiologic significance of human milk proteins. Am J Clin Nutr 77, 1537S1543S.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Boquien, C-Y (2018) Human milk: an ideal food for nutrition of preterm newborn. Front Pediatr 6, 295.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hoffman, D, Harris, C, Wampler, J, et al. (2019) Growth, tolerance, and DHA and ARA status of healthy term infants receiving formula with two different ARA concentrations: double-blind, randomized, controlled trial. Prostaglandins Leukot Essent Fatty Acids 146, 1927.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ingol, TT, Li, R, Boone, KM, et al. (2019) Docosahexaenoic and arachidonic acid supplementation of toddlers born preterm does not affect short-term growth or adiposity. J Nutr 149, 21822190.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Braegger, C, Chmielewska, A, Decsi, T, et al. (2011) Supplementation of infant formula with probiotics and/or prebiotics: a systematic review and comment by the ESPGHAN committee on nutrition. J Pediatr Gastroenterol Nutr 52, 238250.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
van den Nieuwboer, M, Claassen, E, Morelli, L, et al. (2014) Probiotic and synbiotic safety in infants under two years of age. Benef Microbes 5, 4560.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Figure 0

Fig. 1. Dropout rates and exclusions from the baseline visit to 18 months of age. Infants who did not show up at the follow-up visits, but remained in COGNIS study for later visits are described as ‘did not attend’. SF, standard infant formula; EF, experimental infant formula; BF, breastfed infants; EE, energetic efficiency. D = drop outs and E = exclusions, as previously reported(41). *SF and EF infants were randomised between 0 and 2 months of age; BF infants were included up to 6 months of age.

Figure 1

Table 1. Parents and infants baseline characteristics by study group

Figure 2

Table 2. Effect of the type of feeding on anthropometric data in infants during the first 18 months of life.

Figure 3

Table 3. Daily growth gain in infants of the COGNIS study.

Figure 4

Table 4 Growth velocity and catch-up growth in infants during the first 18 months of life by COGNIS study groups.

Figure 5

Fig. 2. Growth patterns up to 18 months of life, according to weight growth velocity up to 6 months of life by feeding group. Data are marginal mean values and standard deviations. Padj was obtained by ANCOVA for the group differences using univariate general linear model, including the effects of the following potential confounders: maternal age, height and IQ. *Padj-values < 0·05 and **Padj-values < 0·01. SGV, slow growth velocity; NGV, normal growth velocity; RGV, rapid growth velocity; SF, standard infant formula; EF, experimental infant formula; BF, breastfed infants; HC, head circumference; WAZ, weight-for-age z-score; LAZ, length-for-age z-score; WLZ, weight-for-length z-score; HCZ, head circumference-for-age z-score; IQ: intelligence quotient. Dotted lines indicate normal growth according to WHO cut-off points(37). , SF; , BF; , EF.

Figure 6

Fig. 3. Energetic efficiency analysis up to 18 months of age by feeding group. Data are marginal mean values and standard deviations. P-values were obtained by multivariate ANCOVA for group differences using multivariate general linear model, including the following confounders: maternal age, height and IQ. At 6 months of age, analysis was additionally adjusted by volume intake (ml/d) of infant formula or breast milk. For variables weight gain g/g protein; weight gain g/g lipids and length gain mm/g protein at 12 months of age, and length gain mm/g lipids at 6 and 12 months of age, Padj corresponds to log-transformed for these variables. *Padj-values < 0·05; ** Padj-values < 0·01 and *** Padj-values < 0·001. SF, standard infant formula; EF, experimental infant formula; BF, breastfed infants; IQ, intelligence quotient. , SF; , EF; , BF.

Supplementary material: File

Sepúlveda-Valbuena et al. supplementary material

Sepúlveda-Valbuena et al. supplementary material

Download Sepúlveda-Valbuena et al. supplementary material(File)
File 279.7 KB