Hostname: page-component-76fb5796d-22dnz Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-28T00:16:28.031Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Imaginary worlds pervade forager oral tradition

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  18 November 2022

Michelle Scalise Sugiyama*
Affiliation:
Anthropology Department, University of Oregon, Eugene, OR 97401, USA mscalise@uoregon.eduhttps://talkingstories.uoregon.edu/

Abstract

Imaginary worlds recur across hunter-gatherer narrative, suggesting that they are an ancient part of human life: to understand their popularity, we must examine their origins. Hunter-gatherer fictional narratives use various devices to encode factual information. Thus, participation in these invented worlds, born of our evolved ability to engage in pretense, may provide adaptations with information inputs that scaffold their development.

Type
Open Peer Commentary
Copyright
Copyright © The Author(s), 2022. Published by Cambridge University Press

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Attla, C., Jones, E., & Thompson, C. (1990). K'etetaalkkaanee: The one who paddled among the people and animals. Alaska Native Language Center, University of Alaska.Google Scholar
Barber, E. W., & Barber, P. (2004). When they severed earth from sky: How the human mind shapes myth. Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Basso, K. (1996). Wisdom sits in places: Landscape and language among the Western Apache. University of New Mexico Press.Google Scholar
Boas, F. (1898). Introduction. In Teit, J. (Ed.), Traditions of the Thompson River Indians of British Columbia (pp. 118). Houghton, Mifflin and Company.Google Scholar
Clark, E. (1953). Indian legends of the Pacific Northwest. University of California Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dixon, R. (1905). The mythology of the Shasta-Achomawi. American Anthropologist, 7(4), 607612.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jobling, I. (2001). The psychological foundations of the hero-ogre story. Human Nature, 12(3), 247272.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ludwin, R. S., Dennis, R., Carver, D., McMillan, A., Losey, R., Clague, J., … James, K. (2005). Dating the 1700 Cascadia earthquake; great coastal earthquakes in native stories. Seismological Research Letters, 76(2), 140148.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McIlwraith, T. F. (1948). The Bella Coola Indians. University of Toronto Press.Google Scholar
Oman, L. (1995). The epic of Qayaq. Carleton University Press.Google Scholar
Onishi, K. H., Baillargeon, R., & Leslie, A. M. (2007). 15-month-old infants detect violations in pretend scenarios. Acta Psychologica, 124(1), 106128.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Opler, M. (1938). Myths and tales of the Jicarilla Apache Indians. Memoirs of the American Folk-lore Society, XXXI. G.E. Stechert.Google Scholar
Scalise Sugiyama, M. (2005). Reverse-engineering narrative: Evidence of special design. In Gottshall, J., & Wilson, D. S. (Eds.), The literary animal (pp. 177196). Northwestern University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Scalise Sugiyama, M. (2017a). Distant time: A possible typological literary universal. In Hogan, P. (Ed.), The literary universals project. University of Connecticut. http://literary-universals.uconn.edu/2017/01/31/distant-time-a-possible-typological-literary-universal/.Google Scholar
Scalise Sugiyama, M. (2017b). Narrative. In Weekes-Shackelford, V., & Shackelford, T. (Eds.), Encyclopedia of evolutionary psychological science. Springer. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-16999-6_3316-1.Google Scholar
Scalise Sugiyama, M. (2017c). Oral storytelling as evidence of pedagogy in forager societies. Frontiers in Cultural Psychology. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00471.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Scalise Sugiyama, M. (2019). The relevance of popularity: Ecological factors at play in story pervasiveness. In Vanderbeke, D., & Cooke, B. (Eds.), Evolution and popular narrative (pp. 245263). Brill Rodopi.Google Scholar
Scalise Sugiyama, M. (2021a). The fiction that fiction is fiction. ASEBL Journal, 15, 812. https://resources.finalsite.net/images/v1610065663/sfc/pj4pmi0aeaffqpvyasgb/ASEBLJournalvol15January2021.pdfGoogle Scholar
Scalise Sugiyama, M. (2021b). Co-occurrence of ostensive communication and generalizable knowledge in forager storytelling: Cross-cultural evidence of teaching in forager societies. Human Nature, 32(1), 279300.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Scalise Sugiyama, M. (2021c). Talking stories: Encyclopedia of traditional ecological knowledge. https://talkingstories.uoregon.edu/Google Scholar
Steward, J. (1936). Myths of the Owens Valley Paiute. University of California Publications in American Archaeology and Ethnology, 34(5), 355440.Google Scholar
Tonkinson, R. (1978). The Mardudjara aborigines: Living the dream in Australia's desert. Holt, Rinehart and Winston.Google Scholar
Tooby, J., & Cosmides, L. (2001). Does beauty build adapted minds? Toward an evolutionary theory of aesthetics, fiction and the arts. SubStance, 30(1/2), 627.Google Scholar
Wilbert, J., & Simoneau, K. (1983). Folk literature of the Bororo Indians. UCLA Latin American Center Publications, University of California, Los Angeles.Google Scholar
Wilbert, J., & Simoneau, K. (1989). Folk literature of the Toba Indians. UCLA Latin American Center Publications, University of California, Los Angeles.Google Scholar
Wilbert, J., & Simoneau, K. (1990). Folk literature of the Yanomami Indians. UCLA Latin American Center Publications, University of California, Los Angeles.Google Scholar