Hostname: page-component-8448b6f56d-mp689 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-04-23T18:31:43.569Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Elderly suicide rates: the importance of a non-linear relationship with distal risk and protective factors

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  15 May 2012

Ajit Shah
Affiliation:
School of Health, University of Central Lancashire, Preston, UK Email: ajitshah123@btinternet.com
Ravi Bhat
Affiliation:
Rural Health Academic Centre, University of Melbourne; and Centre for Older Persons’ Health, Shepparton, Victoria, Australia
Sofia Zarate-Escudero
Affiliation:
West London Mental Health NHS Trust, London, UK
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Extract

The elderly population size is increasing worldwide due to prolonged life expectancy and falling birth rates. Traditionally, suicide rates increase with age. For example, a recent cross-national study of 62 developing and developed countries reported an increase in suicide rates with aging in males and females in 25 and 27 countries respectively (Shah, 2007a). Thus, suicides in the elderly are an important public health concern. While much is known about proximal (individual level) risk and protective factors for elderly suicides (e.g. Conwell et al., 1991; Cattell and Jolley, 1995; Harwood et al., 2001), less is known about more distal (societal or population level) risk and protective factors (Rehkopf and Buka, 2006). Moreover, detailed knowledge of these distal factors may have greater public health relevance for the development of comprehensive prevention strategies (Knox et al., 2004).

Type
Guest Editorial
Copyright
Copyright © International Psychogeriatric Association 2012

The elderly population size is increasing worldwide due to prolonged life expectancy and falling birth rates. Traditionally, suicide rates increase with age. For example, a recent cross-national study of 62 developing and developed countries reported an increase in suicide rates with aging in males and females in 25 and 27 countries respectively (Shah, Reference Shah2007a). Thus, suicides in the elderly are an important public health concern. While much is known about proximal (individual level) risk and protective factors for elderly suicides (e.g. Conwell et al., Reference Conwell, Olsen, Caine and Flannery1991; Cattell and Jolley, Reference Cattell and Jolley1995; Harwood et al., Reference Harwood, Hawton, Hope and Jacoby2001), less is known about more distal (societal or population level) risk and protective factors (Rehkopf and Buka, Reference Rehkopf and Buka2006). Moreover, detailed knowledge of these distal factors may have greater public health relevance for the development of comprehensive prevention strategies (Knox et al., Reference Knox, Conwell and Caine2004).

Proximal risk and protective factors have generally been examined either descriptively in case-series of elderly suicide victims (Shah and De, Reference Shah and De1998) or analytically in case-control studies (Waern et al., Reference Waern, Rubenowitz and Wilhelmson2003). Such studies have primarily examined linear relationships between suicide and proximal risk and protective factors. This approach is understandable because: (1) in case-series studies the most prevalent characteristics of suicide victims have been interpreted to be the risk factors and the least prevalent protective factors; and (2) in case-control studies the statistically significant differences in the characteristics of suicide victims and case-controls have been interpreted to be the risk or protective factors.

The vast majority of early studies of distal risk and protective factors has also taken a similar approach and mainly focused on identifying presence or absence of linear relationships between these factors and suicide rates. For example, in countries with high socio-economic status, including Japan, England, and Wales, lower societal socio-economic status was reported to be linearly associated with increased elderly suicide rates (Kennedy et al., Reference Kennedy, Iveson and Hill1999; Whitley et al., Reference Whitley, Gunnell, Dorling and Smith1999; Gunnell et al., Reference Gunnell, Middleston, Whitley, Dorling and Frankel2003; Fukuda et al., Reference Fukuda, Nakamura and Takano2005), and there was an absence of such a linear relationship with one measure of socio-economic status in a recent study (Shah et al., Reference Shah, Padayatchi and Das2008a). Presence or absence of such linear relationships may be genuine. However, it is also possible that: (1) some such linear relationships may be erroneous and they may mask genuine non-linear relationships; and (2) the absence of a linear relationship may lead to erroneous conclusion of an absence of a relationship, although this too may mask a genuine non-linear relationship. These issues are examined below because of potentially important implications for the development of preventive strategies at a societal level. There are certain circumstances when consideration should be given to examining non-linear relationships between distal risk and protective factors and elderly suicide rates. These include:

  1. (1) when a predicted linear relationship is not discovered;

  2. (2) when an observed linear relationship cannot be explained;

  3. (3) when there are conflicting findings across studies (both across individual-level and societal-level and between both groups of studies) of any combination of positive, negative, and absent linear correlations, which could include conflicting findings between studies of general population and elderly suicide rates also; and

  4. (4) when there are a priori theoretical reasons to suspect a non-linear relationship. The discrepancies in the previous point may also lead to the generation of an a priori non-linear hypothesis.

Two examples of linear relationships that may have masked non-linear relationships are illustrated below. A series of studies reported a negative linear correlation between fertility rates and general population suicide rates (e.g. Leenaars and Lester, Reference Leenaars and Lester1999; Lester, Reference Lester1999). Based on these studies, the hypothesis that there would be a negative linear correlation between fertility rates and elderly suicide rates at a societal-level was examined and confirmed in a large cross-national study (Shah, Reference Shah2008a). The latter findings were initially explained in part by the hypothesis that higher fertility rates would increase the number of younger people available to support the elderly and lead to reduction in elderly suicide rates (Shah, Reference Shah2008a; Shah et al., Reference Shah, Padayatchi and Das2008b). However, closer re-examination of the original data with scatter plots suggested that a linear negative correlation might have masked a non-linear correlation (Shah, Reference Shah2008b). This was confirmed with curve estimation regression models testing the curvilinear relationship (U-shaped curve) fitting the quadratic equation Y = A + BX + CX 2 (where Y is the elderly suicide rate, X is the fertility rate, and A, B, and C are constants; Shah, Reference Shah2008b). This led to modification of the explanatory model given for the linear relationship. Initially, as fertility rates increase, elderly suicide rates may decrease because increased numbers of younger people would be available to support the elderly, and is consistent with Durkheim's hypothesis suggesting a negative association between social integration and suicide rates (Shah, Reference Shah2008a; Shah et al., Reference Shah, Padayatchi and Das2008b). However, if the fertility rate continues to increase, and beyond a certain critical point, this protective effect of fertility rates may be negated by competition for scarce resources within the family between younger members and the elderly, and lead to higher elderly suicide rates. This is also consistent with Durkheim's hypothesis that the overall cohort size (although in this case it is the family size) may influence suicide rates when there is more competition for scarce resources (Shah and De, Reference Shah and De1998). This explanation has the potential to be rigorously tested in studies examining the relationship between elderly suicide rates and various family variables.

Another example of linear correlations masking possible non-linear relationship emerges from recent studies of socio-economic status. Socio-economic status, measured by income inequality (using the Gini coefficient, where higher scores reflect greater income inequality), was reported to have a negative linear correlation with elderly suicide rates in a large cross-national study (Shah et al., Reference Shah, Padayatchi and Das2008a). However, more recently, a curvilinear (inverted U-shaped curve) relationship between elderly suicide rates and socio-economic status fitting the quadratic equation Y = A + BX − CX 2 (where Y is the suicide rate, X is the socio-economic status, and A, B, and C are constants) was demonstrated using curve estimation regression models in a cross-national study (Shah, Reference Shah2010a). Although this study measured socio-economic status using gross national domestic product (Shah, Reference Shah2010a), the findings are identical when income inequality measured by the Gini coefficient is used as a measure of socio-economic status (unpublished data). These findings resulted in the modification of the original five-stage sequential explanatory model (Shah et al., Reference Shah, Padayatchi and Das2008a) and to the development of a four-stage epidemiological transition model (Shah, Reference Shah2010a), which can also simultaneously explain the wide cross-national variations in (1) elderly suicide rates, (2) time trends for elderly suicide rates, and (3) age-associated trends in suicides rates. This epidemiological transition model is open to rigorous testing in both longitudinal within-country and cross-sectional cross-national studies. There are other studies demonstrating linear relationships that actually mask non-linear relationships (e.g. Shah, Reference Shah2010b).

An example of an absence of a linear correlation masking a possible non-linear relationship emerges from two recent studies of socio-economic status. Socio-economic status, measured by gross national domestic product, was reported to have no linear correlation with elderly suicide rates in a large cross-national study (Shah et al., Reference Shah, Padayatchi and Das2008a). However, more recently, a curvilinear (inverted U-shaped curve) relationship between elderly suicide rates and socio-economic status (measured by gross national domestic product) fitting the quadratic equation Y = A + BX − CX 2 (where Y is the suicide rate, X is the socio-economic status, and A, B, and C are constants) at a societal level was demonstrated by using curve estimation regression models in a cross-national study (Shah, Reference Shah2010a).

An example of conflicting findings with linear relationships leading to the development and subsequent examination of non-linear relationships concerns intelligence and education. A cross-national study of 48 Eurasian countries reported a positive linear correlation between elderly suicide rates and intelligence at a societal level (Voracek, Reference Voracek2005). However, Voracek (Reference Voracek2006a) reviewed this relationship and proposed that the relationship between intelligence and suicide rates in the general population and in the elderly followed an exponential-fit model rather than a linear-fit model. However, this exponential-fit model did not fully explain the observed relationship between suicide and lower levels of intelligence in individual-level studies (O'Toole and Cantor, Reference O'Toole and Cantor1995; Gunnell et al., Reference Gunnell, Magnusson and Rasmussen2005) and an absence of a relationship between suicide rates and intelligence in societal-level studies within several countries (Lester, Reference Lester1993; Reference Lester1995; Voracek, Reference Voracek2006b). Given the strong relationship between intelligence and educational attainment (Voracek, Reference Voracek2006c), this exponential-fit model also does not fully explain the observed relationships between suicide and both high (Kowalski et al., Reference Kowalski, Faupel and Starr1987; Fernquist, Reference Fernquist2001) and low (Willis et al., Reference Willis, Coombs, Drentea and Cockerham2003; Abel and Kruger, Reference Abel and Kruger2005) levels of educational attainment in both individual-level and societal-level studies. A Canadian ecological study observed a negative linear correlation between educational attainment and suicide rates in elderly females at a societal level (Agbayewa et al., Reference Agbayewa, Marion and Wiggins1998). The authors of this Canadian study also acknowledged that a possible non-linear relationship between elderly suicide rates and educational attainment might have been missed (Agbayewa et al., Reference Agbayewa, Marion and Wiggins1998). These discrepant findings and the suggestion of a non-linear relationship prompted a cross-national study which, using curve estimation regression models, reported a curvilinear (U-shaped curve) relationship between elderly suicide rates and educational attainment at a societal level fitting the quadratic equation Y = A + BX + CX 2 (where Y is elderly suicide rates, X is educational attainment, and A, B, and C are constants; Shah and Chaterjee, Reference Shah and Chaterjee2008). These findings led to the development of a possible unitary explanatory model explaining the various discrepant findings in both individual-level and societal-level studies (Shah and Chaterjee, Reference Shah and Chaterjee2008).

An example of a priori theoretical reasons to suspect a non-linear relationship concerns urbanization. A cross-national (Simpson and Conklin, Reference Simpson and Conklin1989) and a single country (Stack, Reference Stack1993) study reported a positive linear correlation between urbanization and general population suicide rates at a societal level. Studies from individual countries (Saunderson and Langford, Reference Saunderson and Langford1996; Yip et al., Reference Yip, Callanan and Yuen2000) reported a positive association between residence in rural areas and suicide in the general population. However, studies from advanced industrialized countries, including general population suicide rates in the USA (Kowalski et al., Reference Kowalski, Faupel and Starr1987) and male suicide rates in the general population in Japan (Otsu et al., Reference Otsu, Araki, Sakai, Yokoyama and Voorhees2004), reported a negative linear correlation. Moreover, a large cross-national study reported no such relationship (Zhang, Reference Zhang1998). Stack (Reference Stack1982; Reference Stack2000) explained these conflicting findings by proposing a curvilinear (quadratic) relationship, whereby the suicide rate would initially increase in the early stages of urbanization due to social disruption following migration from rural to urban areas. Thereafter, the suicide rate would plateau, over several generations, as the new urban dwellers begin to adjust to living in urban areas (Stack, Reference Stack1982; Reference Stack2000). This adjustment may ultimately lead to a gradual reduction in the suicide rate (Stack, Reference Stack1982; Reference Stack2000). Stack (Reference Stack2000) argued that this curvilinear relationship was supported by a longitudinal study of general population suicide rates in Finland (Stack, Reference Stack1993), whereby a 1% increase in the urban population increased the suicide rate by 0.22% between 1800 and 1900, and a 1% increase in the urban population increased the suicide rate by only 0.12% between 1900 and 1985 (Stack, Reference Stack1993). However, this proposed curvilinear relationship was never formally evaluated and prompted a cross-national study at a societal level, which demonstrated a curvilinear (inverted U-shaped curve) fitting the quadratic equation Y = A + BX − CX 2 (where Y is elderly suicide rates, X is degree of urbanization, and A, B, and C are constants) in men using curve estimation regression models (Shah, Reference Shah2008c). This study was able to confirm Stack's (Reference Stack1982; Reference Stack1993; Reference Stack2000) hypothesis and explanatory model for men. Other examples of a priori theoretical reasons to suspect non-linear relationships concern population growth (Shah, Reference Shah2009) and the Human Development Index (Shah, Reference Shah2010b).

Non-linear relationships using techniques other than those discussed above have also been reported. Risk factors attributable to age, period, and cohort membership can independently influence suicide rates at a given age and given time in a given birth cohort (Shah and De, Reference Shah and De1998). Suicide rates in individuals born in a particular cohort are peculiar to that cohort – the cohort effect. The specific age at any given time within the cohort will further influence suicide rates – the age effect. Environmental factors at any specified time period will further influence suicide rates – the period effect. Statistical techniques have been developed to partial out the effect of age, cohort, and period on suicide rates (Murphy et al., Reference Murphy, Lindesay and Grundy1986; Woodbury et al., Reference Woodbury, Manton and Blazer1988; Surtees and Duffy, Reference Surtees and Duffy1989; Skegg and Cox, Reference Skegg and Cox1991). For example, suicide rates for white American and New Zealand males increase with age, but the rates for females initially increase with age, peaking at menopause, and decline thereafter (Woodbury et al., Reference Woodbury, Manton and Blazer1988; Skegg and Cox, Reference Skegg and Cox1991). Another example is the effect of a specified period of time on suicide rates including reduction in suicide rates related to the Second World War (Murphy et al., Reference Murphy, Lindesay and Grundy1986), changeover to non-toxic domestic gas (Murphy et al., Reference Murphy, Lindesay and Grundy1986; Surtees and Duffy, Reference Surtees and Duffy1989), and restriction of barbiturate poisoning (Skegg and Cox, Reference Skegg and Cox1991).

Age-associated trends in suicide rates in men in England and Wales, Scotland, and Northern Ireland are low below the age of 25 to 34 years, peak at 25–34 years age, and then decline until age 55–64 years when they begin to rise (Shah and Coupe, Reference Shah and Coupe2009); this has been observed in other countries including Australia (Snowdon and Hunt, Reference Snowdon and Hunt2002). This appears to follow a cubic pattern of curve regression, but has not been formally tested.

Non-linear models examining the relationship between socio-economic status and other mental disorders in old age including dementia have been developed and partially tested with experimental data (Suh and Shah, Reference Suh and Shah2001; Shah, Reference Shah2007b).

Evidence has been presented from emerging studies that a non-linear correlation between distal risk and protective factors and elderly suicide rates is important. Detailed knowledge of distal risk and protective factors is likely to have greater public health relevance for the development of comprehensive prevention strategies (Knox et al., Reference Knox, Conwell and Caine2004). Unless and until this is consistently and robustly examined, prevention strategies based on erroneous linear correlation or lack of such correlations may lead to development of flawed public health preventive strategies at a societal level.

Conflict of interest

None.

References

Abel, E. L. and Kruger, M. L. (2005). Educational attainment and suicide rates in the United States. Psychological Reports, 97, 2528.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Agbayewa, O. M., Marion, S. A. and Wiggins, S. (1998). Socioeconomic factors associated with suicide in elderly populations in British Columbia: an 11-year review. Canadian Journal of Psychiatry, 43, 829836.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cattell, H. and Jolley, D. (1995). One hundred cases of suicide in the elderly. British Journal of Psychiatry, 166, 451457.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Conwell, Y., Olsen, K., Caine, E. D. and Flannery, C. (1991). Suicide in later life: psychological autopsy findings. International Psychogeriatrics, 3, 5966.Google Scholar
Fernquist, R. M. (2001). Education, race/ethnicity, age, sex, and suicide: individual level data in the United States, 1991–1994. Current Research in Social Psychology, 3, 277290.Google Scholar
Fukuda, Y., Nakamura, K. and Takano, T. (2005). Cause-specific mortality differences across socioeconomic position of municipalities in Japan, 1973–1977 and 1993–1998: increased importance of injury and suicide in inequality for ages under 75. International Journal of Epidemiology, 34, 100109.Google Scholar
Gunnell, D., Middleston, N., Whitley, E., Dorling, D. and Frankel, S. (2003). Why are suicide rates rising in young men but falling in the elderly? A time-series analysis of trends in England and Wales 1950–1998. Social Science & Medicine, 57, 595611.Google Scholar
Gunnell, D., Magnusson, P. K. E. and Rasmussen, F. (2005). Low intelligence test score in 18 year old men and risk of suicide: cohort study. BMJ, 330, 167170.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Harwood, D., Hawton, K., Hope, T. and Jacoby, R. (2001). Psychiatric disorder and personality factors associated with suicide in older people: a descriptive and case-control study. International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 16, 155165.3.0.CO;2-0>CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kennedy, H. G., Iveson, R. C. and Hill, O. (1999). Violence, homicide and suicide: strong correlation and wide variation across districts. British Journal of Psychiatry, 175, 462466.Google Scholar
Knox, K. L., Conwell, Y. and Caine, E. D. (2004). If suicide is a public health problem, what are we doing to prevent it? American Journal of Public Health, 94, 3745.Google Scholar
Kowalski, G. S., Faupel, C. E. and Starr, P. D. (1987). Urbanisation and suicides: a study of American counties. Social Forces, 66, 85101.Google Scholar
Leenaars, A. A. and Lester, D. (1999). Domestic integration and suicide in the provinces of Canada. Crisis, 20, 5963.Google Scholar
Lester, D. (1993). Intelligence and suicide in France: an ecological study. Psychological Reports, 73, 1226.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lester, D. (1995). Intelligence and suicide in Ireland and the United Kingdom. Psychological Reports, 77, 122.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lester, D. (1999). Correlates of suicide rates calculated in different ways: Denmark 1950–1985. Perceptual Motor Skills, 89, 172.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Murphy, E., Lindesay, J. and Grundy, E. (1986). 60 years of suicide in England and Wales: a cohort study. Archive of General Psychiatry, 43, 969976.Google Scholar
O'Toole, B. I. and Cantor, C. (1995). Suicide risk factors among Australian Vietnam era draftees. Suicide and Life-Threatening Behaviour, 25, 475488.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Otsu, A., Araki, S., Sakai, R., Yokoyama, K. and Voorhees, A. S. (2004). Effects of urbanization, economic development, migration of workers on suicide mortality in Japan. Social Science & Medicine, 58, 11371146.Google Scholar
Rehkopf, D. H. and Buka, S. L. (2006). The association between suicide and the socio-economic characteristics of geographical areas: a systematic review. Psychological Medicine, 36, 145157.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Saunderson, T. R. and Langford, I. H. (1996). A study of the geographical distribution of suicide rates in England and Wales 1989–1992 using empirical Bayes estimates. Social Science & Medicine, 43, 489502.Google Scholar
Shah, A. K. (2007a). The relationship between suicide rate and age: an analysis of multinational data from the World Health Organisation. International Psychogeriatrics, 19, 11411152.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shah, A. K. (2007b). The importance of socio-economic status of countries for mental disorders in old age: a development of an epidemiological transition model. International Psychogeriatrics, 19, 785787.Google Scholar
Shah, A. K. (2008a). Association of suicide rates in elderly persons with fertility rates. Psychological Reports, 102, 369376.Google Scholar
Shah, A. K. (2008b). A non-linear relation of fertility and suicide rates in elderly people. Psychological Reports, 103, 943946.Google Scholar
Shah, A. K. (2008c). A cross-national study of the relationship between elderly suicide rates and urbanization. Suicide and Life Threatening Behaviour, 38, 714719.Google Scholar
Shah, A. K. (2009). The relationship between population growth and elderly suicide rates: a cross-national study. International Psychogeriatrics, 21, 379383.Google Scholar
Shah, A. K. (2010a). The possible evidence for epidemiological transition hypothesis for elderly suicides. International Psychogeriatrics, 22, 219226.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shah, A. K. (2010b). A replication of the relationship between elderly suicide rates and the Human Development Index in a cross-national study. International Psychogeriatrics, 22, 727732.Google Scholar
Shah, A. K. and Chaterjee, S. (2008). Is there a relationship between elderly suicide rates and educational attainment? A cross-national study. Ageing & Mental Health, 12, 795799.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shah, A. K. and Coupe, J. (2009). A comparative study of elderly suicides in England and Wales, Scotland and Northern Ireland: trends over time and age-associated trends. International Psychogeriatrics, 21, 581587.Google Scholar
Shah, A. K. and De, T. (1998). Suicide in the elderly. International Journal of Psychiatry in Clinical Practice, 2, 317.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shah, A. K., Bhat, R., MacKenzie, S. and Koen, C. (2008a). A cross-national study of the relationship between elderly suicide rates and life expectancy and markers of socio-economic status and healthcare status. International Psychogeriatrics, 20, 347360.Google Scholar
Shah, A. K., Padayatchi, M. and Das, K. (2008b). The relationship between elderly suicide rates and elderly dependency ratios: a cross-national study using data from the WHO data bank. International Psychogeriatrics, 20, 594604.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Simpson, M. and Conklin, G. (1989). Socio-economic development, suicide and religion: a test of Durkhiem's theory of religion and suicide. Social Forces, 67, 945964.Google Scholar
Skegg, K. and Cox, B. (1991). Suicide in New Zealand 1957–1986: influence of age, period and birth cohort. Australia and New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry, 25, 181190.Google ScholarPubMed
Snowdon, J. and Hunt, G. E. (2002). Age, period and cohort effects on suicide rates in Australia, 1919–1999. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 105, 265270.Google Scholar
Stack, S. (1982). Suicide: a decade review of sociological literature. Deviant Behaviour, 4, 4166.Google Scholar
Stack, S. (1993). The effect of modernization on suicide in Finland, 1800–1985. Sociological Perspective, 36, 137148.Google Scholar
Stack, S. (2000). Suicide – a 15-year review of the sociological literature, Part II: modernization and social integration perspectives. Suicide and Life Threatening Behaviour, 30, 163176.Google Scholar
Suh, G. K. and Shah, A. K. (2001). A review of epidemiological transition in dementia: cross-national comparisons of the indices related to Alzheimer's disease and vascular dementia. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 104, 411.Google Scholar
Surtees, P. and Duffy, J. C. (1989). Suicide in England and Wales 1946–1985: an age period cohort analysis. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 79, 216223.Google Scholar
Voracek, M. (2005). National intelligence, suicide rate in the elderly, and a threshold intelligence for suicidality: an ecological study of 48 Eurasian countries. Journal of Biosocial Science, 37, 721740.Google Scholar
Voracek, M. (2006a). Exponential fitting of suicide rate and national intelligence estimates. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 102, 896898.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Voracek, M. (2006b). Regional intelligence and suicide rate in Germany. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 103, 639642.Google Scholar
Voracek, M. (2006c). Social ecology of intelligence and suicide in the United States. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 102, 767775.Google Scholar
Waern, M., Rubenowitz, E. and Wilhelmson, K. (2003). Predictors of suicide in the old elderly. Gerontologist, 49, 328334.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Whitley, E., Gunnell, D., Dorling, D. and Smith, G. D. (1999). Ecological study of social fragmentation, poverty, and suicide. BMJ, 319, 10341037.Google Scholar
Willis, L. A., Coombs, D. W., Drentea, P. and Cockerham, W. C. (2003). Uncovering the mystery: factors of African American suicide. Suicide and Life Threatening Behaviour, 33, 412429.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Woodbury, M. A., Manton, K. G. and Blazer, D. G. (1988). Trends in US suicide mortality rates 1968–1992: race and sex differences in age, period and cohort components. International Journal of Epidemiology, 17, 356362.Google Scholar
Yip, P. S. F., Callanan, C. and Yuen, H. P. (2000). Urban/rural and gender differentials in suicide rates: East and West. Journal of Affective Disorders, 57, 99106.Google Scholar
Zhang, J. (1998). Suicide in the world: toward a population increase theory of suicide. Death Studies, 22, 525539.Google Scholar