Hostname: page-component-848d4c4894-75dct Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-05-08T18:03:30.926Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Are all interferences bad? Bilingual advantages in working memory are modulated by varying demands for controlled processing*

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  22 September 2015

HWAJIN YANG*
Affiliation:
Singapore Management University, Singapore
SUJIN YANG
Affiliation:
Yonsei University, South Korea
*
Address for correspondence: Hwajin Yang, Ph.D., School of Social Sciences, Singapore Management University, 90 Stamford Rd, Singaporehjyang@smu.edu.sg

Abstract

We investigated bilingual advantages in general control abilities using three complex-span tasks of working memory (WM). An operation-span task served as a baseline measure of WM capacity. Additionally, two modified versions of the Stroop-span task were designed to place varying attentional-control demands during memoranda encoding by asking participants either to read the to-be-remembered item aloud (lower cognitive control; i.e., Stroop-span task) or to name the font color of the to-be-remembered item while still encoding the word for later recall (greater cognitive control; i.e., attention-impeded Stroop-span task). Twenty-six Korean–English bilinguals and 25 English-native monolinguals were tested. We found that bilinguals outperformed monolinguals on the attention-impeded Stroop-span task, but on neither the operation-span nor the Stroop-span task. Our findings demonstrate that bilingualism provides advantages in controlled processing, an important component of WM and other executive functions, suggesting that the demand for controlled processing in WM tasks moderates bilingual effects on WM.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2015 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Footnotes

*

We thank Kenneth Paap and two anonymous reviewers for their excellent comments. This research was supported by a research grant from Singapore Management University through a research grant (C242/MSS13S011) from the Ministry of Education Academic Research Fund Tier 1.

References

Anderson, J. R., & Lebiere, C. (1998). The atomic components of thought. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates Publishers.Google Scholar
Baddeley, A. D. (1986). Working Memory. New York: Oxford University Press.Google ScholarPubMed
Baddeley, A. (1998). Random generation and the executive control of working memory. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology: Section A, 51 (4), 819852.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baddeley, A., Della Sala, S., Papagno, C., & Spinnler, H. (1997). Dual-task performance in dysexecutive and nondysexecutive patients with a frontal lesion. Neuropsychology, 11 (2), 187.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baddeley, A.D., Emslie, H., Kolodny, J., & Duncan, J. (1998). Random generation and the executive control of working memory. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 51A, 819852.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baddeley, A.D., & Hitch, G.J. (1974). Working memory. In Bower, G.A. (Ed.), Recent advances in learning and motivation (Vol. 8, pp. 4790). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Barrett, L. F., Tugade, M. M., & Engle, R. W. (2004). Individual differences in working memory capacity and dual-process theories of the mind. Psychological bulletin, 130 (4), 553573.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Battig, W. F., & Montague, W. E. (1969). Category norms for verbal items in 56 categories: A replication and extension of the Connecticut norms. Journal of experimental Psychology, 80, (3, pt. 2).146.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bialystok, E. (2009). Bilingualism: The good, the bad, and the indifferent. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 12, 313.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bialystok, E., & Feng, X. (2010). Language proficiency and its implications for monolingual and bilingual children. In Durgunoglu, A. Y. & Goldenberg, C. (Eds.), Dual language learners: The development and assessment of oral and written language (pp. 121138). New York: Guilford.Bialystok and Martin, 2004.Google Scholar
Bialystok, E., Craik, F. I. M., & Luk, G. (2008). Cognitive control and lexical access in younger and older bilinguals. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 34, 859873.Google ScholarPubMed
Bialystok, E., Craik, F. I., & Luk, G. (2012). Bilingualism: consequences for mind and brain. Trends in cognitive sciences, 16 (4), 240250.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bialystok, E., & Martin, M. M. (2004). Attention and inhibition in bilingual children: Evidence from the dimensional change card sort task. Developmental science, 7 (3), 325339.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Blom, E., Küntay, A. C., Messer, M., Verhagen, J., & Leseman, P. (2014). The benefits of being bilingual: Working memory in bilingual Turkish–Dutch children. Journal of experimental child psychology, 128, 105119.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bogulski, C. A., Rakoczy, M., Goodman, M., & Bialystok, E. (2015). Executive control in fluent and lapsed bilinguals. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 18 (03), 561567 CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bonifacci, P., Giombini, L., Bellocchi, S., & Contento, S. (2011). Speed of processing, anticipation, inhibition and working memory in bilinguals. Developmental science, 14 (2), 256269.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cowan, N. (1995). Attention and memory: An integrated framework. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Conway, A. R., Kane, M. J., Bunting, M. F., Hambrick, D. Z., Wilhelm, O., & Engle, R. W. (2005). Working memory span tasks: A methodological review and user's guide. Psychonomic bulletin & review, 12 (5), 769786.Google ScholarPubMed
Dickinson, E. R., & Adelson, J. L. (2014). Exploring the limitations of measures of students’ socioeconomic staus (SES). Practical Assessment, Research, and Evaluation. 19 (1), 114 Google Scholar
Engel de Abreu (2011). Working memory in multilingual children: Is there a bilingual effect? Memory, 19, 529537.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Engle, R. W. (2002). Working memory capacity as executive attention. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 11, 1923.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Engle, R. W., & Kane, M. J. (2004). Executive attention, working memory capacity, and a two-factor theory of cognitive control. In Ross, B. (Ed.), The psychology of learning and motivation (pp. 145199). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Ganesalingam, K., Yeates, K. O., Taylor, H. G., Walz, N. C., Stancin, T., & Wade, S. (2011). Executive functions and social competence in young children 6 months Following traumatic brain injury. Neuropsychology, 25 (4), 466.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Green, D. W. (1998). Mental control of the bilingual lexico-semantic system. Bilingualism: Language and cognition, 1 (02), 6781.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Green, D. W. (2011). Language control in different contexts: the behavioral ecology of bilingual speakers. Frontiers in psychology, 2.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hayes, M. G., Kelly, A. J., & Smith, A. D. (2013). The Journals of Gerontology, Series B: Psychological Sciences Social Science, 68 (2), 176183.Google Scholar
Hernández, M., Martin, C. D., Barceló, F., & Costa, A. (2013). Where is the bilingual advantage in task-switching?. Journal of Memory and Language, 69 (3), 257276.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hilchey, M. D., & Klein, R. M. (2011). Are there bilingual advantages on nonlinguistic interference tasks? Implications for the plasticity of executive control processes. Psychonomic bulletin & review, 18 (4), 625658.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kane, M. J., Conway, A. R., Hambrick, D. Z., & Engle, R. W. (2007). Variation in working memory capacity as variation in executive attention and control. Variation in working memory, 2148.Google Scholar
Kroll, J. F., & Bialystok, E. (2013). Understanding the consequences of bilingualism for language processing and cognition. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 25 (5), 497514.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Luk, G., De Sa, E., & Bialystok, E. (2011). Is there a relation between onset age of bilingualism and enhancement of cognitive control?. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 14 (04), 588595.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Luo, L., Craik, F. I., Moreno, S., & Bialystok, E. (2013). Bilingualism interacts with domain in a working memory task: Evidence from aging. Psychology and aging, 28 (1), 28.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Marian, V., & Spivey, M. (2003). Competing activation in bilingual language processing: Within-and between-language competition. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 6 (2), 97115.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McCabe, D. P., Roediger III, H. L., McDaniel, M. A., Balota, D. A., & Hambrick, D. Z. (2010). The relationship between working memory capacity and executive functioning: evidence for a common executive attention construct. Neuropsychology, 24 (2), 222243.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McCabe, D. P., Robertson, C. L., & Smith, A. D. (2005). Age differences in Stroop interference in working memory. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 27 (5), 633644.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McEvoy, C. L., & Nelson, D. L. (1982). Category name and instance norms for 106 categories of various sizes. The American Journal of Psychology, 581634.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McVay, J. C., & Kane, M. J. (2009). Conducting the train of thought: Working memory capacity, goal neglect, and mind wandering in an executive-control task. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 35, 196204.Google Scholar
Milner, B. (1971). Interhemispheric differences in the localization of psychological processes in man. Cortex, 27, 272277.Google ScholarPubMed
Miyake, A., Friedman, N. P., Emerson, M. J., Witzki, A. H., Howerter, A., & Wager, T. D. (2000). The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex “frontal lobe” tasks: A latent variable analysis. Cognitive psychology, 41 (1), 49100.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Morales, J., Calvo, A., & Bialystok, E. (2013). Working memory development in monolingual and bilingual children. Journal of experimental child psychology, 114 (2), 187202.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Namazi, M., & Thordardottir, E. (2010). A working memory, not bilingual advantage, in controlled attention. International Journal of Bilingual Education and Bilingualism, 13 (5), 597616.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Paap, K. R., & Greenberg, Z. I. (2013). There is no coherent evidence for a bilingual advantage in executive processing. Cognitive psychology, 66 (2), 232258.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Paap, K. R., Johnson, H. A., & Sawi, O. (2014). Are bilingual advantages dependent upon specific tasks or specific bilingual experiences?. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 26 (6), 615639.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Paap, K. R., & Sawi, O. (2014). Bilingual advantages in executive functioning: problems in convergent validity, discriminant validity, and the identification of the theoretical constructs. Frontiers in psychology, 5, 96. Doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00962 CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Poarch, G. J., & van Hell, J. G. (2012). Executive functions and inhibitory control in multilingual children: Evidence from second-language learners, bilinguals, and trilinguals. Journal of experimental child psychology, 113 (4), 535551.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sullivan, M. D., Janus, M., Moreno, S., Astheimer, L., & Bialystok, E. (2014). Early stage second-language learning improves executive control: Evidence from ERP. Brain and language, 139, 8498.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tan, J. S., Yang, H., & Yang, S. (2014). A holistic intervention program for children from low socioeconomic status families. Frontiers in psychology, 5.Google ScholarPubMed
Tao, L., Marzecová, A., Taft, M., Asanowicz, D., & Wodniecka, Z. (2011). The efficiency of attentional networks in early and late bilinguals: The role of age of acquisition. Frontiers in Psychology, 2, 123. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00123.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Thomas, J. G., Milner, H. R., & Haberlandt, K. F. (2003). Forward and backward recall different response time patterns, same retrieval order. Psychological Science, 14 (2), 169174.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Turner, M. L., & Engle, R. W. (1989). Is working memory capacity task dependent?. Journal of memory and language, 28 (2), 127154.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Unsworth, N., Redick, T. S., Spillers, G. J., & Brewer, G. A. (2012). Variation in working memory capacity and cognitive control: Goal maintenance and micro adjustments of control. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 65 (2), 326355.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Valian, V. (2015). Bilingualism and cognition. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 18 (1), 324.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Weber-Fox, C., & Neville, H. (1996). Maturational constraints on functional specializations for language processing: ERP and behavioral evidence in bilingual speakers. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 8 (3), 231256.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yang, H., Yang, S., Ceci, S. J., & Wang, Q. (2005). Effects of bilinguals’ controlled- attention on working memory and recognition. In ISB4: Proceedings of the 4th International Symposium on Bilingualism (pp. 24012404). Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Press.Google Scholar
Yang, H., Yang, S., & Isen, A. M. (2013). Positive affect improves working memory: Implications for controlled cognitive processing. Cognition & emotion, 27 (3), 474482.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yang, S., Yang, H., & Lust, B. (2011). Early childhood bilingualism leads to advances in executive attention: Dissociating culture and language. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 14 (3), 412422.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Yow, W. Q., & Li, X. (2015). Balanced bilingualism and early age of second language acquisition as the underlying mechanisms of a bilingual executive control advantage: why variations in bilingual experiences matter. Frontiers in Psychology 6:164. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00164.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed