Skip to main content
Log in

The Relation Between Gesture and Speech in Congenitally Blind and Sighted Language-Learners

  • Published:
Journal of Nonverbal Behavior Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Gesture is widely regarded to play an important role in communication, both in conjunction with and independent of speech. Indeed, gesture is known to develop even before the onset of spoken words. However, little is known about the communicative conditions under which gesture emerges. The aim of this study was to explore the role of vision in early gesturing. We examined gesture development in 5 congenitally blind and 5 sighted toddlers videotaped longitudinally between the ages of 14 and 28 months in their homes while engaging in free play with a parent or experimenter. All of the blind children were found to produce at least some gestures during the one-word stage of language development. However, gesture production was relatively low among the blind children relative to their sighted peers. Moreover, although blind and sighted children produced the same overall set of gesture types, the distribution of gesture types across categories differed. In addition, blind children used gestures primarily to communicate about objects that were nearby, while sighted children used them for nearby as well as distally located objects. These findings suggest that gesture may play different roles in the language-learning process for sighted and blind children. Nevertheless, they also make it clear that gesture is a robust phenomenon of early communicative development, emerging even in the absence of experience with a visual model.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Acredolo, L. P., & Goodwyn, S. W. (1985). Symbolic gesturing in language development: A case study. Human Development, 28, 40–49.

    Google Scholar 

  • Acredolo, L. P., & Goodwyn, S. W. (1988). Symbolic gesturing in normal infants. Child Development, 59, 450–466.

    Google Scholar 

  • Andersen, E.S., Dunlea, A., & Kekelis, L.S. (1984). Blind children's language: Resolving some differences. Journal of Child Language, 11, 645–664.

    Google Scholar 

  • Andersen, E.S., Dunlea, A., & Kekelis, L.S. (1993). The impact of input: Language acquisition in the visually impaired. First Language, 13, 23–49.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bates, E. (1976) Language and context. New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bates, E., Benigni, L., Bretherton, I., Camaioni, L. & Volterra, V. (1979). The emergence of symbols: Cognition and communication in infancy. New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bigelow, A. (1987). Early words of blind children. Journal of Child Language, 14, 47–56.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bloom, L., & Capatides, J. B. (1987). Expression of affect and the emergence of language. Child Development, 58, 1513–1522.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bowerman, M. (1973). Early syntactic development: A cross-linguistic study with special reference to Finnish. New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Braine, M. D. S. (1976). Children's first word combinations. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 41 (1, Serial No. 164).

  • Bretherton, I., Bates, E., McNew, S., Williamson, C., & Beeghly-Smith, M. (1981). Comprehension and production of symbols in infancy: An experimental study. Developmental Psychology, 17, 728–736.

    Google Scholar 

  • Butcher, C., & Goldin-Meadow, S. (in press). Gesture and the transition from one-to two word speech: When hand and mouth come together. In D. McNeill (Ed.), Language and gesture: Window into thought and action. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

  • Butcher, C., Mylander, C., & Goldin-Meadow, S. (1991). Displaced communication in a selfstyled gesture system: Pointing at the nonpresent. Cognitive Development, 6, 315–342.

    Google Scholar 

  • Capirci, O., Iverson, J. M., Pizzuto, E., & Volterra, V. (1996). Communicative gestures during the transition to two-word speech. Journal of Child Language, 23, 645–673.

    Google Scholar 

  • Caselli, M. C. (1990). Communicative gestures and first words. In V. Volterra & C. J. Erting (Eds.), From gesture to language in hearing and deaf children (pp. 56–67). New York: Springer-Verlag.

    Google Scholar 

  • Conti-Ramsden, G. & Pérez-Pereira, M. (1999). Social interaction and language development in blind children. London: Psychology Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dunlea, A. (1989). Vision and the emergence of meaning. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dunlea, A., & Andersen, E. S. (1992). The emergence process: Conceptual and linguistic influences on morphological development. First Language, 12, 95–115.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goldin-Meadow, S. (1998). The development of gesture and speech as an integrated system. In J. M. Iverson & S. Goldin-Meadow (Eds.), The nature and functions of gesture in children's communications. New Directions for Child Development, no. 79 (pp. 29–42). San Francisco: Jossey Bass.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goldin-Meadow, S., & Butcher, C. (2000). Pointing toward two-word speech in young children. To appear in S. Kita (Ed.), Pointing: Where language, culture, and cognition meet. Manuscript under review.

  • Goldin-Meadow, S., & Morford, M. (1985). Gesture in early child language: Studies of deaf and hearing children. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 31, 145–76.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goldin-Meadow, S., & Mylander, C. (1984). Gestural communication in deaf children: The effects and non-effects of parental input on early language development. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 49 (1, Serial No. 121).

  • Goodwyn, S., & Acredolo, L. (1998). Encouraging symbolic gestures: A new perspective on the relationship between gesture and speech. In J.M. Iverson & S. Goldin-Meadow (Eds.), The nature and functions of gesture in children's communications. New Directions for Child Development, no. 79 (pp. 61–73). San Francisco: Jossey Bass.

    Google Scholar 

  • Iverson, J. M. (1998). Gesture when there is no gestural model. In J. M. Iverson & S. Goldin-Meadow (Eds.), The nature and functions of gesture in children's communication. New Directions for Child Development, no. 79 (pp. 89–100). San Francisco: Jossey Bass.

    Google Scholar 

  • Iverson, J. M. (1999). How to get to the cafeteria: Gesture and speech in blind and sighted children's spatial descriptions. Developmental Psychology, 35, 1132–1142.

    Google Scholar 

  • Iverson, J. M., Capirci, O., & Caselli, M. C. (1994). From communication to language in two modalities. Cognitive Development, 9, 23–43.

    Google Scholar 

  • Iverson, J. M., & Goldin-Meadow, S. (1997). What's communication got to do with it: Gesture in children blind from birth. Developmental Psychology, 33, 453–467.

    Google Scholar 

  • Landau, B., & Gleitman, L.R. (1985). Language and experience: Evidence from the blind child. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Menyuk, P., Liebergott, J. W., & Schultz, M. C. (1995). Early language development in fullterm and premature infants. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mills, A. E. (1988). Visual impairment. In D. Bishop & K. Mogford (Eds.), Language development in exceptional circumstances. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Morford, M., & Goldin-Meadow, S. (1992). Comprehension and production of gesture in combination with speech in one-word speakers. Journal of Child Language, 9, 559–580.

    Google Scholar 

  • Petitto, L. A. (1988) “Language” in the pre-linguistic child. In F. Kessel (Ed.), The development of language and language researchers: Essays in honor of Roger Brown (pp. 187–221). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum Associates.

    Google Scholar 

  • Piaget, J. (1951). Play, dreams, and imitation in childhood. New York: Norton.

    Google Scholar 

  • Preisler, G. (1993). Developing communication with blind and deaf infants. Reports from the Department of Psychology, Stockholm University, No. 761.

  • Thal, D., & Tobias, S. (1991). Communicative gestures in children with delayed onset of oral expressive vocabulary. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 35, 1281–1289.

    Google Scholar 

  • Thal, D., & Tobias, S. (1994). Relationships between language and gesture in normally-developing and late-talking toddlers. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 37, 157–170.

    Google Scholar 

  • Thal, D., Tobias, S., & Morrison, D. (1991). Language and gesture in late talkers: A one year follow-up. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 34, 604–612.

    Google Scholar 

  • Urwin, C. (1979). Preverbal communication and early language development in blind children. Papers and Reports in Child Language Development, 17, 119–127.

    Google Scholar 

  • Volterra, V. (1981). Gestures, signs, and words at two years: When does communication become language? Sign Language Studies, 33, 351–362.

    Google Scholar 

  • Werner, H., & Kaplan, B. (1963). Symbol formation. New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Iverson, J.M., Tencer, H.L., Lany, J. et al. The Relation Between Gesture and Speech in Congenitally Blind and Sighted Language-Learners. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior 24, 105–130 (2000). https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1006605912965

Download citation

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1006605912965

Keywords

Navigation