Skip to main content
Log in

Immediate Memory for Mismatched Speech and Representational Gesture Across Development

  • Published:
Journal of Nonverbal Behavior Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Two experiments examined how 7- and 8-year-old children, 9- and 10-year-old children, and adults process mismatched, task-related speech and gesture differently as a function of development. Participants watched videotapes of children speaking and gesturing about the concept of conservation. Using a recognition paradigm, we assessed immediate memory for information conveyed in mismatched speech and gesture. In Experiment 1, we used recognition of verbal statements to probe participants' memory, whereas in Experiment 2, we used recognition of gestural statements to probe memory. When probed with verbal statements in Experiment 1, 9- and 10-year-old children failed to retrieve gestured information. When probed with gestural statements in Experiment 2, 9- and 10-year-old children failed to retrieve verbal information. In contrast, the younger children and adults showed retrieval of both verbal and gestural information across both recognition methods in Experiments 1 and 2. These results suggest a U-shaped function with the 9- and 10-year-old children showing a limitation in the ability to process contradictory messages simultaneously conveyed in two modalities. Implications for identifying a transitional period in the development of representational skills are discussed.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Alibali, M., & Goldin-Meadow, S. (1993). Gesture-speech mismatch and mechanisms of learning: What the hands reveal about a child's state of mind. Cognitive Psychology, 25, 468–523.

    Google Scholar 

  • Alibali, M., Flevaras, L., & Goldin-Meadow, S. (1997). Assessing knowledge conveyed in gesture: Do teachers have the upper hand? Journal of Educational Psychology, 89, 183–193.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ayman-Nolley, S., & Taira, L. L. (1998, June). Use of realistic color in middle childhood. Paper presented at the annual meeting of the Jean Piaget Society, Chicago, IL.

  • Berman, R. A., & Slobin, D. I. (1994). Relating events in narrative: an across linguistic study. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bidell, T. R., & Fischer, K. W. (1995). Developmental transitions in children's on-line planning. In M. M. Haith, J. B. Benson, R. J. Roberts, Jr., & B. F. Pennington (Eds.), The development of future oriented processes (pp. 141–176). Chicago: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bjorklund, D. F. (1995). Children's thinking: Developmental function and individual differences. Pacific Grove, CA: Brooks/Cole Publishing Company.

    Google Scholar 

  • Boyatzis, C. J., & Watson, M. W. (1993). Preschool children's symbolic representation of objects through gesture. Child Development, 64, 729–735.

    Google Scholar 

  • Boyatzis, C. J., & Satyaprasad, C. (1994). Children's facial and gestural decoding and encoding: Relations between skills and with popularity. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 18, 37–55.

    Google Scholar 

  • Capelli, C. A., Nakagawa, N., & Madden, C. M. (1990). How children understand sarcasm: The role of context and intonation. Child Development, 61, 1824–1841.

    Google Scholar 

  • Church, R. B. (1999). Using gesture and speech to capture transitions. Cognitive Development, 14, 313–342.

    Google Scholar 

  • Church, R. B., Ayman-Nolley, S., Alibali, M. W., & Kelly, S. D. (1999, April). Can we learn something from televised speech and gesture? Paper presented at the biennial meeting of the Society for Research in Child Development, Albuquerque, New Mexico.

  • Church, R. B., & Goldin-Meadow, S. (1986). The mismatch between gesture and speech as an index of transitional knowledge. Cognition, 23, 43–71.

    Google Scholar 

  • Church, R. B., Schonert-Reichl, K., Goodman, N., Kelly, S. D., & Ayman-Nolley, S. (1995). The role of gesture and speech communication as a reflection of cognitive understanding. Journal of Contemporary Legal Issues, 6, 123–154.

    Google Scholar 

  • Demorest, A., Meyer, C., Phelps, E., Gardner, H., & Winner, E. (1984). Words speak louder than actions: Understanding deliberately false remarks. Child Development, 55, 1527–1534.

    Google Scholar 

  • Diamond, R., & Carey, S. (1977). Developmental changes in the representation of faces. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 23, 1–22.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dweck, C. (1990). Adolescent motivation. In S. S. Feldman & G. R. Elliot (Eds.) At the threshold: The developing adolescent (pp. 33–51). Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ekman, P., & Friesen, W. V. (1969a). The repertoire of nonverbal behavior: Categories, origins, usage and coding. Semiotica, 1, 49–98.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ekman, P., & Friesen, W. V. (1969b). Nonverbal leakage and clues to deception. Psychiatry, 32, 88–106.

    Google Scholar 

  • Fernandez, E., Flevares, L. M., Goldin-Meadow, S., & Kurtzberg, T. (1996, April). The role of hand in teacher-student interaction. Paper presented at the annual meeting of the American Educational Research Association, New York, NY.

  • Flin, R. H. (1980). Age effects in children's memory for unfamiliar faces. Developmental Psychology, 16, 373–374.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goldin-Meadow, S., Alibali, M., & Church, R. B. (1993). Transitions in concept acquisition: Using the hand to read the mind. Psychological Review, 100, 279–297.

    Google Scholar 

  • Goldin-Meadow, S., Kim, S., & Singer, M. (1999). What the teachers' hands tell the students' mind about math. Journal of Educational Psychology, 91, 720–730.

    Google Scholar 

  • Granott, N. (1995, November). How is knowledge co-constructed? A microdevelopmental view. Paper presented at the biennial meeting of the Society of Research in Child Development, Indianapolis, IN.

  • Hayakawa, K., Konishi, Y., Matsuda, T., Kuriyama, M., Konishi, K., Yamashita, K., Okumura, R., & Hamanaka, D. (1989). Development and aging of brain structure: Assessment with MR Imaging. Neuroradiology, 172, 171–177.

    Google Scholar 

  • Joseph, R., & Gallagher, R. E. (1985). Interhemispheric transfer and the completion of reversible operations in non-conserving children. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 41, 796–800.

    Google Scholar 

  • Karmiloff-Smith, A. (1983). Language acquisition as a problem-solving process. In Papers and reports on child language development, Vol. 22 (pp. 1–22). Stanford, CA: Stanford University Publications.

    Google Scholar 

  • Karmiloff-Smith, A. (1984). Children's problem solving. In B. Rogoff (Ed.), Advances in developmental psychology, Vol. 3 (pp. 33–90). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kelly, S. D., & Church, R. B. (1997). Can children detect information conveyed through other children's nonverbal behaviors? Cognition and Instruction, 15, 107–134.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kelly, S. D., & Church, R. B. (1998). A comparison between children's and adults' ability to detect representational information conveyed through nonverbal behaviors. Child Development, 69, 85–93.

    Google Scholar 

  • Leippe, M. R., Manion, A. P., & Romanczyk, A. (1991). Eyewitness memory for a touching experience: Accuracy differences between child and adult witnesses. Journal of Applied Psychology, 76, 367–378.

    Google Scholar 

  • List, J. A. (1986). Age and schematic difference in reliability of eyewitness testimony. Developmental Psychology, 22, 50–57.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lucy, J. A. (1996). The scope of linguistic relativity: A review of empirical research. In J. Gumperz & S. Levinson (Eds.), Rethinking linguistic relativity (pp. 37–69). New York: Cambridge University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lucy, J. A., & Gaskins, S. (in press). Grammatical categories and the development of classification preferences: A comparative approach. In S. Levinson and M. Bowerman (Eds.), Language acquisition and conceptual development. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.

  • McClintock, M. K., & Herdt, G. (1996). Rethinking puberty: The development of sexual attraction. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 5, 178–183.

    Google Scholar 

  • McCluskey, K. W., & Albas, D. C. (1981). Perception of contradictory speech by normal and disturbed children at various age levels. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 90, 490–493.

    Google Scholar 

  • McNeill, D. (1992). Hand and mind: What gesture reveals about thought. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mehrabian, A., & Ferris, S. R. (1967). Inference of attitudes from nonverbal communication in two channels. Journal of Consulting Psychology, 31, 248–252.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mehrabian, A., & Weiner, M. (1967). Decoding of inconsistent communication. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 6, 109–114.

    Google Scholar 

  • Miller, S. A. (1998). Developmental research methods. New Jersey: Prentice Hall.

    Google Scholar 

  • Perry, M., Church, R. B., & Goldin-Meadow, S. (1992). Is gesture-speech mismatch a general index of transitional knowledge? Cognitive Development, 7, 109–122.

    Google Scholar 

  • Perry, M., & Elder, A. D. (1997). Knowledge in transition: Adults' developing understanding of a principle of physical causality. Cognitive Development, 12, 131–157.

    Google Scholar 

  • Perry, M., Wooley, J., & Ifcher, J. (1995). Adults' abilities to detect children's readiness to learn. International Journal of Behavioral Development. 18, 365–381.

    Google Scholar 

  • Reilly, S., & Muzekari, L. H. (1979). Response of normal and disturbed adults and children to mixed messages. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 88, 203–208.

    Google Scholar 

  • Reilly, S. S., & Muzekari, L. H. (1986). Effects of emotional illness and age upon the resolution of discrepant messages. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 62, 823–829.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schwartz, A. R., & Schwanenflugel, P. J. (1989). Eyewitness testimony of children and the school psychologist. School Psychology Review, 18, 235–246.

    Google Scholar 

  • Solomon, D., & Ali, F. A. (1972). Age trends in the perception of verbal reinforcers. Developmental Psychology, 7, 238–243.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sroufe, A., Cooper, R., & DeHart, G. (1998). Child development: Its nature and course. New York: McGraw-Hill.

    Google Scholar 

  • Thelen, E., & Smith. L. B. (1994). A dynamic approach to the development of cognition and action. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Thompson, L. A., & Massaro, D. W. (1994). Children's integration of speech and pointing gestures in comprehension. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 57, 327–354.

    Google Scholar 

  • Thompson, L. A., Driscoll, D., & Markson, L. (1998). Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 22, 167–187.

    Google Scholar 

  • Woolfolk, A. E., & Galloway, C. M. (1985). Nonverbal behavior and the study of teaching. Theory Into Practice, 24, 77–84.

    Google Scholar 

  • Zelazo, P. D., Frye, D., & Rapus, T. (1996). An age-related dissociation between knowing rules and using them. Cognitive Development, 11, 37–63.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Church, R.B., Kelly, S.D. & Lynch, K. Immediate Memory for Mismatched Speech and Representational Gesture Across Development. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior 24, 151–174 (2000). https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1006610013873

Download citation

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1006610013873

Keywords

Navigation