Skip to main content
Log in

A Cognitive Model of Hallucinations

  • Published:
Cognitive Therapy and Research Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

Auditory hallucinations in patients with schizophrenia can be understood within a cognitive framework that incorporates relevant biological constructs. The formation, fixation, and maintenance of hallucinations are dependent on multiple determinants: Hypervalent (“hot ”) cognitions of sufficient energy to exceed perceptual threshold and consequently to be transformed into hallucinations, a low threshold for auditory perceptualization exacerbated by stress, isolation, or fatigue, an externalizing bias that reinforces the purported external origin of the voices and resource-sparing strategies that help to fix belief in external origin and diminished reality-testing: detecting and correcting errors; suspending judgment; collecting more data, reappraisal, and alternative explanations. The maintenance of hallucinations is, in turn, determined by a range of beliefs: delusions regarding an external agent, underlying core beliefs, and the perceived “relationship” with the voices. Specific coping responses and safety-seeking behaviors are also implicated. Biological factors that contribute to hallucinations are the hypoconnectivity resulting from excessive priming of neurons during adolescence, which reduces resources available for higher level cognitive functioning. This results in reduced reality testing and reliance on low-level dysfunctional reasoning strategies. In addition, the cerebral flooding with dopamine and other transmitters (possibly in reaction to the neuronal loss) starts to “hyperprime” the salient cognitions (self-evaluative, intrusive, or obsessive) and leads to their crossing the perceptual threshold to hallucinations.

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

References

  • Aleman, A. (2001). Cognitive neuropsychiatry of hallucinations in schizophrenia: How the brain misleads itself. Tekst: Proefschrift Universiteit Utrecht.

    Google Scholar 

  • Aleman, A., Böcker, K. B. E., & de Haan, E. N. F. (2001). Hallucinatory predisposition and vividness of auditory imagery: Self-report and behavioral indices. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 93, 268–274.

    Google Scholar 

  • Arntz, A., Rauner, M., & van den Hout, M. (1995). “If I feel anxious there must be danger”: Ex-consequentia reasoning in inferring danger in anxiety disorders. Behavior Research and Therapy, 33, 917–925.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baddeley, A. D. (1986). Working memory. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Baker, C., & Morrison, A. (1998). Metacognition, intrusive thoughts and auditory hallucinations. Psychological Medicine, 28, 1199–1208.

    Google Scholar 

  • Barber, T. X., & Calverly, D. S. (1964). An experimental study of 'hypnotic' (auditory and visual) hallucinations. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 63, 13–20.

    Google Scholar 

  • Barrett, T. R. (1992). Verbal hallucinations in normals: I. People who hear “voices.” Applied Cognitive Psychology, 6(5), 379–387.

    Google Scholar 

  • Beck, A. T. (1976). Cognitive therapy and the emotional disorders. New York: Meridian.

    Google Scholar 

  • Beck, A. T., & Rector, N. A. (in press). Delusions: A cognitive perspective. Journal of Cognitive Psychotherapy: An International Quarterly.

  • Beck-Sander, A., Birchwood, M., & Chadwick, P. (1997). Acting on command hallucinations: A cognitive approach. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 36(1), 139–148.

    Google Scholar 

  • Behrendt, R. (1998). Underconstrained perception: A theoretical approach to the nature and function of verbal hallucinations. Comprehensive Psychiatry, 39, 236–248.

    Google Scholar 

  • Benjamin, L. S. (1989). Is chronicity a function of the relationship between the person and the auditory hallucination? Schizophrenia Bulletin, 15(2), 291–310.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bentall, R. P. (1990). The illusion of reality: A review and integration of psychological research on hallucinations. Psychological Bulletin, 107(1), 82–95.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bentall, R., Baker, G., & Havers, S. (1991). Reality monitoring and psychotic hallucinations. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 30, 213–222.

    Google Scholar 

  • Bentall, R., & Slade, P. (1985). Reality testing and auditory hallucinations: A signal detection analysis. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 24, 159–169.

    Google Scholar 

  • Birchwood, M., & Chadwick, P. (1997). The omnipotence of voices: Testing the validity of a cognitive model. Psychological Medicine, 27(6), 1345–1353.

    Google Scholar 

  • Böcker, K., Hijman, R., Kahn, R., & de Haan, E. (2000). Perception, mental imagery and reality discrimination in hallucinating and non-hallucinating schizophrenic patients. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 39, 397–406.

    Google Scholar 

  • Braff, D. L. (1993). Information processing and attention dysfunctions in schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 19, 233–259.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brebion, G., Smith, M. J., & Gorman, J. M. (1996). Reality monitoring failure in schizophrenia: The role of selective attention. Schizophrenia Research, 22(2), 173–180.

    Google Scholar 

  • Brett, E. A., & Starker, S. (1977). Auditory imagery and hallucinations. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 164, 394–400.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chadwick, P., & Birchwood, M. (1994). The omnipotence of voices: I. A cognitive approach to auditory hallucinations. British Journal of Psychiatry, 164, 190–201.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chadwick, P. D., Birchwood, M. J., & Trower, P. (1996). Cognitive therapy for delusions, voices and paranoia. Oxford, England: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chapman, L., & Chapman, J. (2001). Commentary on two articles concerning generalized and specific cognitive deficits. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 110, 31–39.

    Google Scholar 

  • Close, H., & Garety, P. (1998). Cognitive assessment of voices: Further developments in understanding the emotional impact of voices. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 37, 173–188.

    Google Scholar 

  • Csipke, E., & Kinderman, P. (2002). Self talk and auditory hallucinations. Manuscript submitted for publication.

  • Delespaul, P., deVries, M., & van Os, J. (2002). Determinants of occurrence and recovery from hallucinations in daily life. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology, 37, 97–104.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ensink, B. J. (1992). Confusing realities: A study on child sexual abuse and psychiatric symptoms. Amsterdam: VU University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Feelgood, S., & Rantzen, R. (1994) Auditory and visual hallucinations in university students. Personality and Individual Differences, 17, 293–296.

    Google Scholar 

  • Franck, N., Rouby, P., Daprati, B., Dalery, J., Marie-Cardine, M., & Georgieff, N. (2000). Confusion between silent and overt reading in schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Research, 41, 357–368.

    Google Scholar 

  • Freeston, M. H., Ladouceur, R., Gagnon, F., & Thibodeau, N. (1993). Beliefs about obsessional thoughts. Journal of Psychopathology and Behavioral Assessment, 15(1), 1–21.

    Google Scholar 

  • Frith, C. (1979). Consciousness, information processing, and schizophrenia. British Journal of Psychiatry, 134, 225–235.

    Google Scholar 

  • Frith, C., & Done, J. (1989). Positive symptoms of schizophrenia. British Journal of Psychiatry, 154, 569–570.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gallagher, A., Dinan, T., & Baker, L. (1994). The effects of varying auditory input on schizophrenic hallucinations: A replication. British Journal of Medical Psychology, 67, 67–76.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gilbert, D. T., & Malone, P. S. (1995). The correspondence bias. Psychological Bulletin, 117 (1), 21–38.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gould, L. (1950). Verbal hallucinations as automatic speech. American Journal of Psychiatry, 107, 110–119.

    Google Scholar 

  • Gray, J. A., Feldon, J., Rawlins, J. N. P., Hemsley, D. R., & Smith, A. D. (1991). The neuropsychology of schizophrenia. Behavior and Brain Sciences, 14(1), 1–84.

    Google Scholar 

  • Haddock, G., Slade, P. D., Prasaad, R., & Bentall, R. (1996). Functioning of the phonological loop in auditory hallucinations. Personality and Individual Differences, 20(6), 753–760.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hampson, M., Anderson, A. W., Gore, J. C., & Hoffman, R. E. (2002, June 2–6). FMRI investigation of auditory hallucinations in schizophrenia using temporal correlations to language areas. Paper presented at the Eighth International Conference on Functional Mapping of the Human Brain, Sendai, Japan.

  • Heider, F. (1958). The Psychology of interpersonal relations. New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Hoffman, R. E. (2002). Slow transcranial magnetic stimulation, long-term depotentiation, and brain hyperexcitability disorders. American Journal of Psychiatry, 159(7), 1093–1102.

    Google Scholar 

  • Inouye, T., & Shimizu, A. (1970). The electromyographic study of verbal hallucination. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 151, 415–422.

    Google Scholar 

  • Johns, L. C., Hemsley, D., & Kuipers, E. (2002). A comparison of auditory hallucinations in a psychiatric and non-psychiatric group. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 41(1), 81–86.

    Google Scholar 

  • Johns, L. C., Nazroo, J. Y., Bebbington, P., & Kuipers, E. (2002). Occurrence of hallucinatory experiences in a community sample and ethnic variations. British Journal of Psychiatry, 180, 174–178.

    Google Scholar 

  • Johns, L. C., Rossell, S., Frith, C., Ahmad, F., Hemsley, D., Kuipers, E., & McGuire, P. K. (2001). Verbal self-monitoring and auditory verbal hallucinations in patients with schizophrenia. Psychological Medicine, 31, 705–715.

    Google Scholar 

  • Johnson, M., Hashtroudi, S., & Lindsay, D. (1993). Source monitoring. Psychological Bulletin, 114(1), 3–28.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kapur, S. (2002). Psychosis as a state of aberrant salience: A framework linking biology, phenomenology, and pharmacology in schizophrenia. American Journal of Psychiatry, 160, 13–23.

    Google Scholar 

  • Kosslyn, S. M. (1994). Image and brain: The resolution of the imagery debate. Cambridge, MA: The MIT Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Lowens, I., Haddock, G., & Bentall, R. P. (2002). Auditory hallucinations, negative automatic and intrusive thoughts: Similarities in content and process? Manuscript submitted for publication.

  • Margo, A., Hemsley, D., & Slade, P. (1981). The effects of varying auditory input on schizophrenic hallucinations. British Journal of Psychiatry, 139, 122–127.

    Google Scholar 

  • McGuigan, F. (1978). Cognitive psychophysiology: Principles of covert behavior. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.

    Google Scholar 

  • Mintz, S., & Alpert, M. (1972). Imagery vividness, reality testing and schizophrenic hallucinations. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 19, 310–316.

    Google Scholar 

  • Morrison, A. (2001). The interpretation of intrusions in psychosis: An integrative cognitive approach to hallucinations and delusions. Behavioral and Cognitive Psychotherapy, 29, 257–276.

    Google Scholar 

  • Morrison, A. P., & Baker, C. A. (2000). Intrusive thoughts and auditory hallucinations: A comparative study of intrusions in psychosis. Behavior Research and Therapy, 38(11), 1097–1107.

    Google Scholar 

  • Morrison, A., & Haddock, G. (1997). Cognitive factors in source monitoring and auditory hallucinations. Psychological Medicine, 27, 669–679.

    Google Scholar 

  • Peters, E. R., Pickering, A. D., Kent, A., Glasper, A., Irani, M., David, A. S., et al. (2000). The relationship between cognitive inhibition and psychotic symptoms. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 109(3), 386–395.

    Google Scholar 

  • Posey, T., & Losch, M. (1983). Auditory hallucinations of hearing voices in 375 normal subjects. Imagination, Cognition and Personality, 2, 99–113.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rankin, P., & O'Carroll, P. (1995). Reality monitoring and signal detection in individuals prone to hallucinations. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 34, 517–528.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rector, N. A., & Beck, A. T. (2001). Cognitive behavioral therapy for schizophrenia: An empirical review. Journal of Nervous and Mental Diseases, 189(5), 278–287.

    Google Scholar 

  • Rees, W. J. (1971). On the terms “subliminal perception” and “subception.” British Journal of Psychology, 62(4), 501–504.

    Google Scholar 

  • Romme, M., & Escher, D. (1989). Hearing voices. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 15, 209–216.

    Google Scholar 

  • Satorius, N., Jablensky, A., Korten, A., Ernberg, G., Anker, M., Cooper, J., et al. (1986). Early manifestations and first contact incidence of schizophrenia in different countries. Psychological Medicine, 16, 909–928.

    Google Scholar 

  • Schneider, K. (1959). Clinical psychopathology. New York: Grune & Stratton.

    Google Scholar 

  • Seikmeier, P. J., & Hoffman, R. E. (2002). Enhanced semantic priming in schizophrenia: A computer model based on excessive pruning of local connections in association cortex. British Journal of Psychiatry, 180, 345–350.

    Google Scholar 

  • Shergill, S. S., Cameron, L. A., & Brammer, M. J. (2001). Modality specific neural correlates of auditory and somatic hallucinations. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery and Psychiatry, 71(5), 688–690.

    Google Scholar 

  • Slade, P. D. (1976). An investigation of psychological factors involved in the predisposition to auditory hallucinations. Psychological Medicine, 6, 123–132.

    Google Scholar 

  • Slade, P., & Bentall, R. (1988). Sensory deception: A scientific analysis of hallucination. Baltimore: The Johns Hopkins University Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Starker, S., & Jolin, A. (1982). Imagery and hallucination in schizophrenic patients. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 170, 448–451.

    Google Scholar 

  • Starker, S., & Jolin, A. (1983). Occurrence and vividness of imagery in schizophrenic thought: A thought-sampling approach. Imagination, Cognition, and Personality, 3(1), 49–60.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tien, A. Y. (1991). Distributions of hallucinations in the population. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology, 26(6), 287–292.

    Google Scholar 

  • Van Os, J., & Krabbendam, L. (2002, September). Cognitive epidemiology as a tool to investigate psychological mechanisms of psychosis. Paper presented at the annual meeting of the European Association for Behavioural and Cognitive Therapies, Maastricht, The Netherlands.

  • Van Os, J., Verdoux, H., Bijl, R., & Ravelli, A. (1999). Psychosis as a continuum of variation in dimensions of psychopathology. In H. Hafner & W. Gattaz (Eds.), Search for the causes of schizophrenia (Vol.4, pp. 59–80). Berlin: Springer.

    Google Scholar 

  • Vaughan, S., & Fowler, D. (2002). The distress of the voice hearer is associated with the relationships between voice hearer and the voice. Unpublished manuscript.

  • Waters, F., Badcock, J. C., Maybery, M. T., & Michie, P. T. (in press). Inhibition in schizophrenia: Association with auditory hallucinations. Schizophrenia Research.

  • Waters, F. A. V., Badcock, J. C., Maybery, M. T., & Michie, P. T. (2002). The role of affect in auditory hallucinations of schizophrenia. Unpublished manuscript.

  • Wegner, D. M., Schneider, D. J., Carter, S. R., & White, T. L. (1987). Paradoxical effects of thought suppression. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 53, 5–13.

    Google Scholar 

  • Weinberger, D. R. (1996). On the plausibility of “the neurodevelopmental hypothesis” of schizophrenia. Neuropsychopharmacology, 14 (Suppl. 3), 1S-11S.

    Google Scholar 

  • West, D. (1948). A mass observation questionnaire on hallucinations. Journal of Social Psychiatry Research, 34, 187–196.

    Google Scholar 

  • World Health Organization. (1973). International pilot study of schizophrenia. Geneva: Author.

    Google Scholar 

  • Young, H., Bentall, R., Slade, P., & Dewey, M. (1987). The role of brief instructions and suggestibility in the elicitation of auditory and visual hallucinations in normal and psychiatric subjects. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 175, 41–48.

    Google Scholar 

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Beck, A.T., Rector, N.A. A Cognitive Model of Hallucinations. Cognitive Therapy and Research 27, 19–52 (2003). https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1022534613005

Download citation

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1022534613005

Navigation