Skip to main content
Log in

Electrophysiological Correlates of Vigilance During a Continuous Performance Test in Healthy Adults

  • Published:
Applied Psychophysiology and Biofeedback Aims and scope Submit manuscript

Abstract

The present study evaluated patterns of electrophysiological activity associated with sustained vigilance in healthy adults. Quantitative electroencephalographs (QEEG) were recorded during the performance of a Continuous Performance Test (CPT). Participants were divided into low and high vigilance groups based upon their reaction time changes between the early and late portions of the CPT. Coherence measures were calculated from the QEEG across the baseline, early CPT, and late CPT experimental conditions. Participants in the low vigilance group had higher baseline and CPT frontal to posterior coherence in the alpha and beta bands suggestive of a less vigilant state throughout the entire study. Additionally, the low vigilance group had a significantly greater beta 1 band coherence drop from baseline to the initial portion of the CPT than the high vigilance group. The combined groups had significantly lower amounts of right hemisphere frontal to posterior coherence across a number of frequency bands throughout all of the phases of the study when compared to the homologous left hemisphere sites. These interhemispheric coherence differences are consistent with vigilance network theories that implicate the right frontal and parietal lobes in the maintenance of sustained attention (M. I. Posner & M. E. Raichle, 1994).

This is a preview of subscription content, log in via an institution to check access.

Access this article

Price excludes VAT (USA)
Tax calculation will be finalised during checkout.

Instant access to the full article PDF.

Similar content being viewed by others

REFERENCES

  • American Psychiatric Association. (1994). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (4th ed.). Washington, DC: Author.

    Google Scholar 

  • Arruda, J. E., Walker, K. A., Weiler, M. D., & Valentino, D. A. (1999). Validation of a right hemisphere vigilance system as measured by principal component and factor analyzed quantitative electroencephalogram. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 32 ,119–128.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Arruda, J. E., Weiler, M. D., Valentino, D., Willis, W. G., Rossi, J. S., Stern, R. A., et al. (1996). A guide for applying principal component analysis and confirmatory factor analysis to quantitative electroencephalogram data. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 23 ,63–81.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Barkley, R. A., & Murphy, K. R. (1998). Attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder: A clinical workbook (2nd ed.). New York: Guilford.

    Google Scholar 

  • Barry, R. J., Clarke, A. R., McCarthy, R., & Selikowitz, M. (2002). EEGcoherence in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: A comparative study of two DSM-IVtypes. Clinical Neurophysiology, 113,579–585.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Berger, H. (1929). On the electroencephalogram in man. Archives of Psychiatry, 87 ,527–570.

    Google Scholar 

  • Chabot, R. J., & Serfontein, G. (1996). Quantitative electroencephalographic profiles of children with attention deficit disorder. Biological Psychiatry, 40 ,951–963.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Cohen, R. A., & O'Donnell, B. F. (1993). Models and mechanisms of attention: A summary. In R. A. Cohen (Ed.), The neuropsychology of attention (pp. 177–188). New York: Plenum.

    Google Scholar 

  • Dement, W. C., & Carskadon, M. A. (1982). Current perspectives on daytime sleepiness: The issues. Sleep, 5(Suppl. 2), S56–S66.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Dunn, L. M., & Dunn, L. M. (1997). Examiner's manual for the PPVT-III.Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service.

    Google Scholar 

  • DuPaul, G. J., Anastopoulos, A. D., Shelton, T. L., Guevremont, D. C., & Metevia, L. (1992). Multimethod assessment of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: The diagnostic utility of clinic-based tests. Journal of Clinical Child Psychology, 21 ,394–402.

    Google Scholar 

  • Egan, J. P., Greenberg, G. Z., & Schulman, A. (1961). Interval of time uncertainty in auditory detection. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 33 ,771–778.

    Google Scholar 

  • Ford, M. R., Goethe, J.W., & Dekker, D. K. (1986). EEG coherence and power in the discrimination of psychiatric disorders and medication effects. Biological Psychiatry, 21 ,1175–1188.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Gasser, T., Jennen-Steinmetz, C., & Verleger, R. (1987). EEG coherence at rest and during a visual task in two groups of children. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology, 67 ,151–158.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Harmony, T., Fernandz, T., Rodriguez, M., Reyes, A., Marosi, E., & Bernal, J. (1993). Test-retest reliability of EEG spectral parameters during cognitive tasks: II coherence. International Journal of Neuroscience, 68 ,263–271.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Jasper, H. (1958). The ten-twenty electrode system of the International Federation. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology, 10 ,371–375.

    Google Scholar 

  • Klimesch, W., Schimke, H., & Pfurtscheller, G. (1993). Alpha frequency, cognitive load and memory performance. Brain Topography, 5(3), 241–251.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Lexicor Medical Technology. (1992). NeuroLex Software for EEG—Data Acquistion and Analysis (Version 1.2) [Computer software]. Denver, CO: Author.

    Google Scholar 

  • Marosi. E., Harmony, T., Sanchez, L., Becker, J., Bernal, J., Reyes, A., et al. (1992). Maturation of the coherence of the EEG activity in normal and learning-disabled children. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology, 83 ,350–357.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Parasuraman, R. (1979). Memory load and event rate control sensitivity decrements in sustained attention. Science, 205 ,924–927.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Parasuraman, R., & Davies, D. R. (1976). Decision theory analysis of response latencies in vigilance. Journal of Experimental Psychology Humun Perception and Performance, 2 ,569–582.

    Google Scholar 

  • Parasuraman, R., Mutter, S. A., & Molloy, R. (1991). Sustained attention following mild closed-head injury. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 3(5), 789–811.

    Google Scholar 

  • Pfurtscheller, G., & Aranibar, A. (1977). Event-related cortical synchronization detected by power measurements of scalp EEG. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology, 42 ,817–826.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Posner, M. I., & Raichle, M. E. (1994). Images of mind.New York: Freeman.

    Google Scholar 

  • Riccio, C. A., Reynolds, C. R., & Lowe, P. A. (2001). Clinical applications of continuous performance tests.New York: Wiley.

    Google Scholar 

  • Sandford, J. A., & Turner, A. (1994). Integrated Visual and Auditory Continuous Performance Test manual.Richmond, VA: BrainTrain.

    Google Scholar 

  • Teixeira, L. C. H. (1993). Comparison of EEG of depressed and nondepressed college students during an attention task.Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Rhode Island, Providence.

  • Thatcher, R. W. (1999). EEG database-guided neurotherapy. In J. R. Evans & A. Abarbanel (Eds.), Introduction to quantitative EEG and neurofeedback (pp. 29–64). New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Thatcher, R. W., Krause, P. J., & Hrybyk, M. (1986). Cortico-cortical associations and EEG coherence: A two compartmental model. Electroencephalography and Clinical Neurophysiology, 64 ,123–143.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Thatcher, R.W., McAlaster, R., Lester, M. L., Horst, R. L., & Cantor, D. S. (1983). Hemispheric EEG asymmetries related to cognitive functioning in children. In A. Perecman (Ed.), Cognitive processing in the right hemisphere (pp. 125–146). New York: Academic Press.

    Google Scholar 

  • Tucker, D. M., & Roth, D. L. (1984). Factoring the coherence matrix: Patterning of the frequency-specific covariance in a multichannel EEG. Psychophysiology, 21 ,228–236.

    PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Valentino, D. A., Arruda, J. E., & Gold, S. M. (1993). Comparison of QEEG and response accuracy in good vs. poorer performers during a vigilance task. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 15 ,123–133.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Von Stein, A., Rappelsberger, P., Sarnthein, J., & Petsche, H. (1999). Synchronization between temporal and parietal cortex during multimodal object processing in man. Cerebral Cortex, 9 ,137–150.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  • Weiler, M. D. (1992). A measurement model for spectral analyzed EEG.Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Rhode Island, Providence.

  • White, J. N., Jr. (2001). Neuropsychological and electrophysiological assessment of adults with Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder .Unpublished doctoral dissertation, The University of Tennessee, Knoxville.

Download references

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Thomas S. Bearden.

Rights and permissions

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Cite this article

Bearden, T.S., Cassisi, J.E. & White, J.N. Electrophysiological Correlates of Vigilance During a Continuous Performance Test in Healthy Adults. Appl Psychophysiol Biofeedback 29, 175–188 (2004). https://doi.org/10.1023/B:APBI.0000039056.58787.76

Download citation

  • Issue Date:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1023/B:APBI.0000039056.58787.76

Navigation