Skip to main content
Original Article

A Twin Study on Humor Appreciation

The Importance of Separating Structure and Content

Published Online:https://doi.org/10.1027/1614-0001/a000136

The present study focused on genetic and environmental influences on appreciation of structure and content of humor. Monozygotic (n = 135) and dizygotic (n = 60) adult twin pairs rated their trait-like humor appreciation using the 3 WD humor test (Ruch, 1992) which assesses three basic humor stimuli (incongruity-resolution humor; nonsense humor; sexual humor), and two basic components of responses to humor (funniness; aversiveness). Additionally, two indices were derived from these scales, namely structure preference and liking of sexual content (i.e., controlled for humor structure). Intraclass correlations and behavior genetic model-fitting analyses indicated a moderate genetic effect for funniness ratings of liking the sexual content. The remaining funniness scales seemed entirely influenced by environmental effects. Aversiveness scales mainly showed environmental effects represented in reduced CE models, although twin similarity coefficients showed hints of genetic influences as well, which needs to be unraveled in future research. The results demonstrated clearly that funniness ratings should be separated for structure and content, to obtain detailed information about heritability of humor appreciation. Future research should validate these promising initial findings by utilizing larger samples.

References

  • Bouchard, T. J. (2004). Genetic influences on human psychological traits. A survey. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 13, 148–151. doi: 10.1111/j.0963-7214.2004.00295.x First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Carretero-Dios, H., & Ruch, W. (2010). Humor appreciation and sensation seeking: Invariance of findings across culture and assessment instrument? Humor, 23, 427–445. doi: 10.1515/HUMR.2010.020 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Cherkas, L., Hochberg, F., MacGregor, A. J., Snieder, H., & Spector, T. D. (2000). Happy families: A twin study of humour. Twin Research, 3, 17–22. doi: 10.1375/twin.3.1.17 First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Deckers, L. (1993). On the validity of a weight-judging paradigm for the study of humor. Humor, 6, 43–56. doi: 10.1515/humr.1993.6.1.43 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Eley, T. C., Lichtenstein, P., & Moffitt, T. E. (2003). A longitudinal behavioral genetic analysis of the etiology of aggressive and nonaggressive antisocial behavior. Development and Psychopathology, 15, 383–402. doi: 10.1017/S095457940300021X First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Eysenck, H. J. (1942). The appreciation of humor: An experimental and theoretical study. British Journal of Psychology, 32, 295–309. doi: 10.1111/j.2044-8295.1942.tb01027.x First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Eysenck, H. J. (1976). Sex and personality. London, UK: Open Books Publishing. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Kirk, K. M., Bailey, J. M., Dunne, M. P., & Martin, N. G. (2000). Measurement models for sexual orientation in a community twin sample. Behavior Genetics, 30, 345–356. doi: 10.1023/A:1026557719181 First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • McGue, M., & Bouchard, T. J. (1984). Adjustment of twin data for the effects of age and sex. Behavior Genetics, 14, 325–343. doi: 10.1007/BF01080045 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Neale, M. C., Boker, S. M., Xie, G., & Maes, H. H. (2003). Mx: Statistical modeling [computer software] (6th ed.). Richmond, VA: Virginia Institute for Psychiatric and Behavioral Genetics, Virginia Commonwealth University. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Nias, D. K. B., & Wilson, G. D. (1977). A genetic analysis of humour preferences. In A. J. Chapman, & H. C. C., (Eds.), It’s a funny thing, humour (pp. 371–373). Oxford: Pergamon Press. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Oniszczenko, W., Angleitner, A., Strelau, J., & Angert, T. (1993). The questionnaire of twins’ physical resemblance. Unpublished manuscript, University of Bielefeld, Bielefeld, Germany First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Platt, T., & Ruch, W. (2014). 3WD humor test. In S. Attardo, (Ed.), Encyclopedia of humor studies (pp. 763–765). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Ruch, W. (1992). Assessment of appreciation of humor: Studies with the 3 WD humor test. In C. D. Spielberger, & J. N. N., (Eds.), Advances in personality assessment, Vol. 9, (pp. 27–75). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Ruch, W. (2004). Humor. In C. Peterson, & M. E. P. P., (Eds.), Character strengths and virtues: A handbook and classification (pp. 583–598). Oxford, UK: University Press. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Ruch, W. (2008). The psychology of humor. In V. Raskin, (Ed.), A primer of humor (pp. 17–100). Berlin, Germany: Mouton de Gruyter. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Ruch, W., & Hehl, F.-J. (1988). Attitudes to sex, sexual behaviour and enjoyment of humour. Personality and Individual Differences, 9, 983–994. doi: 10.1016/0191-8869(88)90132-8 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Ruch, W., & Hehl, F.-J. (2007). A two-mode model of humor appreciation: Its relation to aesthetic appreciation and simplicity-complexity of personality. In W. Ruch, (Ed.), The sense of humor: Explorations of a personality characteristic (pp. 109–142). Berlin, Germany: Mouton de Gruyter. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Ruch, W., & Malcherek, J. (2009). Sensation seeking, general aesthetic preferences, and humor appreciation as predictors of liking of the grotesque. Journal of Literary Theory, 3, 333–351. First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Ruch, W., & Platt, T. (2012). Separating content and structure in humor appreciation: The need for a bimodal model and support from research into aesthetics. In A. Nijhold, (Ed.), 3rd International workshop on computational humor (pp. 23–27). Amsterdam, The Netherlands: University of Amsterdam. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Schmidt-Hidding, W. (1963). European key words: Humor and wit, Vol. 1, Munich, Germany: Huber. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Shrout, K. J., & Fleiss, J. L. (1979). Intraclass correlation: Use in assessing rater reliability. Psychological Bulletin, 86, 420–428. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.86.2.420 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar

  • Spinath, F. M., Wolf, H., Angleitner, A., Borkenau, P., & Riemann, R. (2005). Multimodal investigation of personality and cognitive ability. Results from two German twin studies BiLSAT and GOSAT. Journal for Sociology of Education and Socialization, 25, 146–161. First citation in articleGoogle Scholar

  • Turkheimer, E. (2000). Three laws of behavior genetics and what they mean. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 9, 160–164. doi: 10.1111/1467-8721.00084 First citation in articleCrossrefGoogle Scholar