Introduction

Defining the precise role of neural circuits involved in the regulation of fear expression is critical for understanding and treating fear and anxiety disorders such as post-traumatic stress disorder. Learned fear has been extensively studied using classical fear conditioning paradigm in rodents, particularly auditory fear conditioning, in which a tone (conditioned stimulus (CS)) is associated with a foot-shock (unconditioned stimulus (US)) (Fanselow and Gale, 2003; Kapp et al, 1979; LeDoux, 2000). After conditioning, the tone alone can induce freezing—an expression of fear—in conditioned animals. The conditioned fear response to a tone CS can be gradually weakened by repeated exposure to unreinforced tone without the US, a process called extinction (Pavlov, 1927).

A number of studies employing various techniques have specifically implicated the infralimbic cortex (IL) within the ventromedial prefrontal cortex (vmPFC) area in controlling fear expression after extinction in rodents and humans (Milad and Quirk, 2002, 2012; Milad et al, 2006; Phelps et al, 2004; Santini et al, 2008). Lesions targeting the vmPFC impair extinction learning and expression (Morgan et al, 1993; Quirk et al, 2000). Specifically, IL lesions within the vmPFC in rats extinguish the freezing response to tone within an extinction session, but excessive freezing spontaneously recovers the following day, suggesting failure of extinction recall (Lebron et al, 2004; Quirk et al, 2000). In vivo recordings or imaging of IL activity in rat or vmPFC activity in human have shown increased CS-related activity during fear extinction memory recall (Knapska and Maren, 2009; Milad and Quirk, 2002; Phelps et al, 2004). In addition, pharmacological inactivation of the IL before extinction training results in failure of extinction recall (Hugues et al, 2006; Laurent and Westbrook, 2009; Morgan and LeDoux, 1995; Morgan et al, 1993). It has also been suggested that the synaptic connection between the IL and amygdala is crucial for fear expression after extinction (Cho et al, 2013; Myers and Davis, 2007; Senn et al, 2014). In human studies, vmPFC activity apparently is correlated with success of extinction expression (Milad et al, 2007; Phelps et al, 2004). These findings support the hypothesis that increased activity in the IL mediates inhibition of fear responses to the tone CS after extinction.

However, analyses of the involvement of the IL in extinction recall via lesion or inactivation experiments have yielded inconsistent results. Some previous studies have reported that lesions to the vmPFC, or specifically to the IL, fail to impair extinction memory recall (Farinelli et al, 2006; Garcia et al, 2006; Gewirtz, 1997; Morgan et al, 2003). Further, inactivation of the vmPFC immediately before extinction retrieval—by infusion of the sodium channel blocker, tetrodotoxin—facilitates rather than impairs extinction (Sierra-Mercado et al, 2006). Moreover, CS-evoked firing in the IL reportedly is greater in rats that fail to acquire extinction (Chang et al, 2010). In a very recent study, optogenetic silencing of IL activity during the retrieval of extinction of auditory-conditioned fear has no effect (Do-Monte et al, 2015). The reason for these divergent results is unclear, but the differences probably arise from different experimental conditions, in particular the spatiotemporal precision of the techniques used to manipulate neuronal activity. For instance, the prelimbic cortex (PL), which is located immediately above the IL, is known to have opposite influences on fear expression and thus precise and very careful manipulation must be applied to clearly determine the role of each division of the mPFC (Sierra-Mercado et al, 2011; Vidal-Gonzalez et al, 2006). Most commonly used manipulations of neural activity, such as lesions, drug-mediated inactivation, and electrical stimulation, provide relatively poor spatiotemporal resolution. These techniques also suffer from potential non-specific side effects, such as functional compensation by other brain structures (lesions), inaccurate control of diffusion (drugs), and unwanted, non-specific stimulation of axon fibers crossing target brain regions (electrical stimulation). Therefore, despite much effort, it remains unclear whether and how vmPFC, particularly IL, controls expression of fear extinction.

To address the role of IL in fear extinction, we employed optogenetics to manipulate the activity of genetically targeted cells in the IL of mice in a temporally and spatially precise manner. This allowed us to investigate the behavioral effects of bidirectional manipulation of neuronal activity in the IL specifically at the time of retrieval on the expression of conditioned fear before and after extinction.

Materials and methods

Subjects

Hybrid male 129/C57B1/6 mice (2–3 months old, 23–35 g) were used for all experiments. Mice were group housed and maintained in a 12 h light/dark cycle at a consistent temperature (22±2 °C) at 40–60% humidity. Mice were provided food and water ad libitum throughout experiments. All procedures were approved by the Animal Ethics Committee at each institution where the experiments were performed.

Surgery

Adeno-associated virus (AAV) virus for brain surgery was prepared as described previously (Kwon et al, 2014). Five AAV viral vector constructs were used in this study (1–5 × 1011 vg/ml): AAV-hSyn-eNpHR3.0-EYFP, AAV-hSyn-EGFP, AAV-CaMKIIα-eNpHR3.0-EYFP, AAV-CaMKIIα-ChR2-Venus, and AAV-CaMKIIα-EGFP. Mice were deeply anesthetized with pentobarbital (83 mg/kg of body weight) by intraperitoneal injection and placed in a stereotaxic frame. A glass micropipette was loaded with the virus and positioned in the IL (anterior-posterior (AP)=+1.65 mm, mediolateral (ML)=±0.35 mm, dorsoventral (DV)=–3.05 mm), or PL (AP=+1.7 mm, ML=–0.35 mm, DV=–2 mm). AAV solutions were injected at a rate of 0.1 μl/min for 5 min (total, 0.5 μl) in the IL or 7 min (total, 0.7 μl) in the PL. The micropipette was left in place for an additional 10 min to allow virus diffusion and withdrawn. Subsequent experiments were conducted 2–3 weeks later to ensure sufficient time for expression of opsins in the targeted region. Afterwards, a guide cannula (Plastics One, VA, USA) was placed into the IL (AP=+1.6 mm, ML=±1.3 mm, DV=–2.3 mm), 15° angled to the midline to avoid damage to the vmPFC or PL (AP=+1.7 mm, ML=–0.35 mm, DV=–1 mm) and fixed with dental cement.

In vitro Patch-Clamp Recordings in Brain Slices

Three weeks after AAV viral injection into IL, mice were deeply anesthetized with halothane and decapitated, and their brains were quickly removed. Coronal slices (350-μm thick) were prepared in ice-cold sucrose solution and incubated in artificial cerebrospinal fluid (ACSF) at 36 °C for 30 min and at room temperature for at least 30 min before use. All of the electrophysiological recordings were performed with continuous perfusion of 32 °C oxygenated ACSF. The patch electrode (4–12 MΩ) was filled with an internal solution. Detailed information of composition of solutions and equipment for data acquisition used in this experiment are available in Supplementary Materials and Methods. Photocurrents in eNpHR3.0- or ChR2-expressing IL neurons were measured in voltage-clamp mode at a holding potential of −50 mV in response to different intensities of light delivered for 2 s. To evoke action potentials, 200 pA depolarizing currents were injected. To determine the fidelity of ChR2-evoked spikes, 4 ms light pulses of different frequencies (5, 10, 20, 30, and 40 Hz) were delivered at 4.6 mW/mm2 for 1 s.

Behavioral Procedure

Auditory fear conditioning and extinction: 7 days after recovery from cannula implantation, all mice were handled and habituated by inserting an optic fiber before behavioral experiments. Mice were submitted to three phases of behavioral procedures for extinction experiment: fear conditioning, extinction training, and extinction retrieval. For auditory fear conditioning (day 1), mice were placed in a conditioning chamber (Coulbourn Instruments, PA, USA) and allowed to explore for 2 min (pre-CS). Subsequently, a CS tone (2.8 kHz, 85 dB, 30 s) was paired with an aversive foot-shock US (0.5 mA, 2 s) three times with a variable, 100–120 s inter-stimulus interval (ISI). Each tone presentation was co-terminated with an electrical foot-shock. Mice were kept in the chamber for an additional 1 min after delivery of the last shock for monitoring of post-shock behavior. Twenty-four hours later (day 2), mice connected to the optic fiber (200 μm core; Thorlabs, NJ, USA) were placed in a novel context-shifted chamber which consisted of a plastic door covered with a vertical stripes, a white acrylic floor and semicircular cylindrical wall for extinction training. After a 2-min exploratory period, mice received repeated presentations of a tone stimulus (40 trials with 5 s ISI, 30 s for each trial) in the absence of a foot-shock. On the next day (day 3), mice underwent the same extinction training procedure. On the test day (day 4), freezing responses were measured in eight trials of tone presentation (30 s for each trial with 5 s ISI) after establishing a 2-min baseline freezing level. Whether significant long-term extinction occurred in each experiment was evaluated by comparing freezing during the first trial block of extinction on day 2 to freezing during the light-OFF period on day 4. It should be noted that this analysis is the best way for the purpose in our experimental design but may not be ideal and reflect within-session extinction during the final test. All groups in each extinction experiment showed statistically significant difference (paired Student’s t-test, data not shown), supporting that significant long-term extinction occurred under the condition we used for extinction in every experiment.

Optogenetic manipulation during extinction retrieval: on the test day, mice were placed in the same chamber as used for extinction training and their baseline level of freezing was measured for 2 min (pre-CS). Immediately thereafter, light was delivered to the target brain area during the first four trials of tone presentation (light-ON) and then was turned-off for the next four trials (light-OFF). Green light (561 nm, 63.7–127.4 mW/mm2) or blue light (473 nm, 6.4 mW/mm2) was delivered from diode laser (CL561-050 and CL473-050, respectively, Crystalaser, NV, USA) via rotary joint. For eNpHR3.0, green light was continuously delivered (including during 4 × CS and 3 × ISIs). For ChR2, blue light (10 Hz, 20-ms pulse duration) was delivered; no light was delivered during the ISI period. The freezing values for light-ON and light-OFF conditions were determined by calculating the mean percentage of freezing from four trials each.

Photoinhibition of IL without CS: The same group of mice that received extinction retrieval tests was placed back into the same camber. Two minutes after entering the chamber, mice received the same green light as above for 135 s in the absence of tone CS presentation (see Figure 1j).

Figure 1
figure 1

Photoinactivation of IL impaired expression of fear extinction with no effect on the expression of conditioned fear before extinction. (a) (left) AAV viral vector construct for expressing eNpHR3.0-EYFP under the control of the human synapsin promoter, and schematic depiction of the experimental design for selective optogenetic inhibition of IL neuronal activity by eNpHR3.0. The vertical line indicates the anterior-posterior axis of the virus injection site; sagittal and coronal planes are shown below. The cannula was implanted at a 15 degree angle to the dorsoventral axis to avoid damaging the dorsal part of the vmPFC. (right) Representative confocal microscopic image showing specific expression of eNpHR3.0 protein in the IL, visualized by EYFP fluorescence. Inset, magnified image showing membrane-targeted eNpHR3.0 protein expression. (b) Histological confirmation of virus expression and optic fiber placement (° represents the location of the fiber optic tip). Representative viral infection areas in the IL for the largest (dark gray) and smallest (light gray) area of eNpHR3.0-EYFP expression. (c) Sample photocurrents from an EYFP-positive neuron in the IL upon green light illumination. Light of different intensities induced graded changes in photocurrent amplitude. (d) Relationship between photocurrent amplitude and light intensity (2 s light exposure; n=14). (e) Spike firing recorded in an EYFP-positive neuron in the IL. Increasing light intensity led to stronger inhibition of spike firing evoked by 200 pA depolarizing current injection. (f) Relationship between inhibition of spike firing and light intensity (2 s light exposure; n=12). (g) Experimental procedure. Mice injected with either eNpHR3.0 or EGFP control AAV viral vector into the IL were trained for auditory fear conditioning and subsequently administered extinction training. Green light (561 nm) was delivered to inactivate IL activity. (h) The percentage of freezing during auditory FC and Ext. (i) The percentage of freezing during extinction retrieval. Light illumination was applied during the first half of total tone CS presentations and was turned-off during the remaining half. Mice from the eNpHR3.0 group showed greater freezing behavior compared with the control group during the light-ON period and this difference disappeared when lights were turned-off (light-OFF) (n=9 for each group). *p<0.05 (post hoc comparison). (j) The percentage of freezing exhibited in the absence of tone with (light-ON) or without (light-OFF) light illumination of the IL. There was no significant group difference in freezing (n=10 for each group). (k) Experimental procedure. Mice injected with eNpHR3.0 into the IL were trained for auditory fear conditioning with a single pairing of tone and shock, and tested 24 h later. Freezing was determined in the presence of tone for 2 min. Light with a wavelength of 561 nm was delivered during the first 1 min of tone presentation and then turned-off for the next 1 min. The percentage of freezing during fear memory test with (light-ON) or without (light-OFF) photoinhibition of the IL. Mice from the two groups showed a similar level of freezing in response to tone presentation regardless of light illumination (EGFP, n=5; eNpHR3.0, n=7). Data in d, f, h–k are expressed as means±SEM. x, baseline (pre-CS) freezing levels. AAV, adeno-associated virus; CS, conditioned stimulus; EGFP, enhanced green fluorescent protein; EYFP, enhanced yellow fluorescent protein; Ext, extinction training; FC, fear conditioning; IL, infralimbic cortex; PL, prelimbic cortex; vmPFC, ventromedial prefrontal cortex.

PowerPoint slide

Optogenetic manipulation during fear memory expression: Mice were fear conditioned as described above, but with a single pairing of CS and US. Fear memory was tested in the context-shifted chamber 24 h later. After a 2-min exploratory period (pre-CS), tone was presented for 2 min to determine fear memory expression. During the first 1 min of tone presentation, either green (continuous) or blue (10 Hz, 20 ms duration) light was delivered to the target brain area (light-ON). During the next 1 min of tone presentation, freezing was measured in the absence of light illumination (light-OFF). For unilateral (20 Hz, 10 ms duration) and bilateral (10 Hz) photoactivation experiments, mice received three pairings of tone and shock presentations to avoid floor effect. Twenty-four hours later, mice were placed in a context-shifted chamber. After 2 min of baseline recording (pre-CS), CS was presented with unilateral 20 Hz or bilateral 10 Hz light stimulation (four trials with 5 s ISI, 30 s for each trial). Light stimulation was delivered either with or without a 0.1 s delay after tone onset. For photoactivation during contextual fear retrieval, mice received fear conditioning and extinction training as described above (days 1–3) and on the test day (day 4), they were re-introduced to the conditioned chamber for 2 min with 10 Hz light stimulation during first minute.

Mouse behavior was recorded using FreezeFrame software (Actimetrics, IL, USA). Freezing level during fear conditioning was determined by automatic scoring using FreezeFrame software, while freezing for all other cases was manually scored in a blinded manner by measuring the time animals spent in freezing.

Histology

At the end of behavioral experiments, all the mice were killed and brain sections were prepared as described previously (Kwon et al, 2014). For histological verification of virus expression and fiber optic placement, fluorescence images of coronal sections were captured with a fluorescence microscope (ECLIPSE 80i, Nikon). To confirm expression in the IL region, the extent of expression was measured with scale bars (NIS Elements Software, Nikon) by reference to the Mouse Brain Atlas (Franklin and Paxinos, 2008). Only mice that showed highly restricted expression of targeted opsin in the IL region were included in data analyses. Animals that were excluded typically showed faint expression in the IL, off-target expression in the PL or dorsal tenia tecta, or physical damage in the IL caused by the guide cannula. Histological verifications were conducted in a blinded manner.

Statistical Analysis

Statistical significance of data was determined using Student’s t-test or two-way repeated-measures analysis of variance (ANOVA) followed by Bonferroni post hoc test for multiple comparisons. A significance level of p<0.05 was used for all analyses. Prism (version 6.04, GraphPad Software) was used for all statistical analyses.

Results

Optogenetic Inhibition of Neuronal Activity in the IL

We employed an AAV system in which expression of genes for third-generation halorhodopsin (eNpHR3.0), a light-activated chloride pump fused to enhanced yellow fluorescent protein (EYFP), or enhanced green fluorescent protein (EGFP; control) was driven by the neuron-specific human synapsin (hSyn) promoter. AAV-eNpHR3.0 was delivered into the right IL by unilateral, stereotaxic injection, followed by chronic implantation of cannulae, angled 15° toward the midline to minimize damage to the dorsal part of the mPFC (Figure 1a). We optimized the injection volume so that the virus-infected region covered the IL, but with little or no unwanted infection in surrounding areas. Virus infections were sometimes observed in the dorsal peduncular cortex (DP), particularly when expression was relatively strong. Because the DP, as part of the vmPFC, is thought to either contribute to extinction learning in a manner similar to that of the IL or to have no effect (LaLumiere et al, 2010; Peters et al, 2009; Van den Oever et al, 2013; Willcocks and McNally, 2013), we also included these animals in our data analysis. In addition, after behavioral experiments we checked the size of the virus-infected areas in each mouse and included in our final analysis only mice that displayed highly restricted expression of eNpHR3.0 in the IL (Figure 1a and b). Next, we determined the proportion of glutamatergic or GABAergic neurons expressing NpHR driven by the hSyn promoter. This was done via immunohistochemistry, using antibodies against CaMKIIα or GAD67. The co-localization analysis showed that most cells expressing NpHR were CaMKIIα-positive excitatory neurons (69.2±3.9% of NpHR-positive cells), while ~24% of NpHR-positive cells were GABAergic interneurons (23.8±4.0%; Supplementary Figure 1). Thus, both excitatory and inhibitory neurons could be photoinhibited, though excitatory neurons were three times more abundant. To verify the physiological effects of eNpHR3.0 on IL neuronal activity, we performed whole-cell patch-clamp recordings in brain slices. We first examined how eNpHR3.0-mediated photocurrents varied with light intensity. Illumination with 540±10 nm wavelength light evoked robust hyperpolarizing currents in recorded neurons that showed a graded increased in size with increases in light intensity (Figure 1c and d). Next, we measured the relationship between light intensity and spike firing inhibition ratio. Under conditions in which spike firing was evoked by injection of a depolarizing current (200 pA), light at an intensity of 0.07 mW/mm2 inhibited spike firing by ~50% (Figure 1e; 46.5±14.7%), whereas a 10-fold increase in light intensity (0.7 mW/mm2) almost completely suppressed spike firing (Figure 1e; 87.4±9.3%). The amount of inhibition was slightly increased with a further increase in light intensity, causing an inhibition of 92.0±8.0% at a maximum light intensity of 7.7 mW/mm2 (Figure 1f). Thus, illumination evokes hyperpolarization in NpHR3.0-expressing IL neurons and effectively inhibits spike firing in these neurons in a temporally precise manner.

Photoinhibition of IL Neuronal Activity Impairs the Expression of Fear Extinction

To determine the effect of IL photoinhibition on the expression of extinction memory, we first confirmed that our protocol reliably formed long-term memory of fear extinction. Mice injected with the control EGFP virus were trained for auditory fear conditioning and extinction. During extinction training, freezing gradually decreased by days 2 and 3 of the extinction sessions (Supplementary Figure 2a). On the test day (day 4), freezing in the extinction group (39.6%) was significantly lower than in the no-extinction group (66.4%; p<0.5, Student’s t-test; Supplementary Figure 2a). Additionally, CS-induced freezing in the extinction group (39.6%) was significantly diminished compared with initial conditioned freezing (62.5%), measured before extinction in the first trial block on day 2 (p<0.01, paired Student’s t-test); however, this was not the case for mice in the no-extinction group (66.4 and 62.5%; p=0.7529, Student’s t-test; Supplementary Figure 2b). Thus, these results confirm that the behavioral protocol efficiently induces long-term fear extinction.

Next, we tested whether IL activity is necessary for the expression of extinction memory using optogenetic inhibition. Virus-injected mice were subjected to fear conditioning, extinction training and testing (Figure 1g). Notably, there were no significant differences between EGFP control and eNpHR3.0 groups in initial conditioned freezing (p=0.49, Student’s t-test) or in the extinction ratio during training (Figure 1h). Two-way repeated-measures ANOVA revealed a significant effect of trial block (Ext1: F9,144=6.139, p<0.0001, Ext2: F9,144=3.323, p=0.001) with no significant group difference (Ext1: F1,16=0.01911, p=0.8918, Ext2: F1,16=0.04034, p=0.8434; Figure 1h). During extinction retrieval testing, pre-CS freezing levels did not differ between groups (14 and 17.1% for EGFP and eNpHR3.0, respectively; p=0.586, Student’s t-test; Figure 1i). However, upon presentation of a tone combined with green light (561 nm) illumination, freezing was significantly higher in eNpHR3.0 mice than in control mice (Figure 1i; p<0.05, Student’s t-test), indicating that the expression of fear extinction memory was impaired by photoinhibition of IL activity at the time of retrieval. This impairment was reversible and specific: freezing returned to control levels in eNpHR3.0 mice in the absence of photoinhibition during the light-OFF phase (35 and 36.5% for EGFP and eNpHR3.0, respectively; p=0.7802, Student’s t-test; Figure 1i). Two-way repeated-measures ANOVA confirmed a significant group difference in freezing during the light-ON period (group × trial interaction, F1,16=8.818, p<0.01; Bonferroni post hoc test, p<0.05). To further confirm the specificity of the behavioral effects, we also analyzed data from animals (n=5) that were excluded due to no or low viral infection, or because of off-target expression. These mice showed a trend toward a low level of freezing during the light-ON period (19.8±7.7%), similar to the control group. This result further supports the conclusion that impairment of extinction expression was specific to optogenetic inhibition of IL neuron activity.

To ask whether selective silencing of glutamatergic neurons could produce a similar effect, we next injected mice with either AAV-eNpHR3.0-EYFP or control AAV-EGFP virus with CaMKIIα promoter regulating transgene expression. These viruses were injected into the right IL and mice were then trained for auditory fear conditioning and extinction (Supplementary Figure 3a–c). These animals showed significant fear extinction during training (Ext1: F9,135=12.37, p<0.0001, Ext2: F9,135=6.259, p<0.0001) with no significant group difference (Ext1: F1,15=3.083, p=0.0995, Ext2: F1,15=3.129, p=0.0972; Supplementary Figure 3c). Unlike the case when eNpHR3.0 expression was controlled by the hSyn promoter, photoinhibition of IL had no effect on expression of fear extinction (group × trial interaction, F1,15=1.666, p=0.2163, two-way repeated-measures ANOVA; Supplementary Figure 3d). Hence, this result indicates that impairment of extinction retrieval was specific to silencing activities of both glutamatergic and GABAergic neurons, targeted by the hSyn promoter.

Although unlikely, it is possible that IL photoinhibition may simply induce freezing even in the absence of CS. To test this possibility, we re-introduced the same mice from each group into the test chamber 1 day after extinction retrieval tests. The green light was again delivered to IL, but this time without the CS tone (Figure 1j). We found no significant induction of freezing compared with baseline in either eNpHR3.0 or control groups under these conditions (group × trial interaction, F1,18=0.6643, p=0.4257, two-way repeated-measures ANOVA; Figure 1j), thus excluding the possibility that photoinhibition of IL neuronal activity directly induced freezing. Taken together, these results indicate that activity of both excitatory and inhibitory neurons in IL is necessary for the expression of fear extinction memory.

No Effect of IL Photoinhibition on Conditioned Fear Before Extinction

It has been suggested that the IL is engaged for fear inhibition after extinction (Sotres-Bayon and Quirk, 2010). We tested this idea by examining the effects of IL photoinhibition on fear memory expression before extinction (Figure 1k). We observed no significant differences in freezing between eNpHR3.0 and control groups during light-ON or light-OFF periods (group × trial interaction, F1,10=0.008, p=0.9327, two-way repeated-measures ANOVA), indicating no significant effect of IL inactivation on the expression of conditioned fear that did not undergo extinction (Figure 1k). Thus, IL activity is specifically engaged for the expression of fear extinction, but not of fear memory per se. This is consistent with previous findings (Sierra-Mercado et al, 2011) and also shows that IL photoinhibition does not simply enhance CS-evoked freezing.

Inhibition of PL Activity Does Not Affect Extinction Memory Retrieval

A recent study reported that neuronal activity in the PL area, in addition to the IL, is increased during extinction memory retrieval (Chang et al, 2010). This suggests a possible role for the PL in extinction recall. To test this possibility, we injected AAV-eNpHR3.0 or AAV-EGFP viral vector specifically into the PL area of the vmPFC and mice were then trained for auditory fear conditioning followed by extinction training as before (Figure 2a–c). During extinction retrieval tests, mice in both groups displayed normal expression of fear extinction with no significant difference in freezing between groups. Two-way repeated-measures ANOVA showed a significant effect of interaction (group × trial interaction, F1,16=7.195, p=0.0164) but no main effect of group (F1,16=0.01264, p=0.9119). Bonferroni post hoc tests revealed no significant difference in freezing between eNpHR3.0 and control groups under light-ON (p=0.406) or light-OFF (p=0.2218) conditions during the test (Figure 2d). These results indicate that PL activity is not critical for the expression of extinction memory and further validate the selective role of the IL in extinction memory retrieval.

Figure 2
figure 2

Photoinactivation of PL did not affect expression of fear extinction. (a) (top) Schematic depiction of the experimental design for AAV virus-mediated eNpHR3.0 expression in the PL. (bottom) Representative confocal microscopic image showing expression of eNpHR3.0 protein fused with EYFP in the PL (green). Inset, magnified image of eNpHR3.0-EYFP–expressing neurons in the PL. (b) Experimental procedure. Mice injected with either eNpHR3.0 or control EGFP AAV viral vector into the PL were trained for auditory fear conditioning and subsequently administered extinction training. Green light (561 nm) was delivered as before to inactivate the PL during the first half of total tone presentations. (c) The percentage of freezing during auditory FC and Ext procedure. (d) The percentage of freezing during extinction retrieval. Mice from eNpHR3.0 and control EGFP groups displayed no significant differences in freezing throughout tone presentations, even with light illumination (n=9 for each group). Data in c, d are expressed as means±SEM. x, baseline (pre-CS) freezing levels. AAV, adeno-associated virus; CS, conditioned stimulus; EGFP, enhanced green fluorescent protein; EYFP, enhanced yellow fluorescent protein; Ext, extinction training; FC, fear conditioning; IL, infralimbic cortex; PL, prelimbic cortex.

PowerPoint slide

Optogenetic Activation of Excitatory Neurons in the IL

Next, we asked whether IL activation is sufficient to inhibit fear expression. For this purpose we photostimulated IL activity by expressing the light-activated cation channel, channelrhodopsin-2 (ChR2), in excitatory neurons. To selectively target excitatory neurons in the IL, we employed an AAV-ChR2-Venus viral vector with the CaMKIIα promoter (Benson et al, 1992). Immunohistochemical analyses confirmed selective expression of ChR2 in CaMKIIα-positive neurons in the IL (Figure 3a and b). To examine the ability of ChR2 to photostimulate IL neurons, we performed whole-cell patch-clamp recordings in brain slices. Large inward photocurrents were induced in ChR2-expressing neurons exposed to blue (470–495 nm) light. Increasing light intensity increased photocurrent amplitude, with a maximal light intensity of 4.6 mW/mm2 evoking peak photocurrents of 273±56 pA (Figure 3c and d). In current clamp recordings, illumination evoked action potentials in IL neurons (Figure 3e). Action potentials were produced by light with high probability at relatively low frequencies (5 or 10 Hz), while the probability of photostimulation gradually declined at higher frequencies up to 40 Hz (Figure 3f). Thus, ChR2-mediated photostimulation elicits high-fidelity activation of IL neurons.

Figure 3
figure 3

Photoactivation of IL excitatory neurons enhanced expression of fear extinction. (a) Left: AAV vector construct for expressing ChR2-Venus in excitatory neurons in the IL, and schematic depiction of the experimental design for photoactivation of the IL. Mice were injected with AAV-CaMKIIα-ChR2-Venus and implanted with a guide cannula targeting the IL at a 15-degree angle to the dorsoventral axis. Right: Representative confocal microscopic image showing the expression pattern of Venus-tagged ChR2 (green). Inset, magnified image of ChR2-expressing cells in the IL. (b) Representative confocal microscopic images showing selective expression of ChR2-Venus in CaMKIIα-positive neurons in the IL. (c) Sample photocurrent evoked in a Venus-positive neuron in the IL upon blue light illumination. Light of different intensities induced graded changes in photocurrents. (d) Relationship between photocurrent amplitude (steady state) and light intensity (2 s duration; n=13). (e) Action potential firing (black line) evoked by illumination (blue traces) of a ChR2-Venus–expressing excitatory neuron in the IL at different light pulse frequencies (4.6 mW/mm2, 4 ms duration). (f) Mean probability of evoking action potentials at different light frequencies. Probability decreased with increasing light pulse frequency (n=12). (g) Experimental procedure. Mice injected with either ChR2-Venus or EGFP vector into the IL were trained for fear conditioning and subsequently administered extinction training. During extinction memory tests, tone-induced freezing was determined with and without blue light (473 nm) illumination at a frequency of 10 Hz. (h) The percentage of freezing during auditory FC and Ext procedure. (i) The percentage of freezing during extinction memory tests. Mice injected with ChR2-Venus exhibited significantly reduced freezing to the tone CS compared with EGFP control mice following photoactivation of IL excitatory neurons (light-ON; n=9 for each group). **P<0.01 (post hoc comparison). Data in d, f, h, i are expressed as means±SEM. x, baseline (pre-CS) freezing levels. AAV, adeno-associated virus; CS, conditioned stimulus; Ext, extinction training; FC, fear conditioning; IL, infralimbic cortex; PL, prelimbic cortex.

PowerPoint slide

Photostimulation of IL Excitatory Neurons Enhances Expression of Fear Extinction

If IL activation controls the inhibition of fear expression, then increasing IL activity should enhance the expression of fear extinction. To test this idea, we trained mice in which virus expressing either ChR2-Venus or EGFP under the CaMKII promoter was injected into the right IL. These mice were then subjected to auditory fear conditioning and subsequent extinction, as before (Figure 3g). Importantly, animals in control and ChR2 groups both showed similar levels of conditioned freezing (p=0.6014, Student’s t-test) and extinction during training (Figure 3h). Two-way repeated-measures ANOVA revealed a significant effect of trial block (Ext1: F9,144=7.063, p<0.0001, Ext2: F9,144=5.472, p<0.0001) with no significant group difference (Ext1: F1,16=1.163, p=0.2968, Ext2: F1,16=0.8747, p=0.3636; Figure 3h). In extinction retrieval tests, photostimulation via pulses of blue light (10 Hz, 20 ms duration), which evoked action potential firing with high temporal precision and spike fidelity (97.7±2.3%; Supplementary Figure 4), was delivered to the IL during the first four trial blocks of the CS presentation. Tone-induced freezing in ChR2-expressing mice was robustly reduced compared with that in control mice during the light-ON period (significant group × trial interaction, F1,16=15.54, p<0.01, two-way repeated-measures ANOVA; p<0.01, Bonferroni post hoc test; Figure 3i). Notably, this reduced freezing in the ChR2 group was similar to baseline pre-CS freezing measured before tone presentation, indicating almost complete block of conditioned freezing (significant group × trial interaction, F1,16=9.599, p<0.01, two-way repeated-measures ANOVA; p=0.1569, Bonferroni post hoc test; Figure 3i). The enhanced expression of fear extinction induced by IL photoactivation was reversible and specific, as indicated by return of freezing behavior to control levels once the light was turned-off (39.5 and 41.2% for EGFP and ChR2, respectively; Figure 3i). Taken together, these results reveal that increasing the activity of IL excitatory neurons at the time of extinction retrieval is sufficient to enhance expression of fear extinction. These results, in conjunction with our observations of opposite effects produced by photoinhibiting IL, indicate that IL activity plays a causal role in controlling fear expression after extinction.

Photostimulation of IL Excitatory Neurons Does Not Affect Expression of Conditioned Fear Before Extinction

If CS-induced activation of IL is a critical mechanism for fear inhibition after extinction, artificially activating IL neurons alone may be sufficient to induce extinction-like inhibition of conditioned fear expression even without extinction training. In fact, previous studies reported that electrical stimulation of the medial prefrontal cortex, or specifically the IL, reduces conditioned freezing in animals that do not undergo extinction (Milad and Quirk, 2002; Milad et al, 2004; Vidal-Gonzalez et al, 2006). However, because uncertainties remain because of potential non-specific effects of electrical stimulation, we took advantage of ChR2-mediated photostimulation to test this prediction. During the fear memory test, ChR2-expressing neurons in the IL were photostimulated using the same conditions as before and were paired with tone presentation (Figure 4a). In contrast to previous findings, we found that conditioned freezing in ChR2 mice was not significantly altered by IL photoactivation compared with control mice (p=0.50, Student’s t-test; Figure 4b).

Figure 4
figure 4

Photoactivation of IL did not affect the expression of conditioned fear without extinction. (a) Experimental procedure. Mice injected with either ChR2-Venus or control EGFP into the IL were trained for auditory fear conditioning with single pairing of tone and shock, and then tested 24 h later. During tone presentation, a 10- Hz light stimulation was delivered to the IL simultaneously with tone onset. (b) The percent of freezing during fear memory tests. There was no significant group difference in freezing between EGFP (n=8) and ChR2 (n=9) groups. (c) Experimental procedure. Mice injected with either ChR2-Venus or control EGFP into the IL were trained for auditory fear conditioning with three pairings of tone and shock, and then tested 24 h later. Three different photoactivation conditions were used: 20 Hz with no delay after tone onset (EGFP, n=7; ChR2, n=8) (d), 20 Hz with a 0.1 s delay (EGFP, n=10; ChR2, n=7) (e), and a 10- Hz bilateral IL stimulation with no delay (EGFP, n=11; ChR2, n=8) (f). (d, e, f) The percentage of freezing during tone CS presentation is shown. There was no significant difference in freezing between ChR2-Venus and EGFP control mice. Data in b, d–f are expressed as means±SEM. CS, conditioned stimulus; EGFP, enhanced green fluorescent protein.

PowerPoint slide

It is possible that the light-stimulation conditions used (10 Hz, 20-ms pulse duration) were too weak or physiologically irrelevant to induce behavioral changes under no-extinction conditions. Indeed, it has been reported that IL activity is increased up to a maximal rate of 20 Hz in the first 100–400 ms after tone onset during extinction recall (Milad and Quirk, 2002), and electrical stimulation is effective in reducing conditioned freezing in a temporally specific manner (Milad et al, 2004). Based on these findings, we next tested photostimulation at 20 Hz (10 ms pulse duration), with or without a 0.1 s delay after tone onset. Patch-clamp recordings in brain slices confirmed that reliable spike firing (87.6±6.1%) was evoked by such stimuli (Supplementary figure 4). Again, we found no significant group difference in freezing without a delay (p=0.2815, Student’s t-test; Figure 4c and d) or with it (p=0.1385, Student’s t-test; Figure 4e), although ChR2 mice showed slightly reduced freezing, compared to control mice, upon IL photoactivation (Figure 4d and e). Finally, we tested bilateral stimulation of the IL. We observed no significant difference in freezing between control and ChR2 groups (p=0.60, Student’s t-test; Figure 4f), indicating no effect of bilateral IL activation on conditioned fear expression. Therefore, these results show that activation of IL excitatory neurons before extinction does not affect expression of conditioned fear, suggesting that IL activation alone is not sufficient to suppress the expression of conditioned fear.

Given the lack of effect of IL stimulation on non-extinguished conditioned fear, we next asked whether the effect of IL stimulation after extinction of auditory fear memory is specific to that fear memory. To test this idea, we investigated the effect of IL photostimulation on the expression of contextual fear that did not undergo extinction after extinction training of auditory fear. After auditory fear conditioning and extinction training to the tone CS as before, mice were re-introduced to the conditioned context where they received shock during fear conditioning (Figure 5a and b). There were no significant differences in freezing between control and ChR2 groups during testing in the light-ON or light-OFF periods (group × trial interaction, F1,13=0.95, p=0.35, two-way repeated-measures ANOVA; Figure 5c). Therefore, our results show that IL stimulation effect is specific to the fear memory that undergoes extinction.

Figure 5
figure 5

Photoactivation of IL did not affect the expression of contextual fear after extinction of auditory-conditioned fear. (a) Experimental procedure. Mice injected with either ChR2 or control EGFP AAV viral vector into the IL were trained for auditory fear conditioning and subsequently administered extinction training. Blue light (473 nm) was delivered as before to activate the IL during the first minute of 2- min contextual fear memory test. Stripped rectangle represents conditioning context, white dome represents extinction context. (b) The percentage of freezing during auditory FC and Ext. (c) The percentage of freezing during contextual fear memory tests. Mice injected with either ChR2-Venus (n=6) or control EGFP (n=9) into the IL did not show significant difference in freezing. Data in b, c are expressed as means±SEM. x, baseline (pre-CS) freezing levels. AAV, adeno-associated virus; CS, conditioned stimulus; EGFP, enhanced green fluorescent protein; Ext, extinction training; FC, fear conditioning; IL, infralimbic cortex.

PowerPoint slide

Discussion

We used optogenetics to determine that precise, real-time inhibition of activity in IL, but not in PL, impairs the expression of fear extinction. Further, activation of excitatory neurons in the IL enhances expression of fear extinction, while neither decreasing or increasing IL activity significantly affects the expression of conditioned fear. Therefore, we conclude that IL activity acts as an important top-down control for fear expression after extinction that is necessary and sufficient to inhibit expression of conditioned fear after extinction.

Because there are no known genetic targeting approaches specific to the IL, we relied on spatially precise injections of virus for the delivery of opsins (eNpHR3.0 or ChR2) specifically to the IL. For highly restricted expression of eNpHR3.0 or ChR2 in the IL with minimal damage to other vmPFC areas, we optimized virus delivery conditions, including target coordinates, virus injection volume, and virus titer. Virus expression was predominantly restricted to neurons in the IL in most animals included in our data analysis, although infections were occasionally observed in the ventral part of the PL and DP. Thus, our optogenetic manipulations were highly specific to cells in the IL, although possible contributions of other vmPFC regions, such as the PL and DP, cannot be completely ruled out.

Our current findings are in line with the ‘IL hypothesis’, which posits that the IL plays a critical role in the control of fear expression after extinction (Herry et al, 2010; Quirk et al, 2006; Quirk and Mueller, 2008; Sotres-Bayon et al, 2004). Among the previous evidence in support of the IL hypothesis, perhaps the best is the demonstration that the CS-evoked response of IL neurons in vivo is increased during recall of extinction memory (Milad and Quirk, 2002). This observation suggests that IL activity controls fear expression after extinction, so that activation of IL by the CS mediates expression of fear extinction. However, this causal link has remained elusive, mainly because techniques capable of providing the necessary spatiotemporal resolution for control of IL activity have been lacking. In addition, most previous studies have focused largely on extinction acquisition or consolidation processes and not specifically on the expression of fear extinction. Accordingly, in most cases functional manipulations such as lesions, drug treatment, or electrical stimulation were performed before fear conditioning or during extinction training (Milad and Quirk, 2002; Milad et al, 2004; Morgan and LeDoux, 1995; Morgan et al, 1993; Quirk et al, 2000). In a few studies, lesions or pharmacological interventions were applied after extinction training to examine effects on extinction retrieval, but such manipulations were targeted to the mPFC generally rather than specifically to the IL. Moreover, some of these studies failed to support a role for the IL in extinction recall. For example, one study showed that lesions made in the vmPFC 1 day after extinction training had no effect on normal expression of extinction memory (Garcia et al, 2006). In another study, inactivation of the vmPFC by the sodium channel blocker tetrodotoxin, infused 30 min before the extinction recall test, actually reduced freezing (Sierra-Mercado et al, 2006). These findings are inconsistent with our data. One possible explanation for these discrepancies is that lesion- or drug-mediated inactivation of the vmPFC encompassing both the IL and PL produces compound effects. Differences in experimental conditions, such as temporal relationships, may also be important contributors. For example, the lesion study employed a 7-day interval between extinction training and testing (Garcia et al, 2006), whereas we tested animals 1 day after extinction training. Interpretation of data may also be further complicated by undesired effects produced by the relatively poor spatial and temporal precision of drug application and potential compensation by other brain structures in lesion or drug-infusion protocols, as shown in a recent study (Goshen et al, 2011). Another recent study reported that optogenetic silencing of IL activity has no effect on retrieval of extinction of auditory-conditioned fear (Do-Monte et al, 2015), which is inconsistent with our conclusion. One of the major differences between the two studies is that we used the hSyn promoter to express eNpHR3.0 to both glutamatergic and GABAergic neurons, while Do-Monte et al. (2015) used the CaMKIIα promoter to selectively silence glutamatergic neurons. Because we also found no significant effect of IL photoinhibition when NpHR expression was driven by the CaMKIIα promoter under our conditions, we presume that GABAergic neurons in the IL might also critically participate in regulating extinction retrieval. Notwithstanding, our positive data support the idea that IL activity is essential for expression of fear extinction at the time of retrieval. Our current hypothesis to explain our results is that both CaMKIIα-positive excitatory neurons and GABAergic interneurons in the IL contribute to expression of fear extinction memory. We hypothesize that there might be specific types of GABAergic interneurons in the IL that may regulate the activity of PL outputs to basolateral amygdala (BLA) through short-range projections to PL. Indeed, anatomical tracing provides evidence for reciprocal projections between PL and IL, although cell types have not yet been identified (Hoover and Vertes, 2007). A recent paper reports GABAergic neurons in the mPFC that project to several subcortical areas including BLA (Lee et al, 2014). Hence, it is also possible that specific types of GABAergic interneurons in the IL may directly project to BLA and inhibit freezing when activated.

Given previous reports showing that IL activation—via electrical, pharmacological or optogenetic stimulation—reduces conditioned freezing, even in animals that did not undergo extinction training (Do-Monte et al, 2015; Milad and Quirk, 2002; Milad et al, 2004; Thompson et al, 2010; Vidal-Gonzalez et al, 2006), we anticipated that IL photostimulation might produce similar effects. However, we unexpectedly found that IL photoactivation did not affect the expression of conditioned freezing without extinction (Figure 4). We tested different light stimulation frequencies, with or without a delay after tone onset, as well as bilateral stimulation. But none of the conditions that we tested produced a significant reduction in conditioned freezing, even though they did have other behavioral effects. Unlike electrical or pharmacological stimulation, the optogenetic photostimulation paradigm used here was specific to CaMKIIα-positive neurons in the IL. Thus, it is possible that stimulation of a larger population of cells encompassing different types of cells within the IL may be necessary to inhibit conditioned freezing. However, this is less likely given a recent optogenetic study in rat showing that the same optical activation of CaMKIIα-positive neurons induced a strong reduction in conditioned freezing (Do-Monte et al, 2015). Alternatively, it is possible that photostimulation was weaker in our conditions, so that an insufficient number of neurons were activated to produce a behavioral outcome. However, this seems unlikely because the same photostimuli robustly reduced conditioned freezing after extinction. Thus, although it is unclear how to explain the discrepancy, our data strongly indicate that an increase in IL activity alone is insufficient, or at least is less efficient, for inhibiting fear output in the absence of extinction. Importantly, the expression of contextual fear was not affected by IL photostimulation after extinction of auditory fear. This result further supports the conclusion that control of fear expression by IL is specific to the fear memory that undergoes extinction. Taken together, our data support the hypothesis that CS-evoked activity in the IL is a key circuit mechanism in the top-down control of fear expression after extinction, but imply that plastic changes in broader neural networks involved in fear and fear extinction are also required for the expression of fear extinction (Amano et al, 2010; Cho et al, 2013; Rosenkranz et al, 2003; Senn et al, 2014; Sierra-Mercado et al, 2011; Trouche et al, 2013).

IL activity correlates with a decrement in freezing behavior during retrieval of extinction (Milad and Quirk, 2002; Milad et al, 2004; Vidal-Gonzalez et al, 2006). Consistent with this, we observed that photostimulation-induced enhancement of IL activity after extinction elicited a further reduction in fear expression to pre-CS levels (Figure 3i). This suggests that extinction training itself may not induce full activation of the IL. Thus, modulation of the intrinsic excitability of the IL excitatory neurons or activity of local inhibitory networks within IL may provide another regulation point for the fear extinction (Herry et al, 1999; Senn et al, 2014). How the intrinsic plasticity of IL neurons is regulated by the internal state of the brain or by experience will be an important issue to consider in future studies.

In summary, by bidirectionally manipulating IL activity with precise spatiotemporal resolution using optogenetics during extinction memory retrieval, our study overcomes the limitations of previous research and provides compelling evidence for a causal role for IL activity in controlling the expression of fear extinction. The level of IL activity determines fear output expressed to the CS after extinction, providing important insights into how vmPFC controls fear expression as a top-down mechanism in extinction.

Funding and disclosure

The authors declare no conflict of interest.