Introduction

The dopamine (DA) system has been implicated in a variety of arousal-related processes including the regulation of motor activity, learning, motivated behavior, psychostimulant abuse, and sleep/wake activity. Although numerous observations highlight the importance of DA neuron activity in regulating striatal DA levels [1, 2], recent advances highlight the influence of the axonal dopamine transporter (DAT) in controlling the intensity and duration of DA actions in the striatum [3,4,5,6,7,8].

The DAT is commonly regarded as a homeostatic regulator that governs extracellular DA in response to acute and long-term physiological demands. Increases in DAT function and phosphorylation have been observed in attention deficit hyperactivity disorder and substance use disorders while decreases in DAT binding has been observed in Parkinson’s disease, presumably to compensate for sustained dysregulation of DA neurotransmission [5, 9,10,11]. High fat diet and stress significantly decrease DAT function over extended periods of time [12,13,14]. In addition to these long-term alterations in function, DATs can also undergo rapid changes in response to environmental and extracellular challenges [15,16,17]. DAT substrates, blockers, and presynaptic receptor ligands, for example, can rapidly regulate DAT function by altering either cell surface expression or posttranslational modifications of the transporter [12, 18,19,20,21,22]. Together, these observations suggest that DATs behave in a malleable fashion on both long and short time scales to tightly control DA’s actions [23].

We previously demonstrated in rodents that DA uptake in the striatum fluctuates in a diurnal manner across the light/dark cycle with the least efficient uptake occurring in the dark phase and most efficient uptake during the light phase [24]. Importantly, these DAT fluctuations were inversely related to extracellular DA levels which could not be accounted for by variations in DA metabolism, tyrosine hydroxylase (TH) activity, or DA D2 receptor (D2R)/D3 receptor (D3R) function, suggesting that extracellular DA cycling was governed by fluctuations in rates of DA uptake [24].

Upon initial consideration, these prior observations could appear to suggest that DAT function may be tracking time of day and/or light/dark conditions. However, the 24 h light/dark cycle is comprised of recurring sequences of distinct sleep/wake states that integrate as greater time spent asleep during the light phase and greater time spent awake during the dark phase. Consequently, it is possible that instead of tracking time of day or light/dark phase, fluctuations in DA uptake observed in our prior work are associated with distinct patterns of sleep/wake architecture that occur across the 24 h cycle.

Here we hypothesize that DAT function tracks sleep/wake state activity such that higher uptake rates occur during periods of sleep and lower uptake rates occur during wakefulness. To test this hypothesis, we performed a series of experiments in rats to examine the degree to which sleep/wake state and light/dark cycle impact DA terminal neurotransmission and phosphorylation of DAT at Thr53 site using a combination of sleep/wake recordings, fast scan cyclic voltammetry (FSCV), and western blotting. Further, given that psychostimulants increase DA through interactions with the DAT, and since there is evidence that DA-dependent behaviors—including psychostimulant self-administration—are strongly influenced by diurnal cycles [25,26,27,28,29], we examined whether the degree of DA uptake inhibition produced by cocaine (cocaine potency) also varies across sleep/wake state. We predicted that sleep/wake state influences DA uptake regardless of light/dark phase via phosphorylation of the DAT at Thr53, and that these fluctuations impact cocaine potency.

Methods

Animal housing and conditions

Experiments were conducted in adult male Sprague-Dawley rats (325–350 g, Envigo, Frederick, MD, USA). Rats were pair-housed prior to surgery and subsequently single-housed in a temperature-controlled room (24 °C) on a 12 h light/dark cycle. To collect brains at the same time of the workday, but at different light/dark phases, lights were on from 3:00 a.m. to 3:00 p.m. for animals sacrificed at zeitgeber time 6 (ZT6; six h into the light phase) and lights were on from 3:00 p.m. to 3:00 a.m. for animals sacrificed at zeitgeber time 18 (ZT18; six h into the dark phase). Rats were given ad libitum access to food, water, and enrichment material. All protocols and animal procedures were conducted in accordance with the National Institutes of Health (NIH) Guide for the Care and Use of Laboratory Animals under the supervision of the Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee at Drexel University College of Medicine.

Surgical procedures

All surgical procedures were performed stereotaxically under aseptic conditions, as described previously [30,31,32]. Rats were implanted with electroencephalographic/electromyographic (EEG–EMG) electrodes to determine sleep/wake state prior to assessing DA release and uptake via ex vivo FSCV or tissue quantification of key DA proteins. To obtain differential recordings of EEG activity, one screw electrode (Plastics One, Roanoke, VA, USA) was implanted above the frontal cortex (+1.3 mm A/P, +1.3 mm M/L, and −1.0 mm D/V) and a second electrode was implanted ipsilaterally over hippocampus (+2.4 mm A/P, +3.2 mm M/L, and −1.0 mm D/V). A third screw electrode was implanted in contralateral cortex (+1.3 mm A/P, −1.3 mm M/L, and −1.0 mm D/V) and served as an isolated ground. Additionally, two EMG wire electrodes were implanted into the dorsal neck muscle for recording EMG activity. EMG electrodes consisted of 100 mm insulated stainless-steel wires (Cooner Wire, Charsworth, CA, USA) with 2 mm of exposed wire in contact with muscle. Electrodes were routed through a connector (Plastics One) and cemented into place on the skull. Ketoprofen (5 mg/kg s.c. of 5 mg/ml) and Baytril (5 mg/kg s.c. of 5 mg/ml) were provided at the time of surgery and a second dose 12 h later. In addition, antibiotic/analgesic powder (Neopredef, Kalamazoo, MI) was applied around the cranial implant immediately after surgery. Rats recovered for 7 days before sleep recordings began.

Sleep recordings

Rats were placed in an acrylic testing chamber (14 × 14 × 20 inches) housed in a sound-attenuated outer chamber containing an LED light and a fan. Rats were supplied with food and water available ad libitum and connected to recording lines via a commutator which allowed unrestricted movement. After a 16 h acclimation period, EEG–EMG signals were recorded for 24 h. EEG (0.3–100 Hz bandpass) and EMG signals (1–50 Hz bandpass) were amplified, filtered, and recorded using Labchart 7 (AD Instruments, Colorado Springs, CO, USA; Fig. 1). Data were analyzed manually using Sirenia Sleep Pro (Pinnacle Technology, Lawrence, KS, USA) to determine the number of bouts, bout length, and percentage of time spent in wakefulness (Wake), rapid eye movement (REM) and non-REM (NREM) sleep, as previously described [30,31,32]. Briefly, EEG signals were scored in 10 s bins and sorted into individual frequency bands (Delta = 0.5–4.0 Hz; Theta = 5.5–8.5 Hz; Alpha, Beta, and Gamma = 8–44 Hz). NREM sleep was defined as high-voltage EEG consisting of >50% delta and low-voltage EMG; REM sleep was defined as low-voltage EEG consisting of >50% theta, combined with EMG activity of ~50% lower amplitude than that observed in NREM sleep; and Wake was defined as low-voltage EEG consisting of <40% delta and <20% theta with EMG activity of an average amplitude twice that observed in NREM. To be scored as a distinct epoch, the appropriate EEG and EMG activity patterns were required to persist for a minimum of three bins (30 s).

Fig. 1: Sleep/wake state across a 24 h period is organized into recurring bouts during which sleep primarily occurs during the light phase and wakefulness during the dark phase.
figure 1

A Rats were implanted with EEG/EMG electrodes. After 7 days of recovery, rats were acclimated to recording chambers for 16 h prior to recording of sleep/wake activity for 24 h. Following sleep recordings, rats were sacrificed 6 h into the light phase (ZT6) or 6 h into the dark-phase (ZT18) and brains were prepared for FSCV recordings in the NAc core. A separate group of rats was subjected to the same sleep recording procedures except that after sacrifice, the NAc was dissected for western blot analysis of plasma membrane fractions of DAT and phosphorylated DAT. B Representative EEG and EMG recordings. C Example hypnogram across a 24 h period. White bar represents the light phase and the black bar represents the dark phase. D Mean percentage of time rats spent in Wake, NREM, and REM during the light and dark phase for the full 24 h recording (n = 18 rats).

To confirm that recordings were representative of normal sleep/wake patterns and consistent with prior reports, the mean percentage of time spent in Wake, NREM and REM during the light and dark phases was analyzed for the full 24 h recording (n = 18 rats; Fig. 1B–D).

Ex vivo slice FSCV procedures

Following sleep recordings, rats were anesthetized with 2.5% isoflurane for 5 min and then decapitated. These anesthesia procedures have been shown previously to have no effect on DA uptake [33]. To control for the effect of light/dark condition, one group of rats was decapitated 6 h into the light phase (ZT6; n = 14) and the other group was decapitated 6 h into the dark-phase (ZT18; n = 16). Animals remained in their corresponding light/dark phase during isoflurane anesthesia prior to decapitation. Brains were rapidly dissected and transferred to oxygenated, ice-cold artificial cerebral spinal fluid (aCSF) containing NaCl (126 mM), KCL (2.5 mM), NaH2PO4 (1.2 mM), CaCl2 (2.4 mM), MgCl2 (1.2 mM), NaHCO3 (25 mM), glucose (11 mM), L-ascorbic acid (0.4 mM), and pH adjusted to 7.4. A vibrating microtome was used to produce 400 µm-thick sections containing the nucleus accumbens (NAc) core. Slices rested at room temperature for 45 min before being transferred into a recording chamber flushed with aCSF (32 °C).

A bipolar stimulating electrode (Plastics One) was placed on the surface of the tissue and a carbon fiber microelectrode was implanted between stimulating electrode leads. DA release was evoked every 3 min using a single electrical pulse (~400 μA, 4 ms, monophasic) and measured using Demon Voltammetry and Analysis software [34]. Baseline DA release and uptake recordings were obtained for the subset of rats in which only single slices were recorded (ZT6, n = 6 slices from six rats; ZT18, n = 7 slices from seven rats) and the subset of rats in which two slices were recorded (ZT6, n = 14 slices from seven rats; ZT18, n = 18 slices and from nine rats). For rats in which two slices were recorded, DA release and uptake measures were averaged to obtain a single data point for those rats. Therefore, we analyzed and report baseline DA release and uptake data for 13 rats at ZT6 and 16 rats at ZT18. Once stable baselines were obtained for all slices (three stimulations with <10% variation), a subset of slices was then exposed to five concentrations of either cocaine (0.3–30 μM; ZT6, n = 9 slices from 9 rats; ZT18 n = 14 slices from 14 rats) or the D2R/D3R agonist quinpirole (3–300 nM; ZT6, n = 12 slices from 12 rats; ZT18 n = 12 slices from 12 rats) as previously described [13, 21, 33, 35,36,37,38,39]. In some cases, one slice was recorded shortly after rats were sacrificed (n = 26) and a second slice was recorded up to 7 h post sacrifice (n = 19). Supplementary Figure 1 demonstrates that the time of recording post sacrifice did not affect DA release or uptake.

DA concentrations were calculated by comparing currents at the peak oxidation potential for DA in consecutive voltammograms with electrode calibrations determined using an in situ calibration method as described previously [40, 41]. To determine if sleep/wake state influences DA terminal neurotransmission, we assessed stimulated DA release, DA uptake rate (Vmax), cocaine-induced DA uptake inhibition (app Km), and the effects of quinpirole on stimulated DA release across various sleep/wake states using a Michaelis–Menten based model [30, 33, 35, 42]. Baseline uptake was determined by setting Km values to 0.18 µM and all cocaine-induced alterations in uptake were attributed to changes in apparent Km. Cocaine inhibition constants (Ki) were determined to calculate the necessary cocaine concentration to produce 50% of DA uptake inhibition. Reductions in stimulated DA release following varying concentrations of quinpirole were expressed as a percent of baseline.

Subcellular fractionation and western blotting

Following sleep recordings, a separate cohort of rats was sacrificed for western blot analysis (ZT6; 13 rats and ZT18; 14 rats). Rats were decapitated, brains extracted, flash frozen in liquid nitrogen, and stored at −80 °C until the day of the assay. Briefly, the NAc core was dissected and subjected to subcellular fractionation methods according to previous work [43, 44]. Between 0.10 and 0.15 g of tissue was homogenized in 1.5 mL of ice-cold PBS containing sucrose (0.32 M) and HEPES (20 mM), EGTA (1 mM), EDTA (1 mM), and protease inhibitors. The homogenate was centrifugated at 1000 × g for 10 min and the pellet discarded. The supernatant (total protein extract) was spun at 12,500 × g for 20 min at 4 °C. Synaptosomes were lysed in hypotonic media on ice for 45 min and centrifuged at 2000 × g for 20 min to obtain the enriched plasma membrane fraction. Previous work using this subcellular fractionation protocol has shown high DAT levels, as well as other membrane proteins including Na+/K+ ATPase in the enriched plasma membrane fraction [43]. Thus, in the current western blot studies, we used the enriched plasma membrane fraction and refer to this fractions as plasma membrane DAT [43]. Protein concentrations were measured using the DCTM protein assay (Bio-Rad), and equal amounts of total protein were loaded on a 10% SDS PAGE gel and immunoblotted for specific antigens: rabbit anti-DAT polyclonal antibody (1:1000, AB2231, EMD Millipore Corp, RRID:AB_1586991), rabbit anti phospho-DAT polyclonal antibody (1:1000, P435-53, PhosphoSolutions, RRID:AB_2492078), and peroxidase-conjugated goat anti-rabbit IgG (H + L) (1:5000, Jackson Immuno Research, RRID:AB_2313586). GAPDH was used as a marker for plasma membrane in the same blots using rabbit anti GAPDH polyclonal antibody (1:5000, PA1-987, Invitrogen, RRID: AB_2107311). GAPDH has been validated as an ideal reference due to a lack of fluctuations in levels across sleep/wake states [45]. Western blot data were quantified by densitometry with ImageQuant LAS4000 (GE Healthcare Bio-Science) and presented as the ratio of total DAT over GAPDH, phosphorylated DAT (pDAT) over GAPDH, or pDAT/GAPDH over DAT/GAPDH [21].

Statistical analyses

Data were analyzed using IBM SPSS Statistics 24 (SPSS Inc, Chicago, IL). Prior in vitro studies indicate that the DAT can undergo rapid posttranslational modifications and trafficking on the order of seconds to minutes [16, 19, 20, 46, 47]. Together with the fast transitions from sleep to awake throughout the day (Fig. 1C), we hypothesized that relatively brief periods of specific sleep/wake states prior to sacrifice would predict differences in DA release and uptake rate. We used linear regressions to examine whether the percentage, bout number, or bout length of Wake, NREM or REM sleep that occurred in the 5, 15, and 30 min prior to sacrifice significantly predicted ex vivo FSCV measures of DA release and uptake (Fig. 2C–E, G–I and Supplementary Tables 1, 2). We refer to these periods of wakefulness and sleep prior to sacrifice as the 5-, 15-, and 30 min time periods (see Supplementary Fig. 2).

Fig. 2: Dopamine release and uptake fluctuate across sleep/wake state.
figure 2

A Average current versus time plots and (B) example color plots for the Wake and Sleep groups during the light (ZT6) and dark (ZT18) phases. Shaded areas in current versus time plots represent SEM. Correlations between DA release and (C) REM bouts, (D) % of Wake, or (E) % of REM during the 30-min time period. F DA release for rats categorized into Wake and Sleep groups for ZT6 and ZT18 based on sleep/wake state during the 30 min time period. Correlations between DA uptake and (G) % of Wake, (H) % of NREM, or (I) % of REM during the 30 min time period. J DA uptake rate for rats categorized into Wake and Sleep groups for ZT6 and ZT18 based on sleep/wake state during the 30 min time period. K DA uptake rate for rats categorized into Wake and Sleep groups collapsed across ZT6 and ZT18. Wake (ZT6) n = 5 rats, Wake (ZT18) n = 9 rats, Sleep (ZT6) n = 8 rats, and Sleep (ZT18) n = 7 rats. Correlation coefficients associated p values, and black regression lines represent analyses on data collapsed across ZT6/ZT18. Yellow dashed lines depict regression lines for ZT6 data, and red dotted lines depict regression lines for ZT18 data. Note: data points in correlations appear different across panels due to overlaying of symbols. Data are shown as mean ± SEM. *p < 0.05; **p < 0.01; ***p < 0.001.

Based on our finding that sleep/wake activity during the 30 min prior to sacrifice (i.e., the 30 min time period) was strongly correlated with measures of DA release and/or uptake, we used a median split to dichotomize sleep/wake data into separate Wake and Sleep groups based on percentage of time spent awake. The median and any value above it was designated as the “Wake” group and every value below the median was designated as the “Sleep” group for each of the light/dark phases (light = ZT6, dark = ZT18). Two-way ANOVAs were then used to determine differences in DA release and uptake with time of day and sleep/wake state as between-subjects variables (Fig. 2F, J). Following the two-way ANOVAs, Sidak’s post hoc tests were used to determine whether DA release, DA uptake, and the effects of cocaine varied across sleep/wake state. Because of our a priori hypothesis that DAT function would vary with sleep/wake states regardless of light/dark phase, a subset of Sidak’s post hoc tests were conducted even though no significant interaction was observed in the two-way ANOVA. When no significant main effect was observed for a variable (e.g., time of day for DA uptake rate) data were collapsed across that variable and Student’s t tests were used to assess differences between the Wake and Sleep groups (Fig. 2K).

To examine if the effects of cocaine or quinpirole differed between Wake and Sleep groups, two-way mixed design ANOVAs were conducted with cocaine or quinpirole concentration as the within-subjects variable and sleep/wake state as the between-subjects variable. Cocaine Ki was determined by plotting the linear uptake inhibition effect of the 5 cumulative concentrations of cocaine and determining the slope of the linear regression. Ki values were calculated by the equation Km/slope [13, 48, 49]. Ki was compared between the Wake and Sleep groups using a Students’ t test. Differences in DAT and pDAT between the Wake and Sleep groups were assessed using a Students’ t tests (Fig. 4C–E).

Results

Sleep/wake activity is organized into bouts occurring on the time scale of minutes

To confirm that sleep/wake activity consists of recurring bouts of Wake, NREM, and REM during both the light- (ZT6) and dark-phase (ZT18), we monitored sleep/wake state for 24 h using EEG–EMG recordings in freely moving rats (Fig. 1). As a group, rats spent ~44.9% of the time awake (633.31 ± 67.5 min), 46.7% in NREM sleep (658.14 ± 68.30 min) and 8.3% in REM sleep (117.61 ± 21.10 min) over the 24 h period; 65.27% of NREM and 60.39% of REM occurred during the light phase while 65.33% of Wake occurred during the dark phase (Fig. 1B–D). Wake, NREM, and REM were organized into numerous bouts over the 24 h period indicating that sleep/wake states undergo several transitions across the day and within each light/dark phase (Fig. 1B–D; Supplementary Table 1). Importantly, the distribution of sleep/wake state percentages, bout number, and bout length were similar to published observations in adult rats [50,51,52,53,54].

Dopamine release fluctuates across sleep/wake state only during the light phase

To examine whether sleep/wake state predicts DA release, we analyzed EEG–EMG signals 5, 15, and 30 min prior to sacrificing rats and then assessed DA dynamics using FSCV in rat NAc slices obtained during the light (ZT6) or dark phase (ZT18; Figs. 1A, 2A–F). While no significant relationships were observed between bout length and DA release for any of the sleep/wake states, we did observe a significant relationship between the number of NREM and REM bouts during the 15 min time period and magnitude of DA release (Supplementary Table 2). However, the number of REM bouts (r = 0.683, p < 0.001) during the 30 min time period resulted in the strongest predictor of DA release, accounting for 46.6% of the variability (Fig. 2C; Supplementary Table 2). Although a significant relationship was observed between the percentage of Wake (r = 0.459, p = 0.012) and REM (r = 0.526, p = 0.003) during the 30 min time period and magnitude of DA release, this effect was comparatively modest, accounting for only 21.1% and 27.7% of the variability, respectively (Fig. 2D, E; Supplementary Table 2).

Despite these observations, the relationship between sleep/wake state and DA release appeared to be driven primarily by data obtained during the light phase (Fig. 2A–E). To test for this possibility, we separated our data into “Wake” and “Sleep” groups using a median split for the 30 min time period, during which we had observed the strongest relationship with DA release. Two-way ANOVA with sleep/wake state and light/dark phase as between-subjects variables revealed a significant effect of sleep/wake state (F(1,25) = 8.193, p = 0.008) and a significant interaction between sleep/wake state and light/dark phase on DA release (F(1,25) = 4.507, p = 0.044), but no significant effect of light/dark phase (F(1,25) = 1.149, p = 0.294). Sidak’s post hoc analysis revealed significantly greater DA release in the Sleep group during the light phase (ZT6; F(1,25) = 11.096, p = 0.003), but not during the dark phase (ZT18; F(1,25) = 0.307, p = 0.585; Fig. 2F). Additionally, there was significantly greater DA release in the Sleep group during the light phase (ZT6) compared to the dark phase (ZT18; F(1,25) = 5.530, p = 0.027), but no differences in DA release between light/dark phase in the Wake group (F(1,25) = 0.522, p = 0.477; Fig. 2F). These data suggest that DA release differs across sleep/wake state depending on the light/dark phase, with REM sleep as the best predictor of DA release.

Dopamine uptake varies across sleep/wake state regardless of light/dark phase

To examine whether sleep/wake state predicts DA uptake rate (Vmax; µM/s), we analyzed EEG–EMG signals for the 5, 15, and 30 min prior to sacrificing rats and then assessed DA dynamics using FSCV in rat NAc slices obtained during the light (ZT6) or dark phase (ZT18; Figs. 1A, 2G–K). Significant relationships were observed between the percentage of time in each sleep/wake state during the 5-, 15-, and 30 min time periods and the rate of DA uptake (Supplementary Table 3). However, the percentage of Wake (r = 0.657, p < 0.001) and NREM (r = 0.624, p < 0.001) at the 30 min time period resulted in the strongest predictor of DA uptake, accounting for 43.2% and 39.0% of the variability, respectively (Fig. 2G, H; Supplementary Table 3). Although a significant relationship between percentage of REM at the 30 min time period and DA uptake was also observed (r = 0.448, p = 0.015), this effect was comparatively modest (Fig. 2I; Supplementary Table 3). Neither the number of bouts nor bout length of any sleep/wake state were robust predictors of DA uptake at any time period (Supplementary Table 3).

To examine whether the relationship between sleep/wake state and DA uptake differs between the light (ZT6) and dark (ZT18) phase. We again used a median split to separate our data into Wake and Sleep groups for the 30 min time period. Two-way ANOVA with sleep/wake state and light/dark phase as between-subjects variables revealed a significant effect of sleep/wake state on DA uptake rate (F(1,25) = 37.300, p < 0.001) but no significant effect of light/dark phase (F(1,25) = 0.511, p = 0.481), nor sleep/wake state and light/dark phase interaction (F(1,25) = 1.456, p = 0.239). Because of our a priori hypothesis that DAT function would vary with sleep/wake state regardless of light/dark phase, we used a Sidak’s post hoc to test for differences in DA uptake across the Wake and Sleep groups at both the light and dark phases. As shown in Fig. 2J, DA uptake rate was significantly faster in the Sleep group during both the light (F(1,25) = 23.823, p < 0.001) and dark phases (F(1,25) = 13.689, p < 0.001). Further, a t test showed significantly faster DA uptake in the Sleep group when collapsed across light/dark phase (t27 = −6.005, p < 0.001; Fig. 2K). Together, these results suggest that DA uptake is more efficient after periods of sleep, independent of the light/dark phase.

DAT phosphorylation state likely accounts for fluctuations in dopamine uptake

DAT trafficking and posttranslational modifications such as phosphorylation at the Thr53 site on the DAT (i.e., pDAT) can influence DA uptake through putative alterations in DAT conformational state [12, 22, 55,56,57,58,59]. To examine whether fluctuations in DA uptake across sleep/wake state could be a consequence of alterations in DAT protein levels, we conducted a western blot experiment in a separate cohort of rats. We analyzed EEG–EMG activity in the 30 min immediately prior to collecting NAc tissue for western blot analysis of plasma membrane DAT and pDAT during the light (ZT6) or dark phases (ZT18; Fig. 3). We again used a median split to separate our data into Wake and Sleep groups for the 30 min time period. A t test revealed that plasma membrane DAT did not differ across sleep/wake state, (t19.979 = 0.779, p = 0.445; Fig. 3B). However, pDAT levels were significantly higher in the Sleep group (t25 = 2.21, p = 0.036; Fig. 3C). Further, plasma membrane pDAT/DAT ratio showed a strong trend (t25 = 2.033, p = 0.053; Fig. 3D) for a significant difference between the Wake and Sleep groups, indicating the possibility that the proportion of transporters that are phosphorylated varies across sleep/wake state. These results are consistent with the higher DA uptake rate observed following periods of sleep, suggesting that fluctuations in DA uptake across sleep/wake state may be driven, in part, by levels of pDAT.

Fig. 3: Plasma membrane pDAT levels are higher after periods of sleep.
figure 3

A Representative blots from adjacent lanes for NAc plasma membrane DAT, pDAT, and GAPDH. B Average plasma membrane DAT and (C) pDAT expressed as a ratio over GAPDH, and (D) pDAT/DAT expressed as a ratio over GAPDH for rats categorized into Wake and Sleep groups. B–D Wake n = 13, Sleep n = 14. Data are shown as mean ± SEM. *p < 0.05.

Fig. 4: Cocaine potency is higher after periods of sleep.
figure 4

A Average current versus time plots and example color plots for rats categorized into Wake and Sleep groups after exposure to 30 µM cocaine. Shaded areas in current versus time plots represent SEM. B Inhibition of DA uptake following cumulative cocaine concentrations (0.3–30 μM) and (C) cocaine Ki for the Wake and Sleep groups. D Average current versus time plots and example color plots for the Wake and Sleep groups after exposure to 300 nM quinpirole. 13.8% and 17.7% represent average percent of baseline for stimulated release amplitudes. Shaded areas in current versus time plots represent SEM. E DA release following cumulative quinpirole concentrations (3–300 nM) for the Wake and Sleep groups. A–C Wake n = 12, Sleep n = 11. D, E Wake n = 12, Sleep n = 12. Data are shown as mean ± SEM. *p < 0.05.

Cocaine potency fluctuates across sleep/wake state

In addition to enhancing basal DA uptake rate, phosphorylation of the DAT at Thr53 has been shown to enhance DAT sensitivity to cocaine analogs [22, 55, 60, 61], suggesting that fluctuations in pDAT could impact the effects of cocaine. To examine whether cocaine potency at the DAT fluctuates across sleep/wake state, we analyzed EEG–EMG signals in the 30 min prior to sacrificing rats and then examined cocaine-induced DA uptake inhibition using FSCV in rat NAc slices obtained during the light (ZT6) or dark period (ZT18; Figs. 1A, 4A–C). We used a median split to separate our data into Wake and Sleep groups for the 30 min time period. Two-way repeated measures ANOVA with sleep/wake state as the between-subjects variable and cocaine concentration as a within-subjects measures variable revealed a significant effect of sleep/wake state (F(1,10) = 5.63, p = 0.039), and cocaine concentration (Greenhouse–Geisser correction; F(1.082,10.818) = 128.210, p < 0.005), but no significant sleep/wake state × cocaine concentration interaction (Greenhouse–Geisser correction; F(1.034,10.34) = 3.998, p = 0.072). Because of our a priori hypothesis that DAT function would vary with sleep/wake state, we used Sidak’s post hoc tests to examine whether the effects of cocaine would differ across the Wake and Sleep groups. As shown in Fig. 4A, B, cocaine was significantly more effective at inhibiting DA uptake in the Sleep group at the 30 µM cocaine concentration compared to the Wake group (F(1,9) = 8.293, p = 0.018; Fig. 4A, B). Consistent with this, the cocaine concentration needed to inhibit 50% of uptake (Ki) was lower in the Sleep group indicating a significant increase in cocaine potency at the DAT (t20 = −2.512), p = 0.044; Fig. 4C). These results are consistent with our observations that both DA uptake rate and pDAT levels are enhanced after periods of sleep, suggesting the likelihood that underlying changes in DAT function are responsible for the observed fluctuations in cocaine potency.

D2R/D3R sensitivity does not account for the fluctuation in dopamine release

D2R/D3R in the striatum have been demonstrated to modulate DA release [13, 62,63,64,65,66] by hyperpolarization via voltage-dependent potassium channels [67] and by inhibiting calcium entry [68]. D2R/D3R are also known to regulate DA uptake via a direct interaction with DAT or through activation of downstream signaling molecules such as ERK1/2 and PKC which can regulate the surface expression of DAT and hence DA uptake [69,70,71]. These observations suggest the possibility that fluctuations in DA release, uptake, or DAT sensitivity to cocaine could be influenced by differences in D2R/D3R function. To test for this possibility, we analyzed EEG–EMG signals for the 30 min prior to sacrificing rats and then assessed the effects of the D2R/D3R agonist quinpirole using FSCV recordings in rat NAc slices obtained during the light (ZT6) or dark phase (ZT18; Figs. 1A, 4D, E). We used a median split to separate our data into Wake and Sleep groups for the 30 min time period. Two-way mixed design ANOVA with sleep/wake state as the between-subjects variable and quinpirole concentration as the within-subjects measures variable revealed a significant effect of quinpirole concentration (F(1,9) = 424.389, p < 0.0005), but no significant effect of sleep/wake state (F(1,9) = 1.571, p = 0.242) nor significant sleep/wake state × quinpirole concentration interaction (F(1,9) = 0.192, p = 0.672). Given the observation that quinpirole had a similar effect on DA release in the Wake and Sleep groups (Fig. 4D, E), it appears that D2R/D3R sensitivity is not likely to be the mechanism through which DA release and uptake fluctuate across sleep/wake state.

Discussion

The current experiments examined whether DA release and uptake fluctuate across sleep/wake state using ex vivo FSCV evaluation of DA dynamics and biochemical assessment of plasma membrane DAT and pDAT levels in the NAc core. We demonstrated that sleep/wake state predicts both DA release and uptake rate, but that only DA uptake fluctuated exclusively across sleep/wake activity and not light/dark phase. These variations in DA release and uptake do not appear to be dependent on changes in D2R/D3R, given that sensitivity to quinpirole did not fluctuate across sleep/wake state. Western blotting demonstrated that periods of sleep were associated with higher levels of phosphorylation of the DAT at Thr53 and not an increase in DAT levels on the plasma membrane. Consistent with this, the effects of cocaine at inhibiting DA uptake were more potent after periods of sleep.

Fluctuations in dopamine release across sleep/wake state are driven primarily by REM sleep

In the current studies, we observed that during the light phase (ZT6), DA release was highest after periods of sleep and lowest after periods of wakefulness. These findings are consistent with variations in DA neuron activity and NAc DA levels across sleep/wake state that are also observed during the light phase [72,73,74]. In contrast, we found that DA release did not differ between wakefulness and sleep during the dark phase (ZT18), which could suggest that light/dark condition exerts an important influence on DA release. This possibility does not appear likely given that (1) there was no significant effect of light/dark phase on DA release (Fig. 2F); and (2) previous evidence indicates that extracellular DA tone in the NAc fluctuates across a 24 h period even when rats were exposed to constant light or darkness—in other words independent of the light/dark phase [75]. Rather, we suggest that variations in DA release across sleep/wake state only during the light phase may be associated with marked differences in sleep architecture observed between the light and dark phases. In particular, we note that we observed more frequent and longer REM bouts during the light phase and few and brief REM bouts during the dark phase. Given that REM sleep has been associated with higher DA tone in the NAc and increased DA neuron activity in the ventral tegmental area [72,73,74]—in addition to our finding that REM is the best predictor of DA release—this increased REM activity during the light phase, positions REM sleep as a potentially critical participant in the regulation of DA release.

Dopamine uptake rate fluctuates across sleep/wake state, regardless of light/dark phase

Recent observations demonstrate that DA metabolism, D2R/D3R sensitivity, and TH expression are not the mechanisms driving diurnal variation in extracellular DA tone [24, 76]. Importantly, however, DAT function was shown to be a critical governor of this fluctuation with higher uptake rate during the light phase when animals are usually asleep and lower uptake during the dark phase when animals are usually awake [24]. Consistent with this, we observed that DATs are more efficient after periods of sleep compared to periods of wakefulness regardless of light/dark phase, suggesting that sleep/wake state is a stronger zeitgeber for DA uptake efficiency than light/dark phase.

High pDAT levels are consistent with increased dopamine uptake and exaggerated cocaine potency

DAT proteins are highly dynamic and continuously distributed between the plasma membrane to intracellular endosomal compartments [12]. This type of DAT trafficking can modulate DAT function in response to physiological conditions on a time scale of seconds to minutes [12, 16]. In spite of previous work showing higher total DAT levels during the dark phase and lower during the light period in the NAc [77], we observed no variations in plasma membrane DAT levels in our studies indicating that DA uptake fluctuations across sleep/wake state are likely not attributable to changes in DAT. This discrepancy between studies could be related to the fact that we measured plasma membrane DAT instead of total DAT number and that we control for both sleep/wake state and light/dark phase.

Numerous observations suggest that enhanced phosphorylation of DAT at Thr53 promotes DA uptake efficiency and sensitivity to psychostimulants [21, 55, 59, 60]. Consistent with these findings, we observed that pDAT was significantly higher following periods of sleep suggesting that the relationship between sleep/wake state and DA uptake observed in our FSCV experiments may be mediated by alterations in phosphorylation of DAT at Thr53. In addition to the importance of DAT phosphorylation state, numerous other mechanisms may be involved in altered DAT function. Indeed, several studies indicate that alterations in basal DA uptake rate may also be a product of changes in the balance of inward/outward facing DAT [78], changes in oligomer/monomer ratios [79, 80], dimerization with sigma receptor [81], or differential DAT phosphorylation states on serine sites [82, 83]. Thus, although our present findings indicate a potentially critical link between pDAT fluctuations across sleep/wake states that match alterations in DA uptake rate, other mechanisms may also be involved.

Emerging evidence posits that faster rates of DA uptake and high levels of pDAT increase cocaine potency at the DAT, which is manifested as higher cocaine-induced DA uptake inhibition [21, 55, 59]. Consistent with these findings, we observed that after periods of sleep—when DA uptake efficiency was at its highest—cocaine potency was exaggerated. These findings suggest the likelihood that fluctuations in DA uptake across sleep/wake state are due to alterations in DAT state that impact its ability to interact with cocaine, and possibly other psychostimulants.

Circadian and ultradian influences on dopamine neurotransmission

In addition to sleep/wake state, it is also possible that circadian oscillations may participate in the regulation of DA neurotransmission. For example, TH—the rate limiting step in DA synthesis—varies in a circadian-like fashion with significantly higher expression and activity in the NAc during the dark phase [24, 84]. Consistent with this finding, mutant mice without a functional circadian Clock gene show increased expression and phosphorylation of TH which is associated with enhanced firing of ventral tegmental area DA neurons [84, 85]. These influences on TH levels and activity do not appear to coincide with our present findings, however, given that we did not observe overall differences in DA release or uptake across time of day. Nevertheless, we did observe that DA uptake rate varied across sleep/wake state regardless of the time of the day suggesting that DAT function may be responsive to influences that occur on a shorter time scale.

Ultradian rhythms are repeated biological patterns that occur with periods shorter than 24 h. Well-recognized ultradian rhythms include sleep, body temperature, motor activity, heart rate, feeding, and many others [86,87,88,89,90]. In the context of our findings, observed fluctuations in DA uptake, and DAT phosphorylation are consistent with ultradian regulation, including by sleep/wake state as we propose here. However, the extent to which these DA changes are directly attributable to sleep/wake state, or possibly to other ultradian rhythms that closely follow sleep/wake patterns (e.g., motor activity or body temperature), remains unclear. Moreover, while we propose that fluctuations in sleep/wake state influence DA release and uptake, it is also possible that the relationship is reversed, with changes in DA responsible for fluctuations in sleep/wake state. In fact, recent observations indicate that DAT knockout mice have lengthened ultradian motor activity periods and display increase time spent awake [8, 91], and that chemogenetic or optogenetic activation of DA neuron activity enhances motor activity and reduces sleep [73, 91].

Behavioral implications

Decades of research indicate that the expression of numerous behaviors is exquisitely reliant on tight regulation of DA neurotransmission across the neuroaxis [92,93,94,95,96,97,98]. Considering the amassing evidence that dysregulation of DA-dependent processes may be a contributing factor to a variety of neuropsychiatric conditions [5, 9, 10, 99], the fluctuations in DA release and uptake observed in our studies are likely to have profound impacts on behavior in both normal and disease states.

In the context of goal-directed and reward-associated behaviors, evidence suggests that food and drug self-administration, locomotor sensitization, conditioned place preference, and drug craving are influenced by diurnal and/or circadian rhythms which have been posited to involve DA signaling [76, 85, 100,101,102,103]. Further, in the case of cocaine, potency at the DAT is known to influence behavior with high cocaine potency tied to increases in cocaine self-administration [21, 35, 49, 59, 104, 105], and reduced cocaine potency leading to reduced cocaine self-administration [49, 106]. Thus, cocaine potency fluctuations across sleep/wake state are expected to drive more pronounced cocaine intake after extended periods of sleep. This finding may help to explain prior research showing that rats exposed to a discrete trials procedure display nearly 100% probability of taking cocaine at the start of the dark phase, when they would have just awakened from an extended period of sleep [29]. Indeed, pharmacological manipulations known to impact DA uptake and cocaine potency disrupt cocaine intake under these discrete trial conditions [26].

When considered together, these observations suggest that the relationship between sleep/wake state and DA neurotransmission may be one explanation for why reward-associated behaviors have marked diurnal oscillations [73, 101,102,103, 107, 108]. Future work will attempt to disentangle the complex interactions between sleep/wake activity, general arousal state, and motivation for rewards, and the degree to which this involves dynamic alterations in DA neurotransmission.

Summary

In conclusion, our studies demonstrate a robust association between sleep/wake state and DA terminal neurotransmission, with higher DA uptake rate, increased pDAT, and enhanced cocaine potency after periods of sleep. In addition to providing a possible link between DA neurotransmission and diurnal variations in goal-directed behaviors across the day, our observations also suggest that fluctuations in DA neurotransmission may also explain diurnal changes in a host of DA-dependent behaviors and physiological processes. Finally, our findings also provide evidence for the need to carefully consider the potential impacts that sleep/wake state, time of day, and light/dark condition may have on measures of both neurotransmission and behavior across many areas of research.

Funding and disclosure

This work was supported by NIH grant DA031900 to RAE, MH106912 to SK, Fondecyt Initiation Fund N°11191049 to JAP, NIH grant DA038598 to GET, as well as the Drexel University Dean’s Fellowship for Excellence in Collaborative or Themed Research to IPA. The authors report no biomedical financial interests or potential competing interest.