INTRODUCTION

It has been proposed that delusions, which are a hallmark of psychosis in schizophrenia, are the product of abnormally reinforced/rewarded thoughts and associations. Under normal circumstances the dopamine neurons fire in a context-appropriate, event-linked fashion and this facilitates the formation of new associations, and strengthens already learnt associations (King et al, 1984). It is hypothesized that in schizophrenia the dopamine system is dysregulated leading to a ‘chaotic’ firing of dopamine neurons and heightened levels of dopamine release (King et al, 1984; Shaner, 1999). As a result, event-independent and context-inappropriate associations are reinforced leading to the formation of fixed and false associations (delusions) (Kapur, 2003; King et al, 1984; Miller, 1984; Shaner, 1999).

The formation and expression of these associations involves the ‘brain reward system’, with a central role for the ventral striatum (nucleus accumbens in animals)—a region that receives information-carrying inputs from the prefrontal cortex and amygdala and dopaminergic inputs from the midbrain. Several recent studies in normal volunteers have shown a significant role of the ventral striatum in the learning of associations—both appetitive as well as aversive (eg Jensen et al, 2007; McClure et al, 2003; O'Doherty et al, 2003, 2004). These studies show that as associations are learnt, structures that are initially activated by unconditioned stimuli, come to respond to neutral stimuli as a function of their repeated pairings. It has been suggested that this process may be abnormal in schizophrenia (Kapur, 2003; King et al, 1984; Shaner, 1999), thus leading to the learning and reinforcement of inappropriate associations. Pharmacological manipulation of the dopamine system affects the Blood oxygen level dependent (BOLD) activity that occurs during learning in instrumental reward learning (Pessiglione et al, 2006) and in aversive conditioning (Menon et al, in press). Menon et al used event related functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) and an aversive conditioning paradigm and showed that amphetamine increases a learning related signal in striatal regions to cues signaling aversive events, while haloperidol eliminates this signal thus suggesting that dopamine modulates the BOLD responses to aversive stimuli in an aversive conditioning paradigm. It is interesting in that regard that the medical treatment of psychosis involves antipsychotics, drugs which have been shown to block the dopamine system within the ventral striatum (and nucleus accumbens in animals) leading to profound molecular and physiological changes in the brain reward circuits (Arnt and Skarsfeldt, 1998; Wadenberg et al, 2001).

Despite this emerging body of evidence linking the ventral striatum to the formation of rewarding associations, only a few reports have considered this implication for schizophrenia. A recent study (Juckel et al, 2006b) reported that unmedicated patients with schizophrenia showed reduced ventral striatal activation during presentation of reward-predicting cues. Another study focusing on the effect generated by the rewarding experience rather than reward anticipation, showed that patients rated the hedonic value of the pleasant odors lower than controls and also failed to activate limbic/paralimbic structures invoked by normal controls (Crespo-Facorro et al, 2001). Studies using positive and negative pictures as rewarding stimuli also showed that patients failed to recruit parts of a reward-related circuit (ventral striatum, amygdala and the orbitofrontal cortex), which were recruited in controls (Paradiso et al, 2003; Taylor et al, 2002, 2005).

While these studies do make a case for some kind of a reward abnormality in schizophrenia (eg Juckel et al, 2006b), they did not obtain any online objective measures of reward-learning, which could link the behavior and brain dysfunction to the disease. Therefore, in this study, we wanted to investigate whether patient's with schizophrenia showed aberrant learning when exposed to motivationally salient (aversive in this case) stimuli. We wanted to relate the neural BOLD findings to objective measures of learning galvanic skin responses (GSR) and subjective reports. Further, to avoid possible performance differences between patients and controls we used a simple Pavlovian conditioning paradigm. We hypothesized that patients would form abnormal associations, either displaying a decreased activation to the CS+ or an increased activation to the CS− resulting in an abnormal distinction between CS+ and CS−, and that these neural abnormalities will be associated with GSR and subjective deficits.

METHODS

Subjects

Eighteen patients with schizophrenia and 18 matched healthy controls gave written informed consent according to the guidelines of the Human Subjects Review Committee of the University of Toronto and completed the study. In an initial separate session before the scanning all subjects underwent interviews concerning their physical and psychiatric health history.

Inclusion criteria for patients were: (i) age 18–65; (ii) have a DSM-IV diagnosis of schizophrenia or schizoaffective disorder in the absence of a current major depressive or manic episode; (iii) accepting of care on a voluntary basis and capable of consenting to participation in the research study; (iv) able to undergo fMRI with a duration of approximately 50 min. Exclusion criteria for patients were: (i) serious, unstable medical illness or any concomitant major medical or neurological illness; (ii) acute suicidal and/or homicidal ideation; (iii) DSM-IV substance dependence (except caffeine and nicotine) within 3 months before entering the study; (iv) current major depressive episode or a manic episode; (v) metal implants or cardiac pacemaker; and (vi) used any illegal psychoactive drugs in the last two weeks or any painkillers or alcohol in the last 48 h. The criteria for control subjects were similar except that they never had suffered from any psychiatric illness. The patients and healthy subjects were matched on age, gender, education, and general cognitive estimates (Table 1).

Table 1 Group Description (Mean±SD)

Experimental Protocol

fMRI BOLD responses were examined in a classical passive Pavlovian learning paradigm involving aversive events randomly mixed with neutral events. The aversive events consisted of an 800 ms long loud fire-truck horn burst as unconditioned stimuli (US). The intensity of the US was individually titrated to where it was reported to be ‘unpleasant but tolerable’. Colored circles, which preceded the US by 5 s, served as conditioned stimuli (CS+) and predicted the US according to a 50% partial reinforcement schedule. Circles of another color that served as neutral comparators (CS−) were followed by a visual star in 50% of the events. Between trials a fixation cross was displayed for 9 s. Forty CS+ and 40 CS− trials were presented during the 18 min of the experimental session.

Apparatus

The subjects used an adjustable mirror located above their eyes to view the projected CS images on a screen placed at the foot of the scanner bed. Sound stimuli were delivered by the Commander XG FMRI Audio system (Resonance Technology Inc., Northridge, CA). The E-prime software (Psychology Software Tools, Inc., Pittsburg, PA) controlled the stimulus presentations. GSR were continuously monitored by PowerLab 2/20 (AD Instruments; Castle Hill, Australia) via long well-isolated rf-filtered cables through a wave guide. MRI compatible Ag/AgCl electrodes attached to the terminal phalanx on the left index and middle finger respectively were used.

Image Acquisition

MRI scans were acquired by a GE Signa 1.5 T scanner (General Electric, Waukesha, WI) equipped with a standard head coil. In a single session, 509 volumes (28 contiguous axial 4.4-mm-thick slices) covering the whole brain were acquired using a T2*-sensitive spiral in–out sequence (TR=2240 ms; TE=40 ms; flip angle 85°; matrix 64 × 64; FOV 200 × 200 mm). This sequence was chosen since it has reduced susceptibility dropout in sensitive areas (Glover and Thomason, 2004) including the ventral striatum. The first three volumes were discarded to allow for T1 equilibrium effects, and the data from the remaining 506 volumes were used in the analysis. For localization purposes, IR-Prepped 3D FSPGR T1-weighted anatomical images (120 contiguous axial 1.1-mm-thick slices) were acquired (TR=12 ms; TE=5.4 ms; flip angle 20°; matrix 256 × 256; FOV 200 × 200 mm).

Data Quality

The images were visually inspected for signal dropout due to magnetic susceptibility in the region of ventral striatum. The visual inspection revealed no signal dropout in this region. fMRI data from five patients and five controls could not be used: six scans were lost due to unsuccessful reconstruction or wrongly prescribed scanning parameters; one subject's structural scan indicated brain abnormalities and; three subjects moved more than the allowed 3 mm during the scan. Of the remaining 13 patients and 13 healthy controls, GSR data could not be obtained for three of the patients and two of the controls due to technical problems.

SPM2 analysis (http://www.fil.ion.ucl.ac.uk/spm)

All volumes were realigned to the first volume (Friston et al, 1995) and the anatomical image was coregistered to the mean functional image to ensure that they were aligned. The images were spatially normalized (Friston et al, 1995) to a standard EPI template (Evans et al, 1993), resampled at 3 × 3 × 3 mm and smoothed using a 10 mm full-width half-maximum (FWHM) isotropic kernel. Data were high-pass filtered using a cutoff value of 128 s. The data were analyzed by modeling four event types as stick functions convolved with a synthetic HRF. The four events consisted of the CS onsets, modelling the CS+ followed by the US and the CS+ not followed by the US separately as well as modelling CS− followed by a visual star and the CS− not followed by anything separately. The two regressors consisting of CS followed by US and a visual star were not used in any contrasts. For every subject three contrasts were specified: CS+, CS− and CS+ vs CS−. The individual contrast images were moved up to second-level random effects models. The between group random effects models investigated the effects of CS+, CS− and CS+ vs CS− respectively. For the healthy controls, we also made a within-group analysis to establish that the contrast [CS+ vs CS−] yielded activations in the ventral striatum similar to previous studies (Jensen et al, 2003). The data were thresholded at FDR<0.05 except for the CS+ vs CS− comparisons where small volume correction for regions of interest (ventral striatum) were used based on coordinates from a previous study (Jensen et al, 2003). We used spheres with a radius of 9 mm centered on MNI coordinates −8, 10, −2 for the left ventral striatum and 12, 6, −4 for the right ventral striatum.

GSR Recording and Analysis

The GSR was sampled at 10 Hz. To correct for possible MRI induced artifacts the GSR signal was digitally low-pass filtered using a cutoff value of 1 Hz. To determine the GSR, the peak amplitude value within the 10 s following the cue onset was taken and subtracted by the value at the CS onset. The frequency of values higher than 0.05 μS (Boucsein, 1992) was calculated for each of the four trial types modeled as above. For comparisons, Student's t-tests for related samples and repeated measures analysis of variance were performed.

Self Reports

Using a post-scan questionnaire, the subjects were asked to rate their degree of uneasiness when the CSs were shown. This was done on scales with four anchors (relaxed to extremely uneasy). In comparisons we used rank-order Wilcoxon test.

Correlations

Pearson or Spearman correlations of BOLD (β values in the ventral striatum) and symptoms (PANSS positive and PANSS negative), and medications were performed depending on distribution of data. For the medication correlation we used the dose from the eight subjects that were given Olanzapine instead of trying to convert all drugs to a standard drug.

Data Handling

For BOLD FMRI, and GSR data only the non-reinforced trials for both CS+ and CS− were used in contrasts.

RESULTS

Data from 13 medicated stable patients with schizophrenia and 13 matched healthy controls were analyzed. Group descriptions are displayed in Table 1.

Patients showed evidence of abnormal learning by self-report (Figure 1). The patients could not distinguish between CS+ vs CS− [0.50 vs 0.25; Z=1.13, p=NS] while the control subjects reported a clear subjective aversive response viewing the CS+ as compared to CS− [1.15 vs 0.08; Z=2.40, p<0.01].

Figure 1
figure 1

The left panel shows the subjective ratings of uneasiness for the conditions. The right panel displays the frequency of galvanic skin responses above threshold (ie >0.05 μS) for the conditions. The error bars refer to SE.

GSR, an autonomic measure of the level of arousal, were continuously recorded during the fMRI session and used as objective indices of conditioned responses. While both patients (p<0.05) and healthy controls (p<0.001) displayed more GSR above threshold to the CS+ as compared to CS−, a group × condition interaction [F(1,19)=10.92; p<0.01] was obtained suggesting that patients showed less GSR above threshold to CS+ but more GSR to CS− stimuli compared to controls (Figure 1). This is in keeping with the self-reported findings.

In the fMRI BOLD data we wanted to examine differences in responses to the CS+ and CS− between the patients and the controls. No differences between the groups in responses to the CS+ were obtained in any region of the brain after correcting for multiple comparisons. For the CS−, an activation in the ventral striatum (coordinate 6, 3, 0; Z=4.14; Figure 2) was obtained for the patients with schizophrenia compared to the controls after correcting for multiple comparisons (pFDR<0.05). Clusters surviving corrections are reported in Table 2. There were no activations in the striatum or in any other brain region that were stronger among controls compared to the patients for the CS− condition after correction for multiple comparisons.

Figure 2
figure 2

A statistical parametric map showing the significantly larger effect in the ventral striatum in patients with schizophrenia compared to healthy controls for the CS−. The colors refer to t-values. N=13 in each group.

Table 2 Activated Regions in Response to CS−; Patients>Controls

A one-sample t-test was performed in the healthy controls showing an activation in the ventral striatum for the contrast [CS+>CS−] at peak coordinate 6, 9, 0 (Z=2.92; p<0.05). Two-sample t-tests were used to test for differences in brain activations for the CS+ vs the CS− between the patients with schizophrenia and the healthy controls. Searching a small volume centered at previously reported coordinates (Jensen et al, 2003), an activation in the ventral striatum (peak at coordinate −6, 12, 3; Z=3.08;

p<0.05) for the healthy controls as compared to the patients were obtained (Figure 3). An examination of the β values (Figure 3) showed that this difference was driven by a much larger response to the CS− among the patients while both patients and controls had similar responses to CS+. To examine the generalizability of the results, we analyzed the data using a small volume search centered on the co-ordinates reported by Juckel et al (coordinate −15, 11, −6) (Juckel et al, 2006b) which again yielded the same findings, that is significant peak activation at co-ordinate −6, 12, 3.

Figure 3
figure 3

The left panel shows the β values for the conditions in the left ventral striatum (at coordinates (−6, 12, 3) where the error bars refer to SE. The right panel is a statistical parametric map in the ventral striatum (Y=12) for the contrast (CS+>CS−) in healthy controls compared to patients with schizophrenia. The figure suggests that the patients have a response above the mean to the CS− while healthy controls are under the mean. In this voxel the response to CS+ is similar in the groups. The colors refer to t-values.

No significant correlations between symptoms (PANSS positive, N=13; r=0.18; p=NS, or PANSS negative, N=13; r=−0.37; p=NS) or medication dose (N=8; ρ=−0.17; p=NS) and BOLD responses for CS+ were obtained among the patients. Neither were there any associations between symptoms (PANSS positive, N=13; r=0.10; p=NS, or PANSS negative, N=13; r=−0.13; p=NS) or medication dose (N=8; ρ=−0.10; p=NS) and BOLD responses for CS−. Exploratory correlation analyses were done to investigate if there were any associations between self reported uneasiness to CS−, GSR to the CS−, and BOLD responses to the CS− in the ventral striatum among the patients. No significant associations were obtained.

DISCUSSION

The main finding here is that patients with schizophrenia, when exposed to neutral stimuli in a threatening situation, show an abnormal pattern of learning. Their ability to distinguish (subjectively as measured with self report and at a physiological level as measured with GSR) between neutral stimuli (CS−) and stimuli truly linked to danger or aversive outcomes (CS+) is impaired. Further, neutral stimuli come to be associated with significant conditioned brain activations among the patients—an abnormality that was accompanied with abnormal physiological (GSR) and explicit learning (by self-report).

The ventral striatum plays a central role in reward processing and it has been suggested that activations of the ventral striatum normally mediate the incentive or motivational salience of environmental stimuli (Jensen et al, 2003, 2007; Zink et al, 2003, 2004, 2006). The stronger responses to the neutral stimulus among patients may reflect an aberrant attribution of motivational salience to neutral stimuli (Kapur, 2003). As a result, stimuli that are neutral and innocuous among healthy controls may gain a status as motivationally salient among patients with schizophrenia. In line with this finding, a recent study using a word classification paradigm showed that deluded patients with schizophrenia displayed a bias towards classifying words as unpleasant, a misattribution of salience, compared to non-deluded patients and healthy controls (Holt et al, 2006b). It has been proposed that positive symptoms (such as delusions and hallucinations) are the product of abnormally reinforced thoughts and associations, and the current study could serve as a model of how context-inappropriate associations are reinforced (Kapur, 2003; King et al, 1984; Miller, 1984; Shaner, 1999).

However, a number of caveats should be added to such a proposal—First, ours is a cross-sectional study in patients who had pre-existing delusions and thus we are unable to resolve the causal contribution of these aberrant associations. Second, we did not find a reliable association between the psychotic symptoms and aberrant activations, though this may have been limited by the small sample size and the fact that the antipsychotics had muted the psychosis. Also, the presence of the antipsychotic medication introduces a possible confound that the aberrant pattern seen may be a product of the dopamine block introduced by antipsychotics. Recently it was shown that patients treated with atypical antipsychotics activated the ventral striatum whereas patients on typical antipsychotics failed to recruit this region during a monetary incentive task (Juckel et al, 2006a). It should be noted that 11 out of 13 patients in the current study were treated with atypical medication and one patient was prescribed both types suggesting that the formation of abnormal associations might be due to the disease rather than medication. However, to address this question a study using drug-naïve patients must be performed.

The study adds to a growing body of literature relating to reward abnormalities in schizophrenia, although the previous literature does not tell a simple story. Using differing paradigms, some studies find reduced reward learning in conditioning experiments in patients with schizophrenia (Hofer et al, 2001; Kosmidis et al, 1999; Lynn, 1963), others describe enhanced reward learning in schizophrenia (Sears et al, 2000; Spain, 1966), and yet others find no differences between patients and controls (Howe, 1958). Latent inhibition (LI), a model of reward learning that requires subjects to ignore irrelevant stimuli and where learning can be measured objectively using GSR has been used to provide a more objective index of reward learning. Acute unmedicated patients show a disruption of LI (ie evidence for aberrant reward learning) whereas chronic and medicated patients show normal LI (Gray et al, 1997; Vaitl et al, 2002). Most earlier imaging studies on reward in schizophrenia have focused on the affect generated by the rewarding experience rather than reward learning. Patients have been reported to rate the hedonic value of pleasant odors lower than controls and fail to activate limbic/paralimbic structures invoked by normal controls (Crespo-Facorro et al, 2001). Studies using positive and negative pictures as rewarding stimuli (Paradiso et al, 2003; Taylor et al, 2002, 2005) also showed that patients failed to recruit parts of reward related circuits. These earlier imaging studies on reward in schizophrenia have focused on the failure to activate structures during reward per se whereas the current study suggest an aberrant activation pattern towards cues predicting neutral stimuli in a reward learning paradigm. In line with the current findings, an elevated activity in hippocampus and amygdala has been shown in schizophrenia during passive viewing of human faces, displaying both neutral and negative emotions (Holt et al, 2006a).

While there are few previous studies that have examined the formation of reward associations using functional neuroimaging, the study that comes closest in its methods and design is by Juckel and collaborators (Juckel et al, 2006b), who found a negative correlation between the severity of negative symptoms and activation of the ventral striatum. In the current study no significant correlations were obtained between activations in the ventral striatum and psychopathological symptoms. A reason for this might be that the patients in the current study were less symptomatic compared to the sample in the study of Juckel et al. A direct comparison with the study by Juckel and collaborators is difficult since they did not report any behavioral data or fMRI β values for the different conditions. Also, there are two major differences between the studies: (1) Juckel et al used a monetary paradigm while the current study used primary aversive stimulations and; (2) they used drug-free patients while the patients in the current study all were stable on medications and displayed less psychopathological symptoms. Nonetheless, Juckel et al (2006b) also showed that patients with schizophrenia displayed reduced ventral striatal activation when exposed to reward predicting vs relative neutral cues, a finding that is consistent with our observation that the aberrant distinction between CS+ and CS− may be driven by abnormal activations in the ventral striatum in response to CS−.

In summary, among patients with schizophrenia neutral stimuli come to be associated with significant conditioned brain activations—an abnormal formation of associations. This neural abnormality was accompanied with changes in physiological measures and subjective reports which support the idea of aberrant conditioning. Future studies should examine whether such abnormalities precede the onset of psychosis and how treatment modifies them.