The American Psychiatric Association (APA) has updated its Privacy Policy and Terms of Use, including with new information specifically addressed to individuals in the European Economic Area. As described in the Privacy Policy and Terms of Use, this website utilizes cookies, including for the purpose of offering an optimal online experience and services tailored to your preferences.

Please read the entire Privacy Policy and Terms of Use. By closing this message, browsing this website, continuing the navigation, or otherwise continuing to use the APA's websites, you confirm that you understand and accept the terms of the Privacy Policy and Terms of Use, including the utilization of cookies.

×

Abstract

OBJECTIVE: Previous research in adults implicates attention bias in posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD). To study attention bias in children, the authors used picture-based versions of the visual-probe attention bias task previously used with adults. They tested the hypothesis that attention bias to threatening facial photographs is associated with maltreatment and PTSD. METHOD: A visual-probe task that manipulated threat levels was used to test 34 children who had been maltreated and 21 children who had not been maltreated. The visual-probe task involved showing photographs of actors with faces depicting neutral, angry/threatening, or happy expressions for 500 msec each. RESULTS: Attention bias away from threat was associated with severity of physical abuse and diagnosis of PTSD. This association reflected the tendency for high levels of abuse or PTSD to predict attention avoidance of threatening faces. CONCLUSIONS: Previous studies examined the engagement of specific brain regions associated with attention orientation to angry/threatening faces. The current study used similar methods to document associations between attention bias and maltreatment in children. This sets the stage for studies examining relationships in children among perturbed brain function, psychopathology, attention bias, and maltreatment.

The disrupting effects of threat cues on attention represents one of the strongest neuropsychological correlates of adult anxiety disorders (13). Depending on methodological or clinical factors, disruption in attention can manifest as a directing of attention either toward or away from threat stimuli. The influence of threat cues on attention has been termed “threat-related attention bias.” This threat bias indicates that the presence of threatening material produces a potentially dysfunctional change in attention allocation. Such dysfunction is found in most anxiety disorders, although associations appear particularly strong in posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) (1, 3).

Much of the research on attention bias to threat used one of two paradigms, the emotional Stroop task (1), which assesses interfering effects of threat on a concurrently performed nonemotional task, and the visual-probe task (2), which assesses effects of threat on attention orientation. Particular interest has emerged in the visual-probe task, based on extensive cognitive neuroscience research using visual orienting tasks coupled with data suggesting that the visual-probe task taps aspects of the underlying risk for anxiety (48).

The current study is designed to address the need for more research on attention bias in children that directly extends previous research in adults. Adult anxiety disorders have been conceptualized as conditions that emerge gradually during development (9, 10), based on data linking adult anxiety to high levels of childhood anxiety symptoms and to adversity during childhood. Given that threat bias is thought to emerge early in the course of anxiety disorders, it is vital to extend to children available research paradigms currently used to study adults. Findings of a few preliminary studies (1113) suggest that biases in attention occur in pediatric anxiety disorders, including PTSD. However, few studies examined children with clinically significant PTSD or children exposed to extreme stress in general or high levels of maltreatment in particular. Recent findings in adults (14, 15) suggest that extreme childhood domestic physical or sexual abuse shows particularly strong associations with measures of abnormal brain function and PTSD. However, these findings derive from retrospective reports in adults; data are needed regarding the associations among extreme stress, PTSD, and threat bias in children.

The few available studies that have examined this issue used words to manipulate degrees of threat. The use of verbal stimuli complicates interpretations, since developmental or cognitive influences on reading facility may confound efforts to manipulate threat in children (16). Similar confounds may arise related to developmental changes in the ease with which emotion can be induced by abstract verbal representations of threat (17).

Studies in adults have implemented picture-based visual-probe tasks to quantify effects of threatening stimuli on attention (2, 18, 19). Such paradigms manipulate threat by using angry faces, a potentially more ecologically valid stimulus set already employed in developmental psychopathology research (16). Specifically, considerable previous research documents abnormal responses to angry facial displays in maltreated children (2022). This includes a reduced threshold for anger detection, disruption in attention from angry faces, and greater cognitive interference from angry faces. Nevertheless, these studies generally have not used face-processing tasks to examine maltreated children with PTSD, nor have they employed tasks previously used in adults.

The use of picture-based visual probe tasks may facilitate efforts to integrate research on adult anxiety, pediatric anxiety, and cognitive neuroscience, given that many recent neuroscience studies used evocative facial displays to study emotion (6, 23–25). These studies found that angry faces are capable of capturing attention, evoking emotion, and engaging brain regions involved in interactions between cognitive and emotional processes (24). In research with children, the most consistent previous findings document associations between abnormalities in the processing of angry faces and maltreatment, independent of psychiatric diagnosis (22). Therefore, the current study tests the hypothesis that increasingly high levels of childhood maltreatment specifically are associated with increasingly abnormal threat-related attention bias. Moreover, since studies in adults relate attention bias to PTSD, the current study also tests the hypothesis that PTSD in children is associated with threat bias. We tested these hypotheses using a visual-probe paradigm previously used in adults (26, 27). This facilitates efforts to integrate research in children and adults on the relationship between maltreatment and attention orientation.

Method

Subjects

Subjects were drawn from a larger study on trauma (Permanency Planning: Service Use and Child Outcomes; Joan Kaufman, Ph.D., principal investigator). The group of participants comprised 34 children 7–13 years old who had been maltreated and 21 children in the same age range who had not been maltreated. All maltreated children had been exposed to severe domestic abuse, defined according to standardized procedures described later in this article. All of the maltreated children had been removed from their homes for ethical and legal reasons because of the extreme nature of the abuse. The decision to remove the child was made by the State of Connecticut Department of Children and Family Services. In all cases, the state had court-awarded custody.

Comparison subjects met three criteria: no history of a maltreatment report in the Department of Children and Families; no evidence of maltreatment based on a standard assessment of maltreatment, described later in this article; and household income of $30,000 or less.

Procedures

Both the Yale University Human Investigations Committee and the State of Connecticut Department of Children and Families Institutional Review Board approved this study. Department of Children and Families caseworkers described the study to birth mothers of maltreated children and obtained permission for the research staff to contact the birth mother. Ninety-five percent of the women who were approached by the caseworkers agreed to participate. Community comparison subjects were recruited through advertisements. Clinical measures were collected within 2 weeks of the neuropsychological measures.

Psychopathology

Self-reports of anxiety symptoms were obtained by using the Screen for Child Anxiety Related Emotional Disorders (28), which consists of written statements about anxiety symptoms that children indicate either applied or did not apply to them. Teacher reports of psychopathology were acquired by using the Child Behavior Checklist Teacher Report Form (29), a standardized measure including 112 statements that a teacher rates as either applying or not applying to the student. A structured psychiatric interview (the Schedule for Affective Disorders and Schizophrenia for School-Age Children—Present and Lifetime Version [K-SADS-PL] [30]) was administered to each child and a parental informant.

Final diagnoses were derived by using the best-estimate procedure (31), which involved a review of all clinical material during a multidisciplinary team meeting chaired by an expert in childhood maltreatment and assessment of childhood psychopathology (J.K.). Clinical material included results from self-reports, teacher reports, and the structured interview. Final diagnoses were assigned by consensus agreement at this meeting. For PTSD, children were diagnosed as either definitely (N=22) or probably (N=7) affected. The probable diagnosis was based on the presence of impairment and at least 85% of the required symptoms.

Maltreatment

Information on maltreatment was collected from multiple sources. Complete Department of Children and Families case records were reviewed. Birth parents completed a standardized questionnaire assessing level of domestic violence (32). The PTSD section of the K-SADS-PL, administered to both children and parents, surveyed a range of traumas. Children’s self-reports also were obtained (33), and additional incidents of maltreatment were reported during contact with children and families.

To reduce the number of statistical tests, the data from these five sources were synthesized by using established procedures to derive two 4-point (0=no maltreatment; 3=extreme maltreatment) scales (32, 34). Based on results from previous studies on face perception (20, 21), one scale rated severity of physical abuse and the other rated severity of physical neglect (interrater reliability [intraclass condition (ICC)] for physical abuse was 0.87; ICC for neglect was 0.84).

Attention Bias

A visual-probe paradigm was used that had been employed previously in research among adults (27). Stimuli were identical to those used to study adults. Stimuli were presented by using the E-Prime software program (Psychology Software Tools, Pittsburgh) on a laptop computer in a darkened room. Subjects sat 50 cm from the computer. Testers were blind to all clinical data. The task relied on photographs of face-pairs from 64 different actors (half of whom were men), each presenting two facial expressions, one neutral and the other either angry/threatening or happy. The photographs were selected because they displayed particularly characteristic threatening or happy poses (27).

The task began with 32 practice trials followed by 80 trials presented in random order; reaction time data were collected during these trials. For the critical 64 trials, pairs of pictures from the same actor were presented to the right or left of the screen, one picture displaying an emotional expression and the other displaying a neutral expression. These 64 trials included 32 threatening trials and 32 happy trials; one-half of the trials displayed the emotional expression on the right, and the other displayed the emotional expression on the left. An additional 16 trials displayed pairs of neutral faces. Each trial began with a 500-msec centrally presented fixation cross and was followed by a face-pair presented for 500 msec. Immediately after, a single-asterisk “dot-probe” was presented for 1100 msec on the left or right side of the screen. Subjects were instructed to press one of two keys as quickly and as accurately as possible to indicate the location of the probe (right versus left). The intertrial interval varied randomly between 750 and 1250 msec.

Data Analysis

As previously described (19, 27), trials with errors were discarded, as were trials with latencies <200 msec or more than two standard deviations above each participant’s mean latency. Error rates are presented in Table 1. Less than 10% of trials involved latencies <200 msec or more than two standard deviations above the mean latency.

We entered the study with clear a priori hypotheses based on previous data relating clinical measures to measures of attention bias to threat. As a result, analyses were based on derived measures of attention bias. These were calculated by using a standard formula (11, 27, 35) that subtracts, for each participant, the mean reaction time on trials where the emotion face and probe appeared on the same side of the display from the mean reaction time on trials where the emotion face and probe appeared on the opposite side of the display. Positive bias scores reflect the tendency to monitor the emotional stimulus, and negative bias scores reflect the tendency to avoid the emotional stimulus.

The primary analyses examined attention bias for threatening faces. Secondary analyses examined attention bias for happy faces, reaction times on neutral face trials, and errors in probe location identification. The relationship between attention bias and maltreatment was examined in the initial set of analyses, and the relationship between attention bias and PTSD was examined in the second set of analyses. These associations were examined by using independent-sample t tests, analysis of covariance (ANCOVA), and Pearson correlations. Since we entered the study with a priori hypotheses relating attention bias to maltreatment and PTSD, statistical inference relies on two-tailed tests, alpha set at 0.05, without correction for multiple comparisons.

Results

Characteristics of the Subjects

Table 1 summarizes the characteristics of the subjects. As shown in Table 1, physically and emotionally maltreated children exhibited higher anxiety symptoms, higher rates of PTSD, and more errors in probe location identification. No differences emerged in their reaction times to neutral-neutral probe trials. Two subjects were taking psychotropic medications. Since results appeared similar with these subjects excluded, data from these subjects were retained in statistical models.

Among the 29 maltreated children with probable or definite PTSD, 21 (72%) had a history of physical abuse, five (17%) sexual abuse, and 20 (69%) neglect. Many experienced severe maltreatment; all sexually abused children were subjected to either oral-genital contact or vaginal and/or anal intercourse. Approximately one-third of the maltreated children were exposed to domestic violence involving substantial injuries to their mothers; approximately one-fifth required medical care for acts of abuse producing injuries such as fractures; approximately one-third had been abandoned.

Attention Bias

The association between level of abuse and attention bias was examined by using both categorical and continuous indicators of abuse severity. For continuous measures, correlations were examined between measures of attention bias and physical abuse or maltreatment. The correlation between threat bias and severity of physical maltreatment was significant (r=–0.40, N=34, p=0.02), but the correlation between threat bias and severity of neglect was not significant (r=–0.15). The significant correlation reflects the fact that more severely physically abused children tended to direct attention away from angry/threatening faces. No associations arose between bias for happy faces and either physical abuse or maltreatment.

For categorical measures, the association between abuse severity and attention bias was examined further by comparing attention bias in children divided into a high-physical-abuse group—children with severe physical abuse (N=21)—and a low-physical-abuse group (N=34)—comprising children with no history of physical abuse (N=21) or minimal history of physical abuse (N=13). This designation was based on ratings from the 4-point physical abuse scale: ratings of 0 and 1 indicated low levels of abuse and ratings of 2 or 3 indicated high levels of abuse. This analysis found that the 21 children in the high-physical-abuse group exhibited significantly greater avoidance of threatening faces (mean=–22 msec, SD=48) than the 34 children in the low-physical-abuse group (mean=12 msec, SD=43) (t=2.7, df=53, p=0.009).

To examine associations with PTSD diagnosis, children were divided into two groups—abused children with PTSD (N=29) and children with no history of abuse and no psychiatric diagnosis (N=17). For this analysis, four children who had not been abused but who had psychopathology were excluded, as were five children who had been abused but did not have PTSD. Because children with PTSD tended to be older, differences in threat bias were examined by using ANCOVA, with age and PTSD status as predictors. This analysis found significantly greater avoidance of threat in abused children with PTSD (mean=–10 msec, SD=46) than in nonabused children with no psychiatric diagnosis (mean=16 msec, SD=43) (F=5.1, df=1, 44, p=0.03). Age also significantly predicted threat bias, with greater avoidance of threat in younger children (F=5.5, df=1, 44, p=0.02). Since 20 of the 22 severely abused children met criteria for PTSD (χ2=18, df=1, p<0.001), too few subjects were available to contrast threat bias meaningfully in children cross-classified with respect to both severe abuse status and PTSD diagnosis. Threat bias exhibited no association with anxiety symptoms determined by scores on the Screen for Child Anxiety Related Emotional Disorders.

Discussion

The main finding in this study is an association between maltreatment and attention avoidance of threatening faces in 34 children who had been abused. Attention avoidance of threat also was associated with a diagnosis of PTSD. However, since a large majority of the children who had experienced severe abuse also met criteria for PTSD, we could not determine whether associations with attention avoidance reflect primarily relationships with severity of abuse independent of PTSD or the presence of PTSD.

Previous studies of maltreated children document distortions in information processing of angry facial displays. These distortions include a hostile attributional bias, manifest as a lower threshold for detecting anger in ambiguous facial displays and hostility in social situations (36), as well as high rates of emotion misidentification, attention bias, and greater interference in concurrent psychological processes (20). However, these studies of children generally did not employ paradigms used previously in adults with a history of psychopathology or maltreatment. Moreover, these studies did not examine children with known diagnoses of PTSD or children exposed to levels of domestic abuse of sufficient severity to necessitate removal of the subject from the home.

The current study demonstrates associations among PTSD, trauma, and attention away from angry faces using a paradigm already used extensively in research on adult psychopathology and cognitive neuroscience. This allows a tighter integration of findings from the fields of developmental psychopathology, adult psychiatry, and cognitive neuroscience.

Interpretation of the current findings can be placed in the context of an extensive set of previous studies of anxiety using the visual-probe paradigm. A few studies have used verbal stimuli to examine attention bias for threat in different pediatric anxiety disorders, including PTSD (11, 12, 37, 38). Although findings remain somewhat inconsistent, children with anxiety symptoms or disorders have been shown to differ from both psychiatrically healthy and depressed children. Nevertheless, effect sizes were relatively small, and the exclusive reliance on verbal stimuli complicates interpretations. A larger set of studies examined adult anxiety disorders, including a growing series of studies using angry facial displays or other evocative photographs (2, 18, 19, 26, 27, 35). In fact, these paradigms have been used in adults to examine the relationship between attention bias for threat and anxiety, although not specifically PTSD (19, 26, 27). In these studies, the most consistent finding is an association between high levels of trait anxiety and an attention bias toward threat stimuli, including angry facial expressions. Nevertheless, other studies have found the opposite result in adult social phobia, manifest as bias away from threat stimuli (39).

The current study found that high levels of maltreatment or a diagnosis of PTSD was associated with an attention bias away from threat stimuli. Multiple explanations might account for differences between the current results and previous studies in adult anxiety disorders. For example, the current paradigms have not been used in research on adult PTSD or interpersonal violence. Some anxiety disorders, such as generalized anxiety disorder, may be associated with a bias toward angry faces, whereas others, such as PTSD or social phobia, may be associated with a bias away from angry faces (39). Regardless, results from both the current study in maltreated children and previous studies in anxious adults consistently use angry faces to document associations between anxiety and attention bias. This emphasizes the need to conduct integrative studies using parallel methods in children and adults to clarify the nature of these associations across diagnoses and age groups.

The current study should be considered in the light of four limitations. First, for ethical reasons, it was not feasible to obtain data on attention bias in a group of children recently exposed to severe domestic abuse who had not changed residence. Current clinical and legal guidelines require health care professionals who identify such children to notify appropriate authorities. In conditions where the presence of severe abuse is confirmed by authorities, as for the children in the current study, clinical and legal guidelines require that such children be removed from their home. As a result, in the current study, as in other studies of children exposed to recent extreme domestic abuse, abuse severity is confounded by removal from the home. Therefore, associations in the current study for attention bias may reflect the combined association with severe maltreatment and removal from the home. To disentangle these possibilities, future studies might consider implementing novel research designs. For example, a study might examine attention bias in nonabused children who changed residence for reasons unrelated to any history of maltreatment.

Second, children were recruited from child protective service agencies. This method has the advantage of selecting children with verified histories of severe domestic abuse and minimizing conflicts that arise when maltreatment is detected in research participants previously unfamiliar to protective service agencies. Nevertheless, biases could influence referral patterns. Although children were matched on relevant demographic variables, other unknown biases may exist. Hence, the current findings may apply to particular populations of referred children. Third, the study examined a relatively small number of subjects. Moreover, because the large majority of maltreated children had a history of PTSD, the degree to which attention bias relates specifically to PTSD as opposed to severe maltreatment, independent of PTSD symptoms, remains unclear. Nevertheless, these findings are important to pursue, given that previous studies generally documenting associations between maltreatment and processing of angry social cues have not examined associations with PTSD (20, 36).

Finally, the current study represents only an initial step in examining the relationship between anxiety and underlying dysfunction in brain circuits associated with the threat response. Future neuroimaging research might evaluate the role of such dysfunction in both attention bias and associated clinical symptoms.

This study lays the groundwork for subsequent research on neurodevelopmental aspects of attention bias as it relates to clinical anxiety and maltreatment (15). Studies in adults suggest that threatening faces modulate attention through effects on underlying brain regions previously implicated in mood and anxiety disorders (5, 23, 24). These include the anterior cingulate gyrus, amygdala, and ventral or medial aspects of the prefrontal cortex. Pharmacological studies suggest that activity in these brain regions can be altered by manipulations of chemical systems previously implicated in mood and anxiety disorders (5). Therefore, the current paradigms might be used to generate insights on the relationships among anxiety, trauma, and underlying dysfunction in neural systems engaged by threat stimuli. Specifically, trauma has been hypothesized to alter functioning in brain structures that modulate attention engagement during threat presentation (6, 15). The current study documents an association between attention bias and maltreatment using a paradigm previously shown to engage the relevant brain structures (5). Future research might examine these associations within the context of an imaging study. Moreover, the current findings document associations between attention bias and PTSD in traumatized children. Future studies might examine resilient children, consider the effects of treatment, or examine the longitudinal nature of associations among attention bias, trauma, and PTSD. Such studies might clarify the degree to which attention bias relates over time to vulnerability to stress-related psychopathology.

Finally, this study also may lay the groundwork for future clinical studies. Avoidance of threatening facial displays may contribute to repetitive dysfunctional patterns in the social lives of traumatized children. Avoidance of threatening facial photographs in the current study may show parallels to aspects of behavior during real-life social interactions. If abnormalities in attention orientation are ultimately shown to contribute to the sequelae of trauma, this observation may carry novel therapeutic insights. For example, novel treatments might specifically target underlying abnormalities in attention orientation as a means of affecting social functioning.

TABLE 1

Received April 19, 2003; revision received Oct. 15, 2003; accepted April 9, 2004. From the Section on Development and Affective Neuroscience and the Section on Experimental Therapeutics, National Institute of Mental Health Intramural Research Program; the Child and Adolescent Research and Education Program, Department of Psychiatry, Yale University School of Medicine, New Haven, Conn.; the National Center for Posttraumatic Stress Disorder, Veterans Administration, West Haven, Conn.; and the School of Psychology, University of Southampton, United Kingdom. Address correspondence and reprint requests to Dr. Pine, NIMH, Bldg. 15-K, Rm. 110, MSC-2670, Bethesda, MD 20817-2670; (e-mail). Supported in part by a grant from the Department of Children and Families (Dr. Kaufman), by the State of Connecticut, and by the National Center for Posttraumatic Stress Disorder. The authors thank the Department of Children and Families staff and the children and families who made this work possible.

References

1. Williams JM, Mathews A, MacLeod C: The emotional Stroop task and psychopathology. Psychol Bull 1996; 120:3–24Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

2. Mogg K, Bradley BP: A cognitive-motivational analysis of anxiety. Behav Res Ther 1998; 36:809–848Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

3. McNally RJ: Cognitive bias in the anxiety disorders. Nebr Symp Motiv 1996; 43:211–250MedlineGoogle Scholar

4. Posner MI: A framework for relating cognitive to neural systems. Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol Suppl 1986; 38:155–166MedlineGoogle Scholar

5. Coull JT: Neural correlates of attention and arousal: insights from electrophysiology, functional neuroimaging and psychopharmacology. Prog Neurobiol 1998; 55:343–361Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

6. Davis M, Whalen PJ: The amygdala: vigilance and emotion. Mol Psychiatry 2001; 6:13–34Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

7. Armony JL, Dolan RJ: Modulation of spatial attention by fear-conditioned stimuli: an event-related fMRI study. Neuropsychologia 2002; 40:817–826Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

8. MacLeod C, Rutherford E, Campbell L, Ebsworthy G, Holker L: Selective attention and emotional vulnerability: assessing the causal basis of their association through the experimental manipulation of attentional bias. J Abnorm Psychol 2002; 111:107–123Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

9. Pine DS, Cohen P, Gurley D, Brook J, Ma Y: The risk for early-adulthood anxiety and depressive disorders in adolescents with anxiety and depressive disorders. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1998; 55:56–64Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

10. Costello EJ, Pine DS, Hammen C, March JS, Plotsky PM, Weissman MM, Biederman J, Goldsmith HH, Kaufman J, Lewinsohn PM, Hellander M, Hoagwood K, Koretz DS, Nelson CA, Leckman JF: Development and natural history of mood disorders. Biol Psychiatry 2002; 52:529–542Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

11. Dalgleish T, Moradi AR, Taghavi MR, Neshat-Doost HT, Yule W: An experimental investigation of hypervigilance for threat in children and adolescents with post-traumatic stress disorder. Psychol Med 2001; 31:541–547Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

12. Vasey MW, el-Hag N, Daleiden EL: Anxiety and the processing of emotionally threatening stimuli: distinctive patterns of selective attention among high- and low-test-anxious children. Child Dev 1996; 67:1173–1185Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

13. Klein RG, Pine DS: Anxiety disorders, in Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 4th ed. Edited by Rutter M, Taylor E, Hersov L. Oxford, UK, Blackwell, 2002, pp 486–509Google Scholar

14. Kaufman J, Charney D: Effects of early stress on brain structure and function: implications for understanding the relationship between child maltreatment and depression. Dev Psychopathol 2001; 13:451–471Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

15. Pine DS: Developmental psychobiology and response to threats: relevance to trauma in children and adolescents. Biol Psychiatry 2003; 53:796–808Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

16. Nelson CA, Bloom FE, Cameron JL, Amaral D, Dahl RE, Pine D: An integrative, multidisciplinary approach to the study of brain-behavior relations in the context of typical and atypical development. Dev Psychopathol 2002; 14:499–520Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

17. Stattin H: Developmental trends in the appraisal of anxiety-provoking situations. J Pers 1984; 52:46–57Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

18. Mogg K, Millar N, Bradley BP: Biases in eye movements to threatening facial expressions in generalized anxiety disorder and depressive disorder. J Abnorm Psychol 2000; 109:695–704Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

19. Bradley BP, Mogg K, White J, Groom C, de Bono J: Attentional bias for emotional faces in generalized anxiety disorder. Br J Clin Psychol 1999; 38(part 3):267–278Google Scholar

20. Pollak SD, Sinha P: Effects of early experience on children’s recognition of facial displays of emotion. Dev Psychol 2002; 38:784–791Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

21. Pollak SD, Kistler DJ: Early experience is associated with the development of categorical representations for facial expressions of emotion. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 2002; 99:9072–9076Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

22. Pollak SD, Tolley-Schell SA: Selective attention to facial emotion in physically abused children. J Abnorm Psychol 2003; 112:323–338Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

23. Dolan RJ: Emotion, cognition, and behavior. Science 2002; 298:1191–1194Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

24. Haxby JV, Hoffman EA, Gobbini MI: Human neural systems for face recognition and social communication. Biol Psychiatry 2002; 51:59–67Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

25. Pessoa L, McKenna M, Gutierrez E, Ungerleider LG: Neural processing of emotional faces requires attention. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 2002; 99:11458–11463Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

26. Mogg K, Bradley BP: Selective orienting of attention to masked threat faces in social anxiety. Behav Res Ther 2002; 40:1403–1414Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

27. Mogg K, Bradley BP: Some methodological issues in assessing attentional biases for threatening faces in anxiety: a replication study using a modified version of the probe detection task. Behav Res Ther 1999; 37:595–604Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

28. Birmaher B, Khetarpal S, Brent D, Cully M, Balach L, Kaufman J, Neer SM: The Screen for Child Anxiety Related Emotional Disorders (SCARED): scale construction and psychometric characteristics. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 1997; 36:545–553Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

29. Achenbach TM: Manual for the Child Behavior Checklist/4–18 and 1991 Profile. Burlington, University of Vermont, Department of Psychiatry, 1991Google Scholar

30. Kaufman J, Birmaher B, Brent D, Rao U, Flynn C, Moreci P, Williamson D, Ryan N: Schedule for Affective Disorders and Schizophrenia for School-Age Children—Present and Lifetime Version (K-SADS-PL): initial reliability and validity data. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 1997; 36:980–988Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

31. Leckman JF, Sholomskas D, Thompson WD, Belanger A, Weissman MM: Best estimate of lifetime psychiatric diagnosis: a methodological study. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1982; 39:879–883Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

32. Kaufman J: Depressive disorders in maltreated children. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 1991; 30:257–265Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

33. Bernstein DP, Ahluvalia T, Pogge D, Handelsman L: Validity of the Childhood Trauma Questionnaire in an adolescent psychiatric population. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 1997; 36:340–348Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

34. Barnett D, Manly JT, Cicchetti D: Defining child maltreatment: the interface between policy and research, in Child Abuse, Child Development, and Social Policy. Edited by Cicchetti D, Toth SL. Norwood, NJ, Ablex, 1993, pp 7–73Google Scholar

35. Mogg K, Bradley BP, Hyare H, Lee S: Selective attention to food-related stimuli in hunger: are attentional biases specific to emotional and psychopathological states, or are they also found in normal drive states? Behav Res Ther 1998; 36:227–237; correction, 36:1011Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

36. Dodge KA: Social-cognitive mechanisms in the development of conduct disorder and depression. Annu Rev Psychol 1993; 44:559–584Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

37. Daleiden EL, Vasey MW: An information-processing perspective on childhood anxiety. Clin Psychol Rev 1997; 17:407–429Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

38. Moradi AR, Taghavi MR, Neshat Doost HT, Yule W, Dalgleish T: Performance of children and adolescents with PTSD on the Stroop colour-naming task. Psychol Med 1999; 29:415–419Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar

39. Chen YP, Ehlers A, Clark DM, Mansell W: Patients with generalized social phobia direct their attention away from faces. Behav Res Ther 2002; 40:677–687Crossref, MedlineGoogle Scholar