Skip to main content

Space use and habitat selection of an invasive mesopredator and sympatric, native apex predator

Abstract

Background

Where mesopredators co-exist with dominant apex predators, an understanding of the factors that influence their habitat and space use can provide insights that help guide wildlife conservation and pest management actions. A predator’s habitat use is defined by its home range, which is influenced by its selection or avoidance of habitat features and intra- and inter-specific interactions within the landscape. These are driven by both innate and learned behaviour, operating at different spatial scales. We examined the seasonal home ranges and habitat selection of actively-managed populations of a native apex predator (dingo Canis dingo) and invasive mesopredator (feral cat Felis catus) in semi-arid Western Australia to better understanding their sympatric landscape use, potential interactions, and to help guide their management.

Methods

We used kernel density estimates to characterise the seasonal space use of dingoes and feral cats, investigate inter- and intra-species variation in their home range extent and composition, and examine second-order habitat selection for each predator. Further, we used discrete choice modelling and step selection functions to examine the difference in third-order habitat selection across several habitat features.

Results

The seasonal home ranges of dingoes were on average 19.5 times larger than feral cats. Feral cat seasonal home ranges typically included a larger proportion of grasslands than expected relative to availability in the study site, indicating second-order habitat selection for grasslands. In their fine-scale movements (third-order habitat selection), both predators selected for roads, hydrological features (seasonal intermittent streams, seasonal lakes and wetlands), and high vegetation cover. Dingoes also selected strongly for open woodlands, whereas feral cats used open woodlands and grasslands in proportion to availability.

Management recommendations

Based on these results, and in order to avoid unintended negative ecological consequences (e.g. mesopredator release) that may stem from non-selective predator management, we recommend that feral cat control focuses on techniques such as trapping and shooting that are specific to feral cats in areas where they overlap with apex predators (dingoes), and more general techniques such as poison baiting where they are segregated.

Background

Apex predators can shape the composition and function of ecosystems through their effects on sympatric competitors and prey [1]. By suppressing herbivore populations [2], or affecting the distribution and space use of subordinate mesopredators [3,4,5], apex predators may initiate trophic cascades, with substantial impacts on prey assemblages, ecosystem structure and hence biodiversity [1, 2, 6]. Where mesopredators co-occur with apex predators, they may modify their behaviour to minimise interactions with larger predators [7].

Invasive mesopredators pose a major threat to biodiversity worldwide [8, 9]. In ecosystems where native apex predators occur in sympatry with invasive mesopredators, maintaining the ecological function of native apex predators while mitigating the impacts of invasive mesopredators presents a key challenge for managers. A better understanding of the factors that influence the space use of sympatric predators can therefore help inform more targeted and effective conservation, and pest and wildlife management [10,11,12].

The space required by an individual predator to survive and reproduce is affected by its interactions with sympatric species and local resource availability [13, 14]. The resulting area, commonly known as the home range, is a fundamental metric in animal ecology [15]. Among predator species, home range area typically scales positively with body size as a result of increasing energetic requirements and specialization on larger prey [16]. However, within a predator population, there is often considerable variation in home range size due to factors including body size, sex, age, reproductive status and season [17]. Therefore, a comparison of space use among sympatric predator species is important for understanding community-level effects, while an examination of space use within a species’ population can provide insights into the species’ ecology, demography and population dynamics.

An individual’s home range is influenced by its selection or avoidance of habitat features within the landscape. This is a hierarchical, multi-scale process which involves a series of innate and learned behavioural decisions that can affect fitness [18]. Broad-scale (second-order) habitat selection describes the selection of a home range from the broader geographic range of a species, and is largely driven by the availability and distribution of resources, climate and landscape features [19]. Finer-scale (third-order) habitat selection describes the use of resources within an individual’s home range, and may be driven by both biotic and abiotic factors [19, 20].

The biological and physical attributes of an environment can influence how predators select habitats and interact with sympatric species. For example, apex predators may preferentially select certain vegetation types according to the likelihood of encountering prey [21,22,23]. Mesopredators may select vegetation types based on prey availability, but may also alter their movement and activity patterns within landscapes according to the occurrence of larger predators [24, 25]. Linear features such as roads, railway lines and seismic exploration lines can facilitate predator access to prey [26] or influence predator behaviour and patterns of movement [27,28,29]. Apex predators [30, 31] and mesopredators [32, 33] have been well-documented using roads independently, but there is little understanding of how they use roads when in sympatry.

Topography and vegetation cover can also mediate predator movement and interactions, as predators may select for areas that don’t impede movement and conserve energy, such as gentle contours and more open habitats [34, 35]. Areas of gentle topography are often associated with hydrological features, so movement through these areas may also relate to the presence of water, dense vegetation cover, and/or higher prey availability. Depending on the predator’s hunting mode (e.g. ambush vs. pursuit), physiological constraints and body-size, a predator may select dense vegetation cover to improve hunting success [36], find shade to assist thermoregulation [37], or provide concealment from other predators or humans [38].

Australia provides a highly tractable system in which to study space use and habitat selection of sympatric terrestrial apex- and meso-predators, because of the continent’s relatively simple mammalian predator guild. There is one large (> 10 kg), terrestrial, native apex predator, the dingo (Canis dingo) [39], and two recently introduced mesopredators: the feral cat (Felis catus) and the European red fox (Vulpes vulpes) (L.), with considerable, though not complete, overlap in their mainland distributions. In some regions, native quolls, varanids and large snake species also constitute important terrestrial mesopredators [40]. Introduced predators, particularly feral cats, are responsible for considerable biodiversity loss worldwide [8, 41] and reducing their impacts is a primary goal for the conservation of Australian fauna [9, 42]. As the apex predator, dingoes are predicted to suppress the activity or abundance of feral cats and there is some empirical evidence to support this [43,44,45,46], but other evidence is equivocal [47], and the overall extent of top-down suppressive control remains contentious [48, 49].

We examined the seasonal home ranges and habitat selection of a sympatric native apex predator (dingo) and an invasive mesopredator (feral cat) in semi-arid Australia. Most dingo populations in Australia are disrupted, to varying degrees, via lethal control [50]. Poison baiting may be targeted specifically at dingoes or affect dingoes as non-targets. In our landscape, poison baiting for feral cats occurs at the study site and dingoes are controlled on surrounding pastoral properties; these programs are likely to affect the demography and space use of both species. Our objective was to understand the landscape use of dingoes and feral cats and their potential interactions under these conditions to help improve feral cat management while minimising impacts to dingoes.

We characterized home range extent and composition of the two predators to quantify inter- and intra-species variation and examine interspecific differences in broad-scale habitat selection. We then used discrete choice modelling to examine differences in fine-scale habitat selection within their home ranges across several habitat features. We predicted that:

  1. 1.

    Dingoes, as the larger predator, would have larger home ranges than feral cats [51].

  2. 2.

    Like many mammalian predators [52], males of both species would have larger home ranges than females.

  3. 3.

    At a broad scale (second-order habitat selection), dingoes would selectively locate their home ranges in areas with more open woodland, while feral cats would select for grassland based on the availability of preferred prey types in these two habitats: large macropods in woodlands [53] and abundance of small vertebrates in grasslands [54].

  4. 4.

    Within their home ranges (third-order habitat selection), dingoes would select for open woodlands [53], roads [50, 55] and hydrological features [56, 57], consistent with previous research. In contrast, feral cats would avoid these features within their home ranges to minimise interaction with dingoes [44] and because their physiology means they do not require water for drinking [58].

  5. 5.

    Within their home ranges (third-order habitat selection), dingoes would also select for low vegetation cover and feral cats would select for high vegetation cover based on their typical prey and open versus ambush hunting modes [40].

We use the results from our study to make management recommendations for more strategic and effective control of feral cats in the presence of dingoes, where it may be desirable to preserve the ecological function of the larger predator [46] — a challenge common to many ecosystems [12]. By identifying where invasive mesopredators are likely to either overlap or segregate spatially with apex predators, control techniques can be adapted to specifically target invasive mesopredators in areas where they overlap with apex predators and more general techniques applied where they are spatially segregated.

Methods

Study area

The study was conducted on the Matuwa Indigenous Protected Area (IPA) and surrounding properties in central Western Australia, 180 km east-northeast of Wiluna (26.23°S, 121.56°E; Fig. 1a). Matuwa IPA is a 2410 km2 former pastoral lease that has been managed jointly as a conservation reserve by the Wiluna Aboriginal community and the Western Australia Department of Biodiversity Conservation and Attractions (formerly Parks and Wildlife) since 2000. All cattle (and a few remaining sheep) were removed from the property by 2003 and recursions are largely prevented by an electric cattle fence along the boundary. This fence presents no barrier to the movement of other medium- to large-sized mammal species in the area including dingoes, feral cats and kangaroos. Camels, donkeys and horses occasionally penetrate the fence and are controlled opportunistically. Surrounding land uses include active pastoral leases as well as unallocated crown land which receives little to no active management. Dingoes and feral cats are common throughout the study area, as are other native predators including large varanids and snakes. Non-native European red foxes are uncommon.

Fig. 1
figure 1

a Location of Matuwa Indigenous Protected Area (IPA) and surrounding properties in semi-arid Western Australia. Fine-scale habitat maps for this study were created from high-resolution aerial imagery b to delineate features predicted to influence dingo and feral cat habitat selection c. Vegetation cover d, also a predictor of habitat selection, was mapped at 3 broad classes using a cover index derived from Landsat data recorded during the period of study. (The red box in a shows the extent in maps b-d)

The climate of the region is classified as a hot, semi-arid desert with highly erratic and unreliable rainfall. The landscape is dominated by gently undulating sand and rocky plains with stony plateau uplands and breakaways, as well as occasional salt lakes with fringing saline alluvial plains [59]. Vegetation communities can be broadly classified as either open woodlands or open hummock grasslands with occasional samphire shrublands. Open woodlands are frequently dominated by Acacia aneura and Eucalyptus kingsmillii and occur on rocky plains, stony uplands, and breakaways. Hummock grasslands are dominated by spinifex grasses (Triodia basedoweii and T. melvillei) with occasional trees and are restricted primarily to the open sand plains. Samphire shrublands typically surround salt lakes and are dominated by halophytic forbs and shrubs.

Feral cat control occurs regularly at Matuwa IPA for the protection of threatened fauna while dingo control occurs intermittently on some of the surrounding pastoral leases for livestock protection. The intensity and regularity of dingo control varies from year to year and between properties. This type of uncoordinated and varying control effort may provide short-term relief from stock depredation and disrupt dingo social stability [60], but is unlikely to have a significant impact on dingo abundance over the long term [61]. A sustained feral cat control program on Matuwa IPA has been in place since 2003 consisting of an annual aerial deployment of toxic sodium monofluroacetate (1080) Eradicat® baits in mid-July (winter) at a density of 50 baits km− 2. In semi-arid regions of Australia, feral cat baiting is most effective during winter when the availability of prey, such as rabbits and reptiles, is typically low and hence bait uptake is generally greater [9].

The annual baiting regime at Matuwa IPA has reduced feral cat activity – as measured by track count indices – to approximately half that of pre-baiting levels; however the impact on feral cat abundance remains unclear [62]. Generally, feral cat activity drops substantially in the first months following baiting and is highest immediately prior to baiting [62]. Dingoes are also susceptible to Eradicat® baits [63], but track indices of dingo activity generally returns to near pre-baiting levels within several months of baiting. This suggests that the scale and frequency of baiting is not enough to outweigh re-invasion by dingoes [64]. Our study was focused on the periods of up to 70 days immediately prior to annual Eradicat bait application to take advantage of the period when cat activity was likely to be the highest, and dingoes would be least affected by baiting [65]. Baits were deployed over a four-day period beginning on July 6 in 2013 and July 10 in 2014; any data collected after the start of these baiting periods were excluded from the analysis.

Animal capture and data collection

Dingoes and feral cats were captured between June 2013 and June 2014 using soft-jaw leg-hold traps (Oneida Victor® Soft Catch® traps, size 1.5 for feral cats, and size 3 for dingoes, Oneida Victor Inc. Ltd., Euclid, USA). Traps were set along unsealed roads at Matuwa IPA and baited with pongo [66] (a blended mixture of cat faeces and urine) for feral cats, and with domestic dog faeces and urine for dingoes. Cats were sedated prior to processing. For cats captured in 2013, sedation was achieved via an intramuscular injection of 4 mg/kg Zoletil 100 (100 mg/mL; teletamine-zolazepam). However, a drawback of this procedure was the relative long time – 30 to 60 min – needed for recovery; therefore, subsequent captures used intramuscular injections of 0.05 mg/kg Domitor (1 mg/mL medetomidine) with the reversal agent Antisedan (5 mg/mL atipamazole) administered intramuscularly at half dose upon completion of processing. Dingoes were not sedated but instead were processed following guidelines set by the Department of Agriculture and Food of Western Australia [67]. This procedure employs a catchpole and restraining board to restrain animals prior to processing.

Captured animals that were large enough, based on a maximum 5% collar to body weight ratio (> 1.9 kg cats, > 9.5 kg dingoes), were fitted with Global Positioning System (GPS) collars. Cat GPS collars (Sirtrack Minitrack with VHF download and internal timed release, Havelock North, New Zealand) weighed 120 g and were programmed to drop off after 3 months. Dingo GPS collars (Sirtrack Pinnacle Lite with VHF transmitter, remote download, and external timed release) weighed 475 g and were programmed to drop off after 12 months. Cat collars recorded the location at a minimum of 4-h intervals starting immediately upon release. Dingo collars recorded locations at 2-h intervals starting on the first full hour following release. Locations collected within the first 24 h of release were discarded from analysis.

Map of landscape features

We developed a fine-scale habitat map of the study area based on four landscape features that we considered likely to influence dingo and feral cat habitat selection or movement: roads, hydrological features, vegetation type and vegetation cover. We digitized the first two features from high-resolution, 80-cm pixel aerial imagery of the study area. All roads in the study site are unsealed and are 5–10 m wide. Hydrological features included seasonal intermittent streams, seasonal lakes and wetlands. Using ArcGIS (v.10.2 ESRI, Redlands, California), we created buffers of 10 m around roads and 20 m around hydrological features in order to encompass errors in the precision of mapping these features; a larger buffer is needed around streams because their mapping is inherently less precise. We mapped two broad classes of vegetation type – grassland and open woodland – by digitizing aerial imagery (Fig. 1b-c). Samphire shrublands represent a relatively low proportion of the vegetation and were not always easy to discern from open woodlands in the imagery, and so we included them within the open woodland classification.

We classified vegetation cover as low, moderate, or high using a Landsat-based vegetation cover index developed by the Land Monitor Project of Western Australia [68]. The index uses spectral bands 3 and 5 to assign a cover value at the 30 m2 pixel scale. We applied a Jenks optimization [69] to find the natural breaks in the distribution of these pixel values across the study area to produce three cover classes (Fig. 1d).

In the final habitat map that we constructed of the study landscape, roads represented less than 0.5% of the total area, while hydrological features represented 3.8%, grasslands 19.0% and open woodlands 81.0%. Low cover represented 15.2%, moderate cover 46.4% and high cover 38.4% of the total area. For the purposes of the broad-scale habitat selection analysis (Prediction 3), habitat classifications were simplified as grassland and woodland (all other habitats).

Data screening and organization

As described above, data analysis was restricted to the periods up to 70 days immediately prior to the annual Eradicat® bait application in early July 2013 and 2014. This period also avoided dingo whelping which typically occurs in mid-July [57]. GPS data were screened for positional errors and unrealistic fixes removed based on the species’ known movement speeds [70]. We excluded fixes exceeding a 16 kmh− 1 gallop speed and 8.75 kmh− 1 trotting speed for dingoes [71] and a 3.2 kmh− 1 gallop and 2.0 kmh− 1 trotting speed for cats [72]. We also removed fixes with elevation values that were ± 100 m from site reference elevation. Based on field-tested errors, we classified dingo fixes that were > 20 m from the preceding fix, and cat fixes that were > 35 m from the preceding fix, as ‘moving’, as fewer than 5% of the fixes from stationary collar tests exhibited distances larger than these cut off values, respectively. Fixes that did not meet the criterion of ‘moving’ were removed from the analysis as we were only interested in habitat selection when individuals were active.

Identification of seasonal home ranges

We used fixed kernel density estimation (KDE) to generate seasonal home ranges for each collared animal by producing a probabilistic model of space use defined by a utilisation distribution (UD). We calculated the UD for each individual at the 95 and 50% isopleths to define seasonal home range and seasonal core home range respectively [73] and use the term ‘seasonal’ to reflect that these home range estimations are restricted to the Martu season of wantajarra (late autumn – winter) [74]. We chose the reference bandwidth to determine the level of smoothing for KDE calculations because this non-parametric method generally results in a smoother contour with less variability and so presents a more generalized snapshot of space use [75]. Kernel estimates were calculated in R in the package rhr [76]. To determine whether seasonal home ranges were sufficiently described we used asymptote analysis with data added randomly at 15-fix intervals for cats and 25-fix intervals for dingoes. The smaller interval for cats was used because the total number of fixes for feral cats was generally lower than for dingoes since collars on cats were programmed to take half as many fixes as dingoes in a 24-h period. We considered home ranges sufficiently described when 75–100% of the fixes were within 95% of the total KDE area using all the fixes.

Home range size

We used linear models to test the effects of species on the area of seasonal home range and seasonal core home range (Prediction 1). Data were log10-transformed to improve normality and the homoscedasticity of residuals. We also used linear models to test for the effects of sex on seasonal home range area and seasonal core home range area in dingoes (Prediction 2); we were unable to test this prediction for feral cats due to the small number of female cats (n = 3). In the latter models, seasonal home range areas were log10 transformed for the same reasons as above, but seasonal core home range areas did not require transformation.

Broad-scale habitat selection

To assess whether dingoes and feral cats selectively located their home ranges in relation to broad habitat types (Prediction 3), we compared the proportion of vegetation type (woodland for dingoes and grassland for feral cats) within observed seasonal home ranges (“used”) to the proportion of that vegetation type within randomly-placed circular home ranges (“available”, n = 1000) equal in area to the median seasonal home range (48,325 ha for dingoes, 2482 ha for feral cats) [77, 78]. Available home ranges for each species were sampled within the area considered available to all individuals of that species. To define the boundaries for sampling available habitat, we generated 100% minimum convex polygons (MCPs) encompassing all GPS locations of all individuals for each species [79]. Using ArcGIS, we then created a buffer around all GPS locations for each species that was equal to the radius of the median circular seasonal home range (12,402 m for dingoes and 2811 m for feral cats) and merged this with the MCPs. We calculated the proportional availability of woodland for dingoes and grassland for feral cats in the used and randomly-sampled available home ranges, then used a resource selection function for each species based on logistic regression to compare used to available samples [78, 80]. We weighted available data to used data as 0.016:1 for dingoes (nobs = 16) and 0.025:1 for cats (nobs = 25), to allow for a balanced comparison and avoid inflating statistical precision [78].

Fine-scale habitat selection

We examined fine-scale habitat selection (prediction 4 and 5) – i.e. third-order habitat selection – by dingoes and feral cats using separate step selection functions (SSFs) for each species [81]. SSFs link each observed GPS location to a set of random control locations or “steps” that are considered available but not selected by an individual animal at that time. We used Geospatial Modelling Environment (v. 0.7.2.1 Spatial Ecology LLC) to generate 20 random alternate locations for each observed GPS fix based on the empirical step length and turning angle distributions across all individuals for each species. We then attributed each observed and control location to the mapped habitat features using ArcGIS.

Habitat selection as a SSF was modelled as mixed effects Cox proportional hazard model for each species in R [82] using the ‘coxme’ package [83]. With Cox models, the selection coefficient for each habitat feature is estimated by conditional logistic regression and expressed as the log odds ratio of that feature being chosen relative to a reference habitat state. We specified one model for each species. We combined road and habitat type into one variable with four categories (i.e. on-road in open woodland, off-road in open woodland, on-road in grassland and off-road in grassland) with the reference community set as off-road in grassland; this allowed us to derive coefficients for each level, facilitating the interpretation of results. We also included a random intercept term for individual and a random coefficient for vegetation type. The inclusion of random intercepts in SFFs can be particularly beneficial when there is a discrepancy in sample size among individuals, as was the case here, while the random slope term for vegetation type allowed us to examine individual variation in responses to this covariate [84]. No pairs of the landscape features showed a Pearson’s correlation greater than 0.39 for either species; thus, all features were included in the models.

After fitting the models for each species, we examined deviance residuals for evidence of serial autocorrelation [85]. To do this, we fitted an intercept-only linear mixed-effects model to the residuals grouped by individual animal (i.e., a random intercept) for each species. Next, we plotted the autocorrelation function and determined the appropriate lag where autocorrelation became non-significant (α = 0.05). We then performed a sensitivity analysis by rerunning our original models with data grouped to these lags and examined the difference between model outputs of the sensitivity analysis and the original models.

We did not include time of day or sex as covariates in the models but rather examined the influence of these factors on habitat selection by subsetting the data and running the models for each subset group within each species separately. For time of day, we subset data as either diurnal or nocturnal for each species. We were only able to examine the effect of sex on habitat selection in dingoes, due to the small number of female feral cats sampled (n = 3).

Results

Seventeen adult dingoes (6 ♂, 11 ♀) and 29 adult feral cats (24 ♂, 5 ♀) were captured between June 2013 and 2014 (Additional file 1: Table S1). One adult female cat was euthanized because of trap injuries. The remaining animals were fitted with GPS collars. Two collars representing one male and one female cat were not recovered, and one collar on a female dingo and one collar on a male cat had transmission errors and were discarded from analysis. As a result, pre-baiting data were collected from 16 dingoes (all in 2014) and 25 feral cats (six in 2013 and 19 in 2014). Following bait deployment, four of these dingoes stopped moving within five days, and 15 feral cats (four in 2013 and 11 in 2014) stopped moving within nine days – we suspect death from toxic bait ingestion.

Only data from the pre-baiting period each year were included in the analysis. This provided a total of 11,160 GPS fixes from 16 dingoes (range: 219–840, mean = 698 fixes per individual) and 5214 GPS fixes from 25 feral cats (range: 84–391, mean = 209 fixes per individual) after screening removed 13 dingo and 12 feral cat fixes. Total fixes represented between 14 and 70 days of location data for each animal. The average GPS-collar fix rate for these collars was 98.5% (range: 93.8–100%) for dingoes and 92.6% (range: 72.9–98.9%) for cats. Cut-off distances for movement (> 20 m for dingoes and > 35 m for feral cats) indicated that dingoes were moving 73.0% and cats 86.4% of the time. Fixes below the cut-off distances for movement were considered resting and removed. The remaining ‘moving’ fixes provided 8082 discrete habitat choices or ‘steps’ for dingoes and 4242 for feral cats (Additional file 1: Table S1). There was a slight female bias in the analysed dingo sample (6 ♂, 10 ♀) and a strong male bias in the cat sample (22 ♂, 3 ♀). The mean weight (± s.e.) of collared animals for this study was 16.34 ± 0.72 (range: 13.0–23.0) kg for dingoes and 4.21 ± 0.15 (range: 2.5–5.5) kg for feral cats.

Home range size

Kernel density estimates for all 16 dingoes and 25 feral cats reached asymptotes and were used in the analysis. Dingoes had substantially larger seasonal home ranges (95% kernel isopleth; β ± SE: 1.25 ± 0.11, t = 11.39, p < 0.001) and seasonal core home ranges (50% kernel isopleth; 1.14 ± 0.12, t = 9.45, p < 0.001) than feral cats (Table 1, Fig. 2, see also Additional files 2 (dingoes) and 3 (feral cats) for asymptote analyses and plots of 95 and 50% KDE utilisation distributions for each individual). There was some overlap in home range size, with the largest feral cat seasonal home range being larger than the five smallest dingo home ranges. However, the remaining feral cat seasonal home ranges were all smaller than all the smallest dingo home ranges. Male dingoes had substantially larger seasonal home ranges (0.43 ± 0.16, t = 2.601, p = 0.021) and seasonal core home ranges (11,733 ± 4898, t = 2.40, p = 0.031) than female dingoes (Table 1).

Table 1 Median, (IQR) and [min – max] of seasonal home range (95% KDE) and seasonal core home range (50% KDE) size of dingoes & feral cats at Matuwa IPA and surrounding properties, Western Australia
Fig. 2
figure 2

Utilization distributions (95% KDE) representing seasonal home ranges of male dingoes (a), female dingoes (b), feral cats captured in 2013 (c) and feral cats captured in 2014 (d) at Matuwa IPA and surrounding properties in semi-arid Western Australia

Broad-scale habitat selection

The proportion of woodland within observed dingo seasonal home ranges varied from 51 to 100% (median 86%). There was no evidence that dingoes selectively located their seasonal home ranges in relation to the availability of woodland (βwoodland ± se: 0.47 ± 1.87, z = 0.251, p = 0.802; Additional file 1: Figure S1a). The proportion of grassland within observed cat seasonal home ranges varied from < 1 to 86% (median 43%). Overall, feral cats selected for home range locations that had a higher proportion of grassland than random (βgrassland ± se: 2.07 ± 0.95, z = 2.179, p = 0.029; Additional file 1: Figure S1b).

Fine-scale habitat selection

In their fine-scale movements, dingoes selected strongly for open woodlands, whereas cats used open woodlands and grasslands in proportion to their availability within their home range (Fig. 3a, Additional file 1: Table S2). Both predators selected for roads in each vegetation type but selected more strongly for roads in grasslands than roads in open woodlands (relative to non-road habitats in the same vegetation type). The odds of dingoes selecting roads was 18.22 times higher in grasslands and 4.04 times higher in open woodlands than the odds of selecting non-road locations in those same vegetation types. For cats, the odds of selecting roads was 3.07 times higher in grasslands and 1.65 times higher in open woodlands compared to non-road locations in those same vegetation types. Both species also selected for hydrological features and high vegetation cover. Dingoes showed no response to low cover, whereas feral cats selected against areas with low cover. Our results also suggested that there was little difference between diurnal and nocturnal selection of habitat features by dingoes (Fig. 3b) or feral cats (Fig. 3c) although there were slight variations in selection patterns for male and female dingoes (Fig. 3d).

Fig. 3
figure 3

Regression coefficients of population-level (fixed) effects for habitat features included in mixed-effects step-selection function models comparing dingoes and feral cats (a) at Matuwa IPA and surrounding properties in semi-arid Western Australia. Model runs also included data subset by time of day (diurnal or nocturnal) for dingoes (b) and feral cats (c), and by sex for dingoes (d). 95% CIs above zero indicate significant selection for the habitat feature within the home range, those below indicate significant avoidance, and those overlapping zero indicate no significant selection of the feature relative to availability

Results of the intercept-only linear mixed-effects models showed that autocorrelation was near zero after a lag of 5 observations (10 h) for dingoes and after 2 observations (8 h) for cats. Sensitivity tests of models including these lags suggested that autocorrelation impacts were minimal as only very minor changes in the model coefficients and standard errors were observed (Additional file 1: Table S3). Therefore, outputs for the full datasets are presented.

Discussion

Understanding how apex- and meso-predators use landscapes in sympatry can inform relevant ecological theory such as species niches and competition, and in an applied context, help inform management decisions regarding pest control and wildlife conservation. We developed and tested predictions about the space use of a native apex predator (dingoes) and a sympatric, invasive mesopredator (feral cats) by examining their home ranges and habitat selection. Our predictions that dingoes would exhibit larger home ranges than feral cats and that male dingoes would have larger home ranges than females were supported by our results. Dingoes did not exhibit broad-scale habitat selection for open woodlands, but feral cats selected for grasslands at this scale. Our results also supported our prediction that dingoes would select for open woodlands, roads and hydrological features in their fine-scale movements. Contrary to our expectations, however, feral cats did not avoid these features. Instead, feral cats also selected for roads and hydrological features in their fine-scale movements but exhibited no third-order habitat selection for vegetation type. Our final prediction that dingoes would select for low vegetation cover and that feral cats would select for high vegetation cover was partially supported: both dingoes and feral cats selected for high vegetation cover, but feral cats also selected against low cover whereas dingoes did not. Our results suggest that mesopredators do not always avoid the same habitat features as sympatric apex predators but can coexist with them even at fine spatial scales. By understanding the habitat preferences of an invasive mesopredator in the presence of a native apex predator, our study emphasizes the importance of spatial considerations in vertebrate pest management.

The 20-fold difference in seasonal home range size observed between dingoes and feral cats was expected due to their difference in body size, energetic requirements and size of prey [16]. In arid regions of Australia, dingoes tend to prey on large macropodids (Macropus spp.) which often exceed their own weight [86, 87], while feral cats feed primarily on small mammals, birds and reptiles that are much smaller than their own body size [86, 88]. Dingo males also had larger seasonal home ranges than females. This difference is common among carnivores and is frequently associated with sexual dimorphism or the maximization of mating opportunities by males [52]. Dingo and feral cat home range estimates vary widely across Australia and between ecosystems [89,90,91]; however the seasonal home range estimates in this study are similar to other observations from arid regions of Australia [64, 92,93,94].

For each predator, we examined both broad-scale and fine-scale habitat selection across the two dominant vegetation types: open woodlands and grasslands. We found evidence to support broad-scale habitat selection of grasslands by feral cats but no evidence of habitat selection of woodlands by dingoes at this scale. In contrast, dingoes selected for open woodlands in their fine-scale movements, whereas third-order habitat selection for vegetation type was equivocal among feral cats; although some individual cats likely selected either for or against open woodlands. Strong selection for open woodlands by dingoes within their home ranges was most likely related to the presence of their main prey, macropodids [86], which typically prefer open woodlands to hummock grasslands in semi-arid ecosystems [53].

Second-order habitat selection for grasslands by feral cats suggests that they prefer to establish home ranges in this vegetation type even though they may not specifically select for it in their fine-scale movements. It is likely that feral cat selection at this scale is driven by prey availability. Hummock grasslands in our study site, and in semi-arid ecosystems more generally, support more abundant and diverse small vertebrates – which make up the bulk of feral cat diets – than open woodlands [54, 95, 96]. At the same time, feral cat second-order habitat selection for grasslands could be influenced by avoidance of dingoes. Although the two predators co-occur at fine spatial scales, feral cats are known to avoid dingoes temporally or spatially [89], similar to other sympatric apex predators and mesopredators [97,98,99,100]. To understand which of these factors is important for feral cat habitat selection, we recommend studies which manipulate either resource availability (e.g. via fire) or dingo abundance (e.g. via exclusion fencing or using existing contrasts in poison baiting) within both habitat types.

Dingoes selected very strongly for roads in both habitats in their fine-scale movements. This finding is consistent with a large body of evidence that demonstrates that many apex predators including wolves [34, 101], bears [27] and African wild dogs [102] select for roads and other linear features especially where traffic or human presence is low. In Australia, Robley, Gormley [55] documented selection for roads by dingoes in a forested region of eastern Victoria, and it is generally accepted that roads provide important movement corridors for dingoes [50]. However, our results indicate a much stronger selection for roads in grasslands than in open woodlands. This stronger selection for roads in grasslands may reflect the difference in utility of these two habitat types. With reduced food resources in grasslands, dingoes may primarily use roads in this habitat to travel efficiently to more productive (i.e. open woodland) foraging areas of their home ranges. However, it is also possible that dingoes select roads more strongly in grasslands to avoid traversing through spiky grasses.

Contrary to our predictions, feral cats selected for roads in both vegetation types, despite dingoes also strongly selecting for this feature. The use and selection of roads by feral cats has been well-documented globally and within Australia [103]. Feral cat selection for roads was substantially weaker than that of dingoes which may be a function of the smaller size of feral cat home ranges and hence the lower likelihood of encountering roads relative to their density in the study area. Like dingoes, feral cats selected for roads more strongly in grasslands than in open woodlands. However, unlike dingoes, feral cats also showed second-order habitat selection for grasslands. The stronger selection for roads in grasslands may be because the higher understory cover and high density of spiky spinifex grasses in grasslands present more resistance to movement than open woodlands. It is also possible that feral cats may use roads less in open woodlands to limit their exposure to dingoes where encounters are more likely. Without manipulating resource or dingo abundance, it is difficult to determine which mechanism is driving habitat choice — these possibilities require further investigation.

As predicted, dingoes selected for hydrological features within their home ranges. Selection for these features is likely due to higher prey availability and the presence of water, and/or the use of waterways as movement corridors, as has been documented for other predators [104,105,106]. Thomson [57] also found that dingoes selected for riparian areas and proposed that the location of water was an important factor in determining the distributions of both dingoes and their prey, while Harden [56] found that dingoes frequently travelled along streams during the course of their regular movements.

In contrast to our prediction, we found that feral cats also selected for hydrological features. While this has previously been documented in other parts of Australia [107, 108], to our knowledge this study represents the first evidence of this preference in arid regions of the country. Unlike dingoes [109], feral cats do not need to drink regularly [110], hence their preference for hydrological features is probably related to the presence of prey, physiological constraints (a cooler, thermal refuge), and/or use as movement corridors.

Our results did not support our prediction that dingoes would preferentially select low vegetation cover; rather they selected for high vegetation cover. Dingo selection for high vegetation cover may be related to the presence of at least one of their main prey species, the common wallaroo (Macropus robustus), which typically prefers habitat with moderate cover and complexity [111]. It may also reflect the dingoes’ need for concealment and shade in hot arid climates [50].

Like dingoes, feral cats also selected for areas of high vegetation cover but unlike dingoes they simultaneously selected against areas of low vegetation cover. Many felids prefer areas of high vegetation cover for stalking and ambushing prey; feral cats have been documented using high vegetation cover both globally [103] and within Australia [93, 94, 112]. Avoidance of low vegetation cover is probably important for shelter and concealment from other predators, such as dingoes. However, McGregor, Legge [108] found that cats select more strongly for the edges of open habitats, particularly newly-burnt areas and concluded that this was probably to maximize hunting efficiency. We did not investigate predator habitat selection with respect to fire attributes but acknowledge that this is likely to be of value particularly for fire-prone landscapes [113], and it should be a focus for further work in our study region.

Conclusions and management implications

Roads and other linear features have disproportionately large effects on ecological processes, despite occupying a relatively small proportion of land [114, 115]. Previous studies have quantified predator selection between different types of linear features such as between trail [116] or road types [35], but to our knowledge this is the first study to quantify the strength of selection by multiple predator species for roads or linear features across different habitat types. We found that both predators selected more strongly for roads within grasslands than roads within open woodlands. Because many predators – particularly carnivores – are known to use roads, studies that investigate the variability in the selection for roads across different habitats have the potential to greatly improve our understanding of predator space use and help inform predator management.

Our case study highlights the potential for strategically designed and spatially-heterogenous pest control programs to limit non-target impacts and maximise conservation outcomes. We found that both dingoes and feral cats selected for hydrological features, high vegetation cover and roads. Therefore, invasive predator control in these parts of the landscape should focus on protecting key sites of high biodiversity value (such as lakes or waterholes) by employing techniques that are specific to feral cats such as trapping or shooting at scales that are feasible for management. Aerial dispersal of predator baits which do not discriminate between feral cats and dingoes is currently applied once a year over the entire study site. Restricting baiting to grasslands away from roads could increase the specificity of this management and reduce non-target impacts on dingoes, as feral cats showed some preference for grasslands while dingoes selected strongly for open woodlands and occurred very infrequently off-road in grasslands.

Availability of data and materials

The datasets used and/or analysed during the current study are available from the corresponding author on reasonable request.

Abbreviations

GPS:

Global Positioning System

IPA:

Indigenous Protected Area

KDE:

Kernel Density Estimation

MCP:

Minimum Convex Polygon

SSF:

Step Selection Function

UD:

Utilisation Distribution

References

  1. Ritchie EG, Johnson CN. Predator interactions, mesopredator release and biodiversity conservation. Ecol Lett. 2009;12(9):982–98.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  2. Ripple WJ, Beschta RL. Wolves and the ecology of fear: can predation risk structure ecosystems? Bioscience. 2004;54(8):755–66.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  3. Palomares F, Ferreras P, Fedriani JM, Delibes M. Spatial relationships between Iberian lynx and other carnivores in an area of South-Western Spain. J Appl Ecol. 1996:5–13.

  4. Newsome TM, Greenville AC, Ćirović D, Dickman CR, Johnson CN, Krofel M, et al. Top predators constrain mesopredator distributions. Nat Commun. 2017;8:15469.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  5. Creel S, Christianson D. Relationships between direct predation and risk effects. Trends Ecol Evol. 2008;23(4):194–201.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  6. Terborgh J, Estes JA. Trophic cascades: predators, prey, and the changing dynamics of nature. Washington, DC: Island Press; 2010.

    Google Scholar 

  7. Laundré JW, Hernández L, Ripple WJ. The landscape of fear: ecological implications of being afraid. Open Ecol J. 2010;3:1–7.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  8. Doherty TS, Glen AS, Nimmo DG, Ritchie EG, Dickman CR. Invasive predators and global biodiversity loss. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2016;113(40):11261–5.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  PubMed Central  Google Scholar 

  9. Doherty TS, Dickman CR, Johnson CN, Legge SM, Ritchie EG, Woinarski JC. Impacts and management of feral cats Felis catus in Australia. Mamm Rev. 2017;47(2):83–97.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  10. Soulé ME, Estes JA, Miller B, Honnold DL. Strongly interacting species: conservation policy, management, and ethics. Bioscience. 2005;55(2):168–76.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  11. Ritchie EG, Elmhagen B, Glen AS, Letnic M, Ludwig G, McDonald RA. Ecosystem restoration with teeth: what role for predators? Trends Ecol Evol. 2012;27(5):265–71.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  12. Doherty TS, Ritchie EG. Stop jumping the gun: a call for evidence-based invasive predator management. Conserv Lett. 2017;10(1):15–22.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  13. Adams ES. Approaches to the study of territory size and shape. Annu Rev Ecol Syst. 2001;32(1):277–303.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  14. Börger L, Dalziel BD, Fryxell JM. Are there general mechanisms of animal home range behaviour? A review and prospects for future research. Ecol Lett. 2008;11(6):637–50.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  15. Burt WH. Territoriality and home range concepts as applied to mammals. J Mammal. 1943;24(3):346–52.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  16. Gittleman JL, Harvey PH. Carnivore home-range size, metabolic needs and ecology. Behav Ecol Sociobiol. 1982;10(1):57–63.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  17. Saïd S, Gaillard JM, Widmer O, Débias F, Bourgoin G, Delorme D, et al. What shapes intra-specific variation in home range size? A case study of female roe deer. Oikos. 2009;118(9):1299–306.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  18. Krausman PR. Some basic principles of habitat use, grazing behavior of livestock and wildlife. Wildl Range Exper Station Bull. 1999;70:85–90.

    Google Scholar 

  19. Johnson DH. The comparison of usage and availability measurements for evaluating resource preference. Ecology. 1980;61(1):65–71.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  20. van Beest FM, Rivrud IM, Loe LE, Milner JM, Mysterud A. What determines variation in home range size across spatiotemporal scales in a large browsing herbivore? J Anim Ecol. 2011;80(4):771–85.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  21. Manly BFL, McDonald L, Thomas D, McDonald TL, Erickson WP. Resource selection by animals: statistical design and analysis for field studies. New York: Springer Science & Business Media; 2007.

    Google Scholar 

  22. Palomares F, Delibes M, Revilla E, Calzada J, Fedriani JM. Spatial ecology of Iberian lynx and abundance of European rabbits in southwestern Spain. Wildl Monogr. 2001;148:1–36.

    Google Scholar 

  23. Hopcraft JGC, Sinclair ARE, Packer C. Planning for success: Serengeti lions seek prey accessibility rather than abundance. J Anim Ecol. 2005;74(3):559–66.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  24. Glen AS, Dickman CR. Complex interactions among mammalian carnivores in Australia, and their implications for wildlife management. Biol Rev. 2005;80(03):387.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  25. Fedriani JM, Palomares F, Delibes M. Niche relations among three sympatric Mediterranean carnivores. Oecologia. 1999;121(1):138–48.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  26. Latham ADM, Latham MC, McCutchen NA, Boutin S. Invading white-tailed deer change wolf-caribou dynamics in northeastern Alberta. J Wildl Manag. 2011;75(1):204–12.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  27. Roever CL, Boyce MS, Stenhouse GB. Grizzly bear movements relative to roads: application of step selection functions. Ecography. 2010;33(6):1113–22.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  28. Hradsky BA, Robley A, Alexander R, Ritchie EG, York A, Di Stefano J. Human-modified habitats facilitate forest-dwelling populations of an invasive predator, Vulpes vulpes. Sci Rep. 2017;7(1):12291.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  CAS  Google Scholar 

  29. Raiter KG, Hobbs RJ, Possingham HP, Valentine LE, Prober SM. Vehicle tracks are predator highways in intact landscapes. Biol Conserv. 2018;228:281–90.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  30. Thurber JM, Peterson RO, Drummer TD, Thomasma SA. Gray wolf response to refuge boundaries and roads in Alaska. Wildl Soc Bull (1973–2006). 1994;22(1):61–8.

    Google Scholar 

  31. Graham K, Boulanger J, Duval J, Stenhouse G. Spatial and temporal use of roads by grizzly bears in west-Central Alberta. Ursus. 2010;21(1):43–56.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  32. Frey SN, Conover MR. Habitat use by Meso-predators in a corridor environment. J Wildl Manag. 2006;70(4):1111–8.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  33. May SA, Norton TW. Influence of fragmentation and disturbance on the potential impact of feral predators on native fauna in Australian forest ecosystems. Wildl Res. 1996;23(4):387–400.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  34. Whittington J. St. Clair CC, Mercer G. spatial responses of wolves to roads and trails in mountain valleys. Ecol Appl. 2005;15(2):543–53.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  35. Dickson BG, Jenness JS, Beier P. Influence of vegetation, topography, and roads on cougar movement in southern California. J Wildl Manag. 2005;69(1):264–76.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  36. Milakovic B, Parker KL, Gustine DD, Lay RJ, Walker ABD, Gillingham MP. Habitat selection by a focal predator (Canis lupus) in a multiprey ecosystem of the northern Rockies. J Mammal. 2011;92(3):568–82.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  37. Careau V, Morand-Ferron J, Thomas D. Basal metabolic rate of canidae from hot deserts to cold arctic climates. J Mammal. 2007;88(2):394–400.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  38. Janssen A, Sabelis MW, Magalhães S, Montserrat M, Van der Hammen T. Habitat structure affects intraguild predation. Ecology. 2007;88(11):2713–9.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  39. Smith BP, Cairns KM, Adams JW, Newsome TM, Fillios M, Deaux EC, et al. Taxonomic status of the Australian dingo: the case for Canis dingo Meyer, 1793. Zootaxa. 2019;4564(1):173–97.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  40. Glen A, Dickman C. Carnivores of Australia: past, present and future. Melbourne: CSIRO Publishing; 2014.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  41. Prugh LR, Stoner CJ, Epps CW, Bean WT, Ripple WJ, Laliberte AS, et al. The rise of the Mesopredator. Bioscience. 2009;59(9):779–91.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  42. Woinarski J, Burbidge A, Harrison P. Action plan for Australian mammals 2012. Melbourne: CSIRO Publishing; 2014.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  43. Glen AS, Dickman CR, Soule ME, Mackey BG. Evaluating the role of the dingo as a trophic regulator in Australian ecosystems. Austral Ecol. 2007;32(5):492–501.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  44. Brook LA, Johnson CN, Ritchie EG, Dickman C. Effects of predator control on behaviour of an apex predator and indirect consequences for mesopredator suppression. J Appl Ecol. 2012;49(6):1278–86.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  45. Kennedy M, Phillips BL, Legge S, Murphy SA, Faulkner RA. Do dingoes suppress the activity of feral cats in northern Australia? Austral Ecol. 2012;37(1):134–9.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  46. Letnic M, Ritchie EG, Dickman CR. Top predators as biodiversity regulators: the dingo Canis lupus dingo as a case study. Biol Rev. 2012;87(2):390–413.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  47. Allen BL, Fleming PJS, Allen LR, Engeman RM, Ballard G, Leung LKP. As clear as mud: a critical review of evidence for the ecological roles of Australian dingoes. Biol Conserv. 2013;159:158–74.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  48. Hayward MW, Boitani L, Burrows ND, Funston PJ, Karanth KU, MacKenzie DI, et al. FORUM: ecologists need robust survey designs, sampling and analytical methods. J Appl Ecol. 2015;52(2):286–90.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  49. Nimmo DG, Watson SJ, Forsyth DM, Bradshaw CJA, Frair J. FORUM: dingoes can help conserve wildlife and our methods can tell. J Appl Ecol. 2015;52(2):281–5.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  50. Fleming P, Corbett LK, Harden RH, Thomson PC. Managing the impacts of dingoes and other wild dogs. Canberra: Bureau of Rural Sciences; 2001.

    Google Scholar 

  51. Carbone C, Mace GM, Roberts SC, Macdonald DW. Energetic constraints on the diet of terrestrial carnivores. Nature. 1999;402(6759):286–8.

    Article  CAS  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  52. Lindstedt SL, Miller BJ, Buskirk SW. Home range, time, and body size in mammals. Ecology. 1986:413–8.

  53. Short J, Caughley G, Grice D, Brown B. The distribution and abundance of kangaroos in relation to environment in Western Australia. Wildl Res. 1983;10(3):435–51.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  54. Chapman T, Burrows N. Lorna Glen (Matuwa) small vertebrate fauna monitoring program 2002–2010, preliminary analysis and review. Kensington: Department of Parks and Wildlife, Western Australia; 2015. p. 70.

    Google Scholar 

  55. Robley A, Gormley A, Forsyth DM, Wilton AN, Stephens D. Movements and habitat selection by wild dogs in eastern Victoria. Aust Mammal. 2010;32(1):23–32.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  56. Harden RH. The ecology of the dingo in North-Eastern New South Wales I. movements and home range. Wildl Res. 1985;12(1):25–37.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  57. Thomson PC. The behavioural ecology of dingoes in North-Western Australia. II. Activity patterns, breeding season and pup rearing. Wildl Res. 1992;19(5):519–29.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  58. Bradshaw JW. The behaviour of the domestic cat. 2nd ed. Wallingford: CAB International; 2012.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  59. Mabbutt JA, Litchfield WH, Speck NH, Wright RL, Wilcox DG, Arnold JM, et al. No. 7 general report on lands of the Wiluna-Meekatharra area, Western Australia, 1958. CSIRO Land Res Surv. 2010;1:1–223.

    Google Scholar 

  60. Wallach AD, Ritchie EG, Read J, O'Neill AJ. More than mere numbers: the impact of lethal control on the social stability of a top-order predator. PLoS One. 2009;4(9):e6861.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  CAS  Google Scholar 

  61. Allen L, Engeman R, Krupa H. Evaluation of three relative abundance indices for assessing dingo populations. Wildl Res. 1996;23(2):197–205.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  62. Algar D, Onus M, Hamilton N. Feral cat control as part of rangelands restoration at Lorna Glen (Matuwa), Western Australia: the first seven years. Conserv Sci West Aust. 2013;8(3):367–81.

    Google Scholar 

  63. Doherty TS, Algar D. Response of feral cats to a track-based baiting programme using Eradicat® baits. Ecol Manage Restor. 2015;16(2):124–30.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  64. Burrows ND, Algar D, Robinson AD, Sinagra J, Ward B, Liddelow G. Controlling introduced predators in the Gibson Desert of Western Australia. J Arid Environ. 2003;55(4):691–713.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  65. Christensen PE, Ward BG, Sims C. Predicting bait uptake by feral cats, Felis catus, in semi-arid environments. Ecol Manage Restor. 2013;14(1):47–53.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  66. Algar D, Angus G, Williams M, Mellican A. Influence of bait type, weather and prey abundance on bait uptake by feral cats (Felis catus) on Peron peninsula, Western Australia. Conserv Sci West Austr. 2007;6(1):109.

    Google Scholar 

  67. Kennedy M. Restraint, handling and marking of wild Canids captured in foot-hold traps: AEC standard operating procedure. Perth: Department of Agriculture and Food, Western Australia; 2013. p. 13.

    Google Scholar 

  68. Caccetta PA, Allen A, Watson I, editors. The land monitor project. Proc Australasian remote sensing and photogrammetry Conf,10th, 21–25 Aug. Adelaide: Remote sensing and photogrammetry Assoc. of Australasia Ltd, Australia; 2000.

    Google Scholar 

  69. Jenks GF. Generalization in statistical mapping. Ann Assoc Am Geogr. 1963;53(1):15–26.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  70. Bjørneraas K, Moorter B, Rolandsen CM, Herfindal I. Screening global positioning system location data for errors using animal movement characteristics. J Wildl Manag. 2010;74(6):1361–6.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  71. Deban SM, Schilling N, Carrier DR. Activity of extrinsic limb muscles in dogs at walk, trot and gallop. J Exp Biol. 2012;215(2):287–300.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  72. Goslow GE, Reinking RM, Stuart DG. The cat step cycle: hind limb joint angles and muscle lengths during unrestrained locomotion. J Morphol. 1973;141(1):1–41.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  73. Laver PN, Kelly MJ. A critical review of home range studies. J Wildl Manag. 2008;72(1):290–8.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  74. Walsh F. To hunt and to hold: Martu Aboriginal people's uses and knowledge of their country, with implications for co-management in Karlamilyi (Rudall River) National Park and the Great Sandy Desert, Western Australia [PhD thesis]. Perth: University of Western Australia; 2009.

    Google Scholar 

  75. Fieberg J. Kernel density estimators of home range: smoothing and the autocorrelation red herring. Ecology. 2007;88(4):1059–66.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  76. Signer J, Balkenhol N. Reproducible home ranges (rhr): a new, user-friendly R package for analyses of wildlife telemetry data. Wildl Soc Bull. 2015;39(2):358–63.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  77. Katnik DD, Wielgus RB. Lubow. Landscape proportions versus Monte Carlo simulated home ranges for estimating habitat availability. J Wildl Manag. 2005;69(1):20–32.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  78. Squires JR, DeCesare NJ, Olson LE, Kolbe JA, Hebblewhite M, Parks SA. Combining resource selection and movement behavior to predict corridors for Canada lynx at their southern range periphery. Biol Conserv. 2013;157:187–95.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  79. Beasley JC, DeVault TL, Retamosa MI, Rhodes OE Jr. A hierarchical analysis of habitat selection by raccoons in northern Indiana. J Wildl Manag. 2007;71(4):1125–33.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  80. Manly BFJ, McDonald LL, Thomas DL, McDonald TL, Erickson W. Resource selection by animals: statistical design and analysis for field studies. Boston: Kluwer Academic Publishers; 2002.

    Google Scholar 

  81. Fortin D, Beyer HL, Boyce MS, Smith DW, Duchesne T, Mao JS. Wolves influence elk movements: behavior shapes a trophic cascade in Yellowstone National Park. Ecology. 2005;86(5):1320–30.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  82. R Core Team. R: A language and environment for statistical computing. Vienna: R Foundation for Statistical Computing; 2013. URL: http://www.r-project.org.

    Google Scholar 

  83. Therneau T. Coxme: mixed effects cox models. R package version 2.2–3. Vienna: R Foundation for Statistical Computing; 2012.

    Google Scholar 

  84. Gillies CS, Hebblewhite M, Nielsen SE, Krawchuk MA, Aldridge CL, Frair JL, et al. Application of random effects to the study of resource selection by animals. J Anim Ecol. 2006;75(4):887–98.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  85. Forester JD, Im HK, Rathouz PJ. Accounting for animal movement in estimation of resource selection functions: sampling and data analysis. Ecology. 2009;90(12):3554–65.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  86. Wysong ML, Tulloch AI, Valentine LE, Hobbs RJ, Morris K, Ritchie EG. The truth about cats and dogs: assessment of apex-and mesopredator diets improves with reduced observer uncertainty. J Mammal. 2019;100(2):410–22.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  87. Doherty TS, Davis NE, Dickman CR, Forsyth DM, Letnic M, Nimmo DG, et al. Continental patterns in the diet of a top predator: Australia's dingo. Mamm Rev. 2019;49(1):31–44.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  88. Doherty TS, Davis RA, van Etten EJB, Algar D, Collier N, Dickman CR, et al. A continental-scale analysis of feral cat diet in Australia. J Biogeogr. 2015;42(5):964–75.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  89. Brook LA. Predator guild interactions in northern Australia: behaviour and ecology of an apex predator, the dingo Canis lupus dingo, and an introduced mesopredator, the feral cat Felis catus [PhD thesis]. Townsville: James Cook University 2013.

    Google Scholar 

  90. Corbett LK. The dingo in Australia and Asia. Sydney: University of New South Wales Press; 1995.

    Google Scholar 

  91. Bengsen A, Algar D, Ballard G, Buckmaster T, Comer S, Fleming PJ, et al. Feral cat home-range size varies predictably with landscape productivity and population density. J Zool. 2016;298(2):112–20.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  92. Newsome TM, Ballard G-A, Dickman CR, Fleming PJS, van de Ven R. Home range, activity and sociality of a top predator, the dingo: a test of the resource dispersion hypothesis. Ecography. 2013;36(8):914–25.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  93. Moseby KE, Stott J, Crisp H. Movement patterns of feral predators in an arid environment–implications for control through poison baiting. Wildl Res. 2009;36(5):422–35.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  94. Edwards GP, Preu N, Crealy IV, Shakeshaft BJ. Habitat selection by feral cats and dingoes in a semi-arid woodland environment in Central Australia. Austral Ecol. 2002;27(1):26–31.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  95. Morton SR, James CD. The diversity and abundance of lizards in arid Australia: a new hypothesis. Am Nat. 1988:237–56.

  96. Frank ASK, Wardle GM, Dickman CR, Greenville AC. Habitat-and rainfall-dependent biodiversity responses to cattle removal in an arid woodland–grassland environment. Ecol Appl. 2014;24(8):2013–28.

    Article  PubMed  Google Scholar 

  97. Schuette P, Wagner AP, Wagner ME, Creel S. Occupancy patterns and niche partitioning within a diverse carnivore community exposed to anthropogenic pressures. Biol Conserv. 2013;158:301–12.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  98. Wang Y, Allen ML, Wilmers CC. Mesopredator spatial and temporal responses to large predators and human development in the Santa Cruz Mountains of California. Biol Conserv. 2015;190:23–33.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  99. Torretta E, Serafini M, Puopolo F, Schenone L. Spatial and temporal adjustments allowing the coexistence among carnivores in Liguria (NW Italy). Acta Ethol. 2016;19(2):123–32.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  100. Fancourt BA, Hawkins CE, Cameron EZ, Jones ME, Nicol SC. Devil declines and catastrophic cascades: is mesopredator release of feral cats inhibiting recovery of the eastern quoll? PLoS One. 2015;10(3):e0119303.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  CAS  Google Scholar 

  101. Latham ADM, Latham MC, Boyce MS, Boutin S. Movement responses by wolves to industrial linear features and their effect on woodland caribou in northeastern Alberta. Ecol Appl. 2011;21(8):2854–65.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  102. Whittington-Jones BM, Parker DM, Bernard RTF, Davies-Mostert HT. Habitat selection by transient African wild dogs (Lycaon pictus) in northern KwaZulu-Natal, South Africa: implications for range expansion. S Afr J Wildl Res. 2011;44(2):135–47.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  103. Doherty TS, Bengsen AJ, Davis RA. A critical review of habitat use by feral cats and key directions for future research and management. Wildl Res. 2014;41(5):435.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  104. Santos MJ, Matos HM, Palomares F, Santos-Reis M. Factors affecting mammalian carnivore use of riparian ecosystems in Mediterranean climates. J Mammal. 2011;92(5):1060–9.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  105. Phoebus I, Segelbacher G, Stenhouse GB. Do large carnivores use riparian zones? Ecological implications for forest management. For Ecol Manag. 2017;402:157–65.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  106. Hilty JA, Merenlender AM. Use of riparian corridors and vineyards by mammalian predators in northern California. Conserv Biol. 2004;18(1):126–35.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  107. Buckmaster AJ. Ecology of the feral cat (Felis catus) in the tall forests of far east Gippsland [PhD thesis]. Sydney: The University of Sydney; 2012.

    Google Scholar 

  108. McGregor HW, Legge S, Jones ME, Johnson CN. Landscape management of fire and grazing regimes alters the fine-scale habitat utilisation by feral cats. PLoS One. 2014;9(10):e109097.

    Article  PubMed  PubMed Central  CAS  Google Scholar 

  109. Allen BL. Do desert dingoes drink daily? Visitation rates at remote waterpoints in the Strzelecki Desert. Aust Mammal. 2012;34(2):251–6.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  110. Abbott I. Origin and spread of the cat, Felis catus, on mainland Australia, with a discussion of the magnitude of its early impact on native fauna. Wildl Res. 2002;29(1):51–74.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  111. Dawson TJ. Kangaroos. Melbourne: CSIRO Publishing; 2012.

    Book  Google Scholar 

  112. Mahon PS, Bates PB, Dickman CR. Population indices for wild carnivores: a critical study in sand-dune habitat, south-western Queensland. Wildl Res. 1998;25(2):217.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  113. Geary WL, Ritchie EG, Lawton JA, Healey TR, Nimmo DG. Incorporating disturbance into trophic ecology: fire history shapes mesopredator suppression by an apex predator. J Appl Ecol. 2018;55(4):1594–603.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  114. Forman RTT, Alexander LE. Roads and their major ecological effects. Annu Rev Ecol Syst. 1998:207–31.

  115. Trombulak SC, Frissell CA. Review of ecological effects of roads on terrestrial and aquatic communities. Conserv Biol. 2000;14(1):18–30.

    Article  Google Scholar 

  116. Harmsen BJ, Foster RJ, Silver S, Ostro L, Doncaster CP. Differential use of trails by forest mammals and the implications for camera-trap studies: a case study from Belize. Biotropica. 2010;42(1):126–33.

    Article  Google Scholar 

Download references

Acknowledgements

We thank the Western Australian Department of Biodiversity, Conservation and Attractions (DBCA) and the Wiluna Martu Aboriginal community for access to the site and assistance with collections and logistics. We particularly thank Katherine Zudnic for her assistance with GIS data file access and advice on habitat map construction, and Michael Craig for his early input on the manuscript. We also give special thanks to Malcolm Kennedy, Huw Nolan and Benjamin Allen for their dingo trapping and collaring training and advice. Additionally, we thank the following DBCA staff and volunteers for their field assistance: Colleen Sims, Kelly Rayner, Mike Lohr, Judy Dunlop, Heather Denham, Joe Porter, Cara McGary and Tim Doherty.

Funding

Funding for this project was provided by the WA Department of Biodiversity, Conservation and Attractions and the Gorgon Gas Development offset program. MLW was supported by an International Postgraduate Research Scholarship (IPRS) and Australian Postgraduate Award (APA) from the Australian Government and by a scholarship from the Australian Research Council, Centre of Excellence in Environmental Decisions (ARC-CEED).

Author information

Authors and Affiliations

Authors

Contributions

EGR and KM developed the research concept and KM provided the project funding and logistical support. MLW conducted all fieldwork, collected all the data and developed the map of landscape features. MLW and BAH processed all the data. GDI and BAH developed habitat selection models with assistance from MLW. LEV assisted in research design and provided early input on manuscript structure and outline. MLW wrote the manuscript and all authors discussed results and contributed to the final manuscript. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Corresponding author

Correspondence to Michael L. Wysong.

Ethics declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate

All data collection met the legal requirements for field research in Australia as defined under the Western Australian Animal Welfare Act 2002. Research for this project was approved by the University of Western Australia Animal Ethics Committee (RA/3/100/1277) and the Western Australian Department Biodiversity, Conservation and Attractions (DPaW AEC 2013–21). All research was conducted with the permission and support of the DBCA and the local Wiluna Martu Aboriginal community who hold title over Matuwa IPA. Access to adjacent properties, when needed, was done so with landholder permission. No threatened or protected species were involved in this research.

Consent for publication

Not applicable.

Competing interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Additional information

Publisher’s Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Supplementary information

Additional file 1: Table S1.

Details of animal capture and GPS-collar data collection for dingoes and feral cats at Matuwa Indigenous Protected Area, Western Australia. Table S2. Parameter estimates and odds-ratios for mixed-effects step-selection function models of dingo and feral cat habitat selection at Matuwa Indigenous Protected Area and surrounding properties in semi-arid Western Australia. Table S3. Results of sensitivity tests with data grouped by serial autocorrelated lags. Figure S1. Logistic regressions of proportional availability of woodland and grassland in the used and randomly-sampled available home ranges for dingoes and feral cats.

Additional file 2.

Asymptote analysis and KDE utilisation distributions for individual dingo home ranges.

Additional file 3.

Asymptote analysis and KDE utilization distributions for individual feral cat home ranges.

Rights and permissions

Open Access This article is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License, which permits use, sharing, adaptation, distribution and reproduction in any medium or format, as long as you give appropriate credit to the original author(s) and the source, provide a link to the Creative Commons licence, and indicate if changes were made. The images or other third party material in this article are included in the article's Creative Commons licence, unless indicated otherwise in a credit line to the material. If material is not included in the article's Creative Commons licence and your intended use is not permitted by statutory regulation or exceeds the permitted use, you will need to obtain permission directly from the copyright holder. To view a copy of this licence, visit http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/. The Creative Commons Public Domain Dedication waiver (http://creativecommons.org/publicdomain/zero/1.0/) applies to the data made available in this article, unless otherwise stated in a credit line to the data.

Reprints and permissions

About this article

Check for updates. Verify currency and authenticity via CrossMark

Cite this article

Wysong, M.L., Hradsky, B.A., Iacona, G.D. et al. Space use and habitat selection of an invasive mesopredator and sympatric, native apex predator. Mov Ecol 8, 18 (2020). https://doi.org/10.1186/s40462-020-00203-z

Download citation

  • Received:

  • Accepted:

  • Published:

  • DOI: https://doi.org/10.1186/s40462-020-00203-z

Keywords