Skip to main content
Advertisement
  • Loading metrics

Age-related cognitive decline and associations with sex, education and apolipoprotein E genotype across ethnocultural groups and geographic regions: a collaborative cohort study

Abstract

Background

The prevalence of dementia varies around the world, potentially contributed to by international differences in rates of age-related cognitive decline. Our primary goal was to investigate how rates of age-related decline in cognitive test performance varied among international cohort studies of cognitive aging. We also determined the extent to which sex, educational attainment, and apolipoprotein E ε4 allele (APOE*4) carrier status were associated with decline.

Methods and findings

We harmonized longitudinal data for 14 cohorts from 12 countries (Australia, Brazil, France, Greece, Hong Kong, Italy, Japan, Singapore, Spain, South Korea, United Kingdom, United States), for a total of 42,170 individuals aged 54–105 y (42% male), including 3.3% with dementia at baseline. The studies began between 1989 and 2011, with all but three ongoing, and each had 2–16 assessment waves (median = 3) and a follow-up duration of 2–15 y. We analyzed standardized Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE) and memory, processing speed, language, and executive functioning test scores using linear mixed models, adjusted for sex and education, and meta-analytic techniques. Performance on all cognitive measures declined with age, with the most rapid rate of change pooled across cohorts a moderate -0.26 standard deviations per decade (SD/decade) (95% confidence interval [CI] [-0.35, -0.16], p < 0.001) for processing speed. Rates of decline accelerated slightly with age, with executive functioning showing the largest additional rate of decline with every further decade of age (-0.07 SD/decade, 95% CI [-0.10, -0.03], p = 0.002). There was a considerable degree of heterogeneity in the associations across cohorts, including a slightly faster decline (p = 0.021) on the MMSE for Asians (-0.20 SD/decade, 95% CI [-0.28, -0.12], p < 0.001) than for whites (-0.09 SD/decade, 95% CI [-0.16, -0.02], p = 0.009). Males declined on the MMSE at a slightly slower rate than females (difference = 0.023 SD/decade, 95% CI [0.011, 0.035], p < 0.001), and every additional year of education was associated with a rate of decline slightly slower for the MMSE (0.004 SD/decade less, 95% CI [0.002, 0.006], p = 0.001), but slightly faster for language (-0.007 SD/decade more, 95% CI [-0.011, -0.003], p = 0.001). APOE*4 carriers declined slightly more rapidly than non-carriers on most cognitive measures, with processing speed showing the greatest difference (-0.08 SD/decade, 95% CI [-0.15, -0.01], p = 0.019). The same overall pattern of results was found when analyses were repeated with baseline dementia cases excluded. We used only one test to represent cognitive domains, and though a prototypical one, we nevertheless urge caution in generalizing the results to domains rather than viewing them as test-specific associations. This study lacked cohorts from Africa, India, and mainland China.

Conclusions

Cognitive performance declined with age, and more rapidly with increasing age, across samples from diverse ethnocultural groups and geographical regions. Associations varied across cohorts, suggesting that different rates of cognitive decline might contribute to the global variation in dementia prevalence. However, the many similarities and consistent associations with education and APOE genotype indicate a need to explore how international differences in associations with other risk factors such as genetics, cardiovascular health, and lifestyle are involved. Future studies should attempt to use multiple tests for each cognitive domain and feature populations from ethnocultural groups and geographical regions for which we lacked data.

Author summary

Why was this study done?

  • The prevalence of dementia varies around the world, but it is not known whether international differences in rates of cognitive decline contribute to this.
  • The extent to which risk and protective factors such as sex, education, and apolipoprotein E ε4 allele (APOE*4) carrier status have different associations with dementia in different ethnocultural groups and geographic regions is also not known.

What did the researchers do and find?

  • We analyzed cognitive performance data from 42,170 mostly elderly individuals, provided by 14 studies of aging representing 12 countries (Australia, Brazil, France, Greece, Hong Kong, Italy, Japan, Singapore, Spain, South Korea, United Kingdom, United States).
  • The Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE) and memory, processing speed, language, and executive functioning test scores all declined with age, and rates of decline accelerated with age.
  • The 14 studies showed different rates of decline, and decline in MMSE scores was faster for Asians than whites, females than males, and APOE*4 carriers than non-carriers. APOE*4 carriers also declined faster than non-carriers on test of memory, processing speed, and language.

What do these findings mean?

  • International differences in rates of cognitive decline might contribute to the global variation in dementia prevalence.
  • Further research is needed to determine whether cardiovascular health, lifestyle, and other risk factors for dementia have different associations with cognitive decline in different ethnocultural groups and geographic regions.

Introduction

The age-specific prevalence of dementia varies around the world, reportedly being the highest in Latin America and lowest in sub-Saharan Africa [1]. While age-specific prevalence is a good indicator of the population burden of dementia, the relative risk of dementia in different countries is better reflected in the age-specific incidence data. Unfortunately, such data are frequently lacking, especially in low- and middle-income countries. A reasonable proxy for dementia incidence is the rate of cognitive decline, with the expectation that more rapid cognitive decline will lead to higher rates of dementia in one population than another. Differences in the rates of cognitive decline may also contribute to global variation in late-life cognitive deficits less severe than dementia, such as the prevalence of mild cognitive impairment (MCI), which varies even when applying uniform diagnostic criteria [2], and performance on immediate word list recall tasks [3].

Different rates of cognitive decline have been a focus of research seeking to account for lower cognitive performance scores and more prevalent dementia among blacks than whites in the United States [4]. However, the results have been mixed, with some studies suggesting faster decline in whites [5,6] but others suggesting faster decline in blacks [7,8] or no difference between blacks and whites [7,9,10]. There are only a few studies comparing rates of late-life cognitive decline among international cohorts. One of these studies reported a similar rate of decline in immediate word list recall scores across samples from Europe, the US, China, and Mexico but a slower rate of decline in a sample from India [3]. However, not only were these findings based on one type of cognitive task, they were derived primarily from cross-sectional data and, thus, complicated by cohort effects. Another study found no difference in how Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE) scores declined with age across six samples from four countries [11], but all of these had predominantly white populations with presumably less cultural and socioeconomic disparities than present among a broader range of international regions.

Given the current state of the research, it is not yet known whether different rates of cognitive decline contribute to the global variation in cognitive functioning and rates of dementia. Also unknown is the extent to which risk and protective factors have different associations with cognitive decline in different ethnocultural groups and geographic regions. One important factor is sex, with decline in cognitive performance found to occur more rapidly in women than men in a Chinese cohort, but not in a Danish cohort [12]. The apolipoprotein E ε4 allele (APOE*4) is an established risk factor for Alzheimer disease (AD) [13] and for the transition from MCI to AD [14]. However, the prevalence of APOE*4 among AD patients varies across geographic regions and is significantly lower in Asia than in Europe and North America [15]. Educational attainment has been considered a likely determinant of cognitive decline rates, but research has been inconclusive, with different (even opposite) effects found in different cohorts [7]. Educational attainment is also a factor that varies substantially among late-life cohorts from around the world (e.g., as shown in Sachdev et al. [2]).

The Cohort Studies of Memory in an International Consortium (COSMIC) is a collaborative effort comprising members from around the world who share data from current or past longitudinal population-based studies of cognitive aging [16]. For the current study, data were available for 14 cohorts, representing 12 countries from North and South America, Europe, Asia, and Australia. Our primary goal was to harmonize these data and compare the rates of age-related decline on various types of cognitive tests across the samples. We also aimed to investigate the extent to which sex, educational attainment, and APOE*4 carrier status were associated with decline. Knowing whether rates of cognitive decline differ across different ethnocultural groups and geographic regions will help to clarify why there is global variation in cognitive functioning and prevalence of dementia. With such a diverse overall sample, our study should also help to clarify how sex, education, and APOE*4 carrier status are associated with cognitive decline.

Methods

Contributing studies and participants

The total sample size of 42,170 individuals for this project was arrived at by combining the samples of all 14 COSMIC studies contributing longitudinal cohort data (listed in Table 1 with their abbreviations). In most cases the full cohort was not used, as we excluded individuals missing data for age, sex, or years of education. The samples we used varied in size from 785 to 12,630 individuals.

Contributing studies had various assessment schedules and follow-up durations. The number of assessment waves (including baseline) was two for six studies, three for five studies, four for two studies, and 16 for two studies (Bambui and EAS), and the maximum follow-up duration was between 2 and 10 y for all studies except Bambui and EAS (each 15 y). For CFAS, the number and type of follow-up assessments differed among the participants (see http://www.cfas.ac.uk/cfas-i/cfasistudy-design/), and we used an abridged schedule comprising baseline and two follow-up waves that captured the majority of participants (waves S0, C2/S2, CX). For each cohort and assessment wave, the number of participants assessed and the average time since baseline are shown in S1S3 Tables.

Ethics approval

This COSMIC project was approved by the University of New South Wales Human Research Ethics Committee (Ref: # HC12446). Each of the 14 contributing studies had previously obtained ethics approval from their respective institutional review boards, and all participants provided informed consent (see S4 Table for details). Further participant consent was not required, as de-identified health data are not considered to be protected health information under current research principles (e.g., as per the Privacy Rule proposed by the National Institutes of Health: http://privacyruleandresearch.nih.gov/research_repositories.asp).

Measures

We obtained information on age, sex, educational attainment, and dementia status at baseline from all studies. Data for educational attainment were provided as years by all studies except ESPRIT, for which categories (e.g., higher primary, long technical or professional) had to be assigned discrete year values based on informed decisions. All but four studies also provided APOE*4 carrier status data (see the references in Table 1 for collection details), which we classified as carriers of one or two ε4 alleles versus non-carriers. Cognitive performance was assessed with scores for the MMSE [31] and for neuropsychological tests representing each of four cognitive domains: memory, language, processing speed, and executive functioning (a visuospatial domain was not included because there were not enough common relevant tests across the cohorts). All studies except EAS and SPAH administered the MMSE. However, EAS administered the Blessed Information Memory Concentration test, and a validated formula was used to convert these scores to MMSE scores [32]. Scores for neuropsychological tests from one or more of the cognitive domains were available for all studies except Bambui, SGS, and ZARADEMP. For each of the domains, we used a single test or type of test as common to all studies as possible. For memory, this was a delayed word list recall test, though the particular test varied between studies. The most commonly used memory test was the Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test [33]. For studies without a specific memory test, we used the MMSE three-word recall sub-score. Tests were allocated to the remaining domains in a manner reflecting common practice [3335], though we acknowledge that opinions vary and our approach may differ from how the studies previously allocated tests to domains. We allocated semantic fluency tests, typically the number of animals named in 60s [33], to language (as per Ganguli et al. [36]). Trail Making Tests A and B [33] were allocated to processing speed and executive functioning, respectively (as endorsed by others, e.g., Lim et al. [37]). The tests from each study and variations in type or nonstandard administration are detailed in S5 Table.

Statistical analysis

The following analyses were performed separately for each cohort and each test. First, scores greater than three standard deviations (SDs) from the mean were considered outliers and excluded (the proportions of outliers and other missing scores are shown in S6 Table). Where required, a logarithmic or other transformation was applied to reduce a distribution’s absolute value of skewness from >1 to <1 before identifying outliers. Next, linear mixed models with random effects terms for intercept and age (but not age2) were applied to the original, untransformed data (with outliers removed) to produce estimates of the mean and SD for common values of age (75 y), education (9 y), and sex (50% female). These estimates were used to transform the raw test scores to standardized Z-scores by subtracting the estimated means from the raw scores and then dividing this difference by the estimated SD. These Z-scores were then used in analyses examining longitudinal associations with age, sex, education, and APOE*4 carrier status. Across the contributing studies, standardized scores for the different tests (or different versions or administration protocols for the same test) used to represent a domain were regarded as equivalent in that they provided a comparable metric to compare effect sizes for relationships across studies and between different types of tests.

The type of analysis employed was dependent upon whether the distribution of test scores was approximately symmetric (|skewness| <1) or more highly skewed (|skewness| >1). If approximately symmetric, linear mixed modelling was used, with fixed effects for age, age2, sex, education, and interactions of both sex and education with age, and with random effects for the intercept and age (but not age2). Age was centered at 75 y (approximately the mean age across all cohorts and waves) to reduce multicollinearity between age and age2. For more highly skewed distributions, we used generalized linear mixed effect modelling with the gamma distribution, featuring the same fixed and random effects as above. Note that because age was centered at 75 y, estimates of the fixed effects of age obtained from the above models (that include an age2 term in the equations) represent model estimates of longitudinal associations with age at 75 y. Associations with APOE*4 carrier status were also investigated by repeating the above analyses, with this variable, as well as its interaction with age, included in the model. The dominant genetic model was used, and there was no race-based stratification in comparisons of ε4 carriers and non-carriers.

Estimates of effect sizes pooled across samples were obtained by meta-analysis (using random effects models) and presented in forest plots. Heterogeneity of effect sizes among samples was evaluated with the I2 statistic, which is the percentage of the total variation that is due to variation between studies, rather than sampling error or chance. We report I2 values derived from fixed effects models that give more appropriate indications of variation across studies. We took values of I2 as corresponding to levels of heterogeneity that were low if less than 40%, moderate if 40%–60%, and substantial or considerable if greater than 60% (as per the Cochrane Collaboration [38]).

We repeated our analyses separately for two racial/ethnic groups, one with all individuals from cohorts predominantly comprising white participants (CFAS, ESPRIT, HELIAD, Invece.Ab, PATH, Sydney MAS, ZARADEMP) and one with all individuals from cohorts predominantly comprising Asian participants (HK-MAPS, KLOSCAD, SGS, SLASI). The statistical significance of differences in pooled corresponding cognitive measures between the two groups was obtained using the means and standard errors (SEs) of the pooled measure derived from the meta-analyses. The SE of the difference between two pooled measures (SEdiff) was calculated as the square root of the sum of the squares of the SEs of the two pooled measures. Differences between the means greater than 1.96 times SEdiff were regarded as statistically significant.

Meta-analyses were also used to obtain pooled estimates of fixed effects of sex, education, and APOE*4 carrier status, as well as the interactions of these risk factors with age, and to examine how consistent these associations were across cohorts. Age was analyzed in years, but for ease of interpretation, Bs and 95% confidence intervals (CIs) are presented using age in decades. CIs were obtained as , where B is the estimate, SE(B) is the standard error of B, and is the upper 97.5% percentile point of the standard normal distribution.

In a number of study/test distributions, ceiling or floor effects had produced data spikes in which a relatively large proportion of scores were of either the minimum or maximum possible value. The most prominent reason for this was the termination of timed tests after a predetermined period and the recording of a score equal to that time. These scores were removed in order to achieve convergence or acceptable model fit (the numbers of scores removed are shown in S6 Table). We subsequently examined whether the removal of these scores affected our results by repeating the meta-analyses used to obtain pooled estimates of the fixed effects for each cognitive measure with studies featuring data spikes excluded.

Our primary analyses used data from all available individuals with sufficient information, including those identified by the contributing studies as having dementia at baseline. The inclusion of individuals with dementia at baseline meant that our evaluations of cognitive change were more likely to truly represent those of aging populations. However, as it is possible that individuals with dementia may decline at rates different from those without dementia, the analyses were repeated with cases of baseline dementia removed. The majority of studies diagnosed or classified dementia using DSM-IV criteria, with the exceptions being Bambui (an MMSE score cutoff point 13/14 appropriate for Brazilian populations with low schooling [39]), CFAS (AGECAT organicity level of O3), ESPRIT (standardized interview by a neurologist incorporating cognitive testing, with diagnoses validated by an independent panel of expert neurologists), HK-MAPS (Clinical Dementia Rating ≥1), and SGS (self-reported medical history). We note that these approaches are not harmonized or necessarily optimal for identifying dementia, including the case in which MMSE scores were used while other criteria for dementia, such as impaired functional ability, were not considered.

The Sydney team created the pooled dataset and performed the analyses. IBM SPSS Statistics 23 was used to create the dataset and identify outliers, the function Ime in the R (version 3.3.1) package mlme (https://www.r-project.org/) was used for linear mixed effects modelling, and the Penalised Quasi-Likelihood method implemented in the program glmPQL of the MASS package [40] was used for generalized linear mixed effects modelling. The meta-analyses were conducted and forest plots made using the metafor package in R [41].

Results

Sample description

Table 2 shows the demographic characteristics of the 14 cohorts contributing to our longitudinal analyses. All of the cohorts except one (PATH) had a greater proportion of females than males. For nine of the cohorts, the age of the youngest participant at baseline was 60 or more years (it was no less than 54 y for the remaining cohorts). The design of both Invece.Ab and PATH led to their cohorts having a much narrower age range than others. While most cohorts contained some participants with no formal schooling, the mean number of years of education varied considerably. Participants from the Brazilian cohorts (Bambui and SPAH) had the fewest years of formal education. Each cohort was essentially homogenous for race/ethnicity, except for EAS (approximately two-thirds white and one-third black), Bambui (white, black, and Brazilian indigenous native), and SPAH (mostly mixed race and white). Across the cohorts, the total number of individuals with APOE*4 data was 15,199, and 22.9% of these were APOE*4 carriers. However, the proportion of APOE*4 carriers varied across the cohorts, being lowest for the two comprising predominantly Chinese participants (HK-MAPS and SLASI). Across all cohorts, the proportion of individuals with dementia at baseline was between almost zero and 5.8% (not counting two studies that excluded individuals with dementia during recruitment: HK-MAPS and Sydney MAS). The overall proportion of individuals with dementia at baseline was 3.3%.

Rates of cognitive decline

Fixed effects of age and age2.

Across all cohorts, analyses adjusting for sex and education revealed statistically significant negative pooled associations between age and performance on the MMSE and all four cognitive domains (Fig 1, values in S7 Table). This demonstrates decline, with the rate most rapid for processing speed (-0.26 Z-score units per decade, 95% CI [-0.35, -0.16], p < 0.001) and least rapid for the MMSE (-0.12 Z-score units per decade, 95% CI [-0.17, -0.06], p < 0.001). There was a relatively high level of consistency in the direction of these associations with age but a considerable degree of variation in their size across cohorts, with high levels of heterogeneity indicated by I2 values between 86.2% and 98.9%.

thumbnail
Fig 1. Forest plots for associations between age and Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE) and cognitive domain scores.

The x-axis plots change in Z-score units per decade; negative values indicate decline with age.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pmed.1002261.g001

Results showed small but statistically significant negative pooled associations between age2 and all cognitive measures, indicating decline becoming more rapid with increasing age, with the strongest associations detected for processing speed and executive functioning (S1 Fig, values in S8 Table). The associations with age2 exhibited high levels of heterogeneity among the cohorts for the MMSE and each of the cognitive domains, with values of I2 ranging from 74.0% to 96.3%.

Fig 2 displays the longitudinal trajectories with age for the MMSE and each of the cognitive domains, calculated using the pooled estimates of the fixed effects of age and age2 adjusted for sex and education (as per S7 and S8 Tables), as well as 95% confidence bands calculated from the standard errors of these pooled estimates and the intercept. Each panel shows declining performance with increasing age; steepening of the slopes with age reflects the negative age2 fixed effects.

thumbnail
Fig 2. Longitudinal variation with age for the Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE) and cognitive domains.

Results were calculated from pooled estimates of the fixed effects of age and age2, adjusted for sex and education. Dashed lines indicate 95% confidence bands.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pmed.1002261.g002

Rates of decline for white and Asian groups.

Fig 3 shows the fixed effects of age and age2 pooled across samples for each cognitive measure, as well as the weighted average of the pooled cognitive measures, separately for the white and Asian groups (values in S9S12 Tables). Mirroring the finding for all cohorts analyzed as a whole, the most rapid rate of decline within each of the white and Asian groups was for processing speed. The Asian group had a slight overall tendency for higher rates of decline with age than the white group, as reflected in the small difference in the averages of pooled scores shown in Fig 3. However, across all cognitive measures and comparisons of age and age2 effects, the only statistically significant group difference was for the fixed effect of age on MMSE scores (Bdiff = 0.0110, 95% CI [0.0004, 0.0211], p = 0.021), with a faster rate of decline in the Asian group than in the white group. Fig 3 also suggests that the difference in the strengths of the pooled age2 fixed effects between processing speed and language is greater for whites than for Asians.

thumbnail
Fig 3. Pooled values of age and age2 fixed effects on Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE) and cognitive domain scores for white and Asian groups.

The x-axis plots change in Z-score units per decade; negative values indicate decline with age (left panel) and accelerating decline with age (right panel).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pmed.1002261.g003

Associations with sex, education, and APOE*4 carrier status, as well as their interactions with age

Sex.

Values for the pooled associations with sex, and its interaction with age, are shown for each cognitive measure in S13 and S14 Tables. The strongest association with sex was for memory (B = -0.16, 95% CI [-0.25, -0.08], p < 0.001), with females performing better than males. Males tended to perform better than females on all other cognitive measures, but not statistically significantly so for any measure. The interactions of sex with age were positive for all cognitive measures except processing speed, which was near zero and nonsignificant (p = 0.795), indicating a trend toward slightly slower decline with age for males than for females. However, this slightly slower decline for males was only statistically significant for the MMSE (B = 0.023, 95% CI [0.011, 0.035], p < 0.001).

Education.

The values for the pooled associations between education and each of the cognitive measures are shown in S15 Table. Having more years of education was associated with significantly better performance on all cognitive measures. The strongest effect was for language, which exhibited an almost 0.05 Z-score unit increase for every additional year of education (B = 0.049, 95% CI [0.032, 0.066], p < 0.001), and the lowest for the MMSE (B = 0.011, 95% CI [0.007, 0.016], p < 0.001). Statistically significant interactions between education and age were found for the MMSE (B = 0.004, 95% CI [0.002, 0.006], p = 0.001) and language (B = -0.007, 95% CI [-0.011, -0.003], p < 0.001), with more years of education associated with a slower decline in MMSE scores but a faster decline in language performance (S16 Table).

APOE*4 carrier status.

The values for the pooled associations between APOE*4 carrier status and each of the cognitive measures are shown in S17 Table. The strongest associations were with memory (B = -0.055, 95% CI [-0.075, -0.035], p < 0.001) and processing speed (B = -0.043, 95% CI [-0.060, -0.025], p < 0.001), for each of which the performance of APOE*4 carriers was around 0.05 Z-score units below that of noncarriers. APOE*4 carriers also performed significantly lower than noncarriers on the MMSE, but the negative associations with language and executive functioning were not statistically significant. There were statistically significant interactions between APOE*4 carrier status and age for all cognitive measures except executive functioning (S18 Table). The strongest interaction was for processing speed (B = -0.08, 95% CI [-0.15, -0.01], p = 0.019), but this was not significantly different from the interaction with any of the other measures. For all significant associations, the negative value for B indicates greater decline in cognition with age for APOE*4 carriers than for noncarriers. The results for both APOE*4 carrier status and APOE*4 carrier status by age interactions exhibited extremely low heterogeneity among the cohorts for memory and language (0.0% < I2 < 0.5%).

Repeat analyses.

In order to achieve model convergence or adequate fit, our initial analyses excluded scores that formed data spikes, which were groups of scores reflecting ceiling or floor effects most prominently associated with the termination of timed tests at a predetermined time. We repeated our meta-analyses with studies featuring data spikes excluded and observed only one change to the original pooled values: the emergence of significantly lower executive functioning performance for APOE*4 carries than for noncarriers (B = -0.22, 95% CI [-0.42, -0.03], p = 0.025). We also performed a separate round of repeat analyses with cases of dementia at baseline removed (the pooled values of estimated fixed effects are shown in the last rows of S7S18 Tables). While the overall pattern of results remained similar to that originally found, there were a small number of changes in the results when comparing whites and Asians and when investigating associations with sex, education, and APOE*4 carrier status. However, as can be seen in S7S18 Tables, in most instances this involved only a small change in p-value and no substantial change in effect size.

Discussion

We used individual participant-level data provided by members of the COSMIC collaboration to investigate rates of cognitive decline in 14 longitudinal population-based studies of cognitive aging, representing 12 countries and 5 continents. We also investigated the extent to which sex, education, and APOE*4 carrier status were associated with cognitive performance and decline across these diverse ethnocultural groups and geographic regions. Our findings were minimally affected when repeating our analyses with cases of baseline dementia removed, probably in large part because the overall proportion of these cases was low (3.3%).

Main findings

The cognitive measures analyzed in this study were the MMSE and tests representing four cognitive domains: memory, language, processing speed, and executive functioning. For all of these measures and across all cohorts, we found performance to not only decline substantially with age, but to decline more rapidly with increasing age. Processing speed exhibited the strongest decline with age, and the MMSE exhibited the weakest. The rate of age-related change in processing speed was almost -0.25 Z-score units per decade, which was not too dissimilar to the rates of decline for memory and executive functioning but twice the rate of decline we found for the MMSE. This is consistent with previous reports of age-related associations being stronger for processing speed, intermediate for memory, and weaker for language [42]. Processing speed being the cognitive measure most strongly associated with age could be seen as supporting the processing speed theory of cognitive aging [43]. However, it was not our aim to investigate this, and further analyses examining the extent to which change in performance on other domains is driven by changes in processing speed would be required to test this idea. The slowest rate of decline being found for the MMSE could stem from this measure being insensitive to changes at high levels of cognition [44].

Rates of cognitive decline and changes in the rates of decline with age exhibited a degree of heterogeneity across the cohorts. The direction of associations with age was highly consistent across cohorts for all cognitive measures (with no instance of significant improvement rather than decline), but the strength differed. These differences remained when cases of baseline dementia were excluded, suggesting that they could lead to international differences in rates of incident dementia, and thereby contribute to the global variation in prevalence of dementia [1]. Future COSMIC projects will aim to harmonize data on incident dementia across the cohorts and match these to rates of cognitive decline and prevalence of dementia.

Race/ethnicity

Despite the differences seen across all cohorts, our initial results indicated only one significant difference in rates of decline or change in rates of decline between groups of cohorts classified as white or Asian: a slightly faster decline with age on the MMSE in the Asian group. There also seemed to be a group difference in the strengths of the pooled age2 fixed effects between processing speed and language, which was greater for whites than for Asians. Analyses with cases of baseline dementia removed showed some additional age2 effects within each group, amplifying the differences between whites and Asians. Further research is needed to determine the reliability of these differences and their implications.

Sex

Across our cohorts, females generally performed better than males on verbal memory tests, although the difference was not large. Previously reported differences in late-life memory performance between men and women have varied depending on where the samples were from. Reports of better memory in women have come from developed nations, including the UK [45], US [46], and Denmark [12]. Conversely, women have shown poorer memory performance than men in samples from developing nations or where women have historically not been afforded the same educational opportunities as men, including India [47] and China [48]. Nevertheless, it should be noted that our findings were relatively consistent across the diverse range of cohorts investigated, including some that may be from developing nations. Better verbal memory performance in women than men could arise via an effect of estrogen [49] or sex-specific cognitive reserve [50]. Our initial finding of faster decline in MMSE scores for females than for males is ostensibly consistent with reports that women exhibit both a steeper decline in general cognition with increasing age [51] and a greater prevalence of AD [52]. However, there was only a trend for this association (p = 0.089) after excluding baseline dementia cases from our analyses. Future COSMIC projects will use harmonized incidence of dementia data to more fully examine sex differences in cognitive decline.

Education

Previous research has consistently found higher levels of educational attainment to be associated with better late-life cognitive functioning [7,53,54], but associations between education and rates of cognitive decline are mixed [7]. Our finding that more years of education was associated with better performance on all cognitive measures is consistent with this. Also consistent are declines with age that were slower for the MMSE but faster for language, though the reasons for the mixed directions of these associations are unclear.

APOE*4

Compared to noncarriers, APOE*4 carriers performed worse on memory, processing speed, and the MMSE (as well as on executive functioning in analyses with studies featuring data spikes excluded). APOE*4 carriers also exhibited greater rates of decline than noncarriers for all measures except executive functioning. Such findings are not unexpected given that APOE*4 is a risk factor for AD [13] and for the transition from MCI to AD [14]. With cases of baseline dementia excluded from our analyses, APOE*4 carriers continued to show significantly poorer performance only for memory, which fits with a recent meta-analysis finding memory to be the cognitive measure most strongly affected in APOE*4 carriers with no diagnosed cognitive impairment [55]. The reasons for extremely low heterogeneity among the cohorts for associations with APOE*4 carrier status and APOE*4 carrier status by age interactions on memory and language are unclear. We note that the differences in APOE*4 carrier prevalence across our cohorts are generally consistent with previously reported racial/ethnic differences [56], particularly the relatively low prevalence for the Chinese (HK-MAPS and SLASI) and Italian (Invece.Ab) cohorts.

Strengths and limitations

Strengths of our study include the large number of independent cohorts from diverse geographical, ethnic, and sociocultural groups and the use of the same or very similar cognitive tests by these studies. Even with analyses based on standardized scores, we expect that the use of common tests helped to minimize heterogeneity across the studies within each of the cognitive domains investigated. Nevertheless, with only one test being used to represent cognitive domains, we caution against generalizing our results to domains rather than viewing them as test-specific associations, although it is noteworthy that the tests used were prototypical of their domains, and that for the memory domain a variety of verbal memory tests were used across the cohorts. We also note that the MMSE has been criticized as psychometrically unsound for assessing cognitive change in healthy older adults [57] and prone to practice effects [58]. Indeed, with the same cognitive tests used repeatedly in all assessment waves, it is possible we underestimated age-related change because of practice effects. Being reportedly stronger in younger adults [59], practice effects could partially explain increasing rates of decline with increasing age. Other limitations include the cohorts differing in size, number of assessment waves, and overall follow-up duration. Despite all being population-based, the use of particular strategies for recruitment and regional specificity may mean that the cohorts are not necessarily representative of the countries or entire populations they were from. Our study did not have data on chronic degenerative diseases or cardiovascular and lifestyle-related factors commonly associated with aging. These factors could have independent associations with cognitive decline, and not controlling for them may lead to overestimating the strength of associations between age and cognitive decline. Limitations also come with having to harmonize some data from among a heterogeneous group of studies. For example, the use of different memory tests by the studies entailed differences in the range of possible scores, which, despite harmonization, potentially influenced the variability within studies and, thus, also potentially influenced our findings of differences between studies.

Conclusion

In conclusion, we found that cognitive performance consistently declined with age, and more rapidly with increasing age, across cohorts from a diverse range of ethnocultural groups and geographical regions. Similar patterns of results were found for analyses that either included or excluded individuals with dementia at baseline. The strengths of the observed associations varied across the cohorts, and there were also some small differences between groups of cohorts classified as white or Asian. This suggests that different rates of cognitive decline might contribute, via different rates of incident dementia, to the global variation in dementia prevalence. Given the diversity of cohorts and our large overall sample size (more than 42,000 individuals), the associations with sex, education, and APOE genotype we found should help to clarify the contributions of these factors to cognitive ageing on a global scale. We intend for future research with COSMIC cohorts to explore how risk factors not investigated in the current study, including other genetic, epigenetic, cardiovascular, and lifestyle-related factors, contribute to cognitive decline and neurocognitive disorders, and to determine the extent to which their associations vary internationally. We also aim to feature populations from ethnocultural groups and geographical regions for which the current study lacked data, including Africa, India, and mainland China. This will provide important information for developing efficacious interventions to prevent or minimize cognitive impairment and dementia in the rapidly aging population worldwide.

Supporting information

S1 Fig. Forest plots for the quadratic effects of age on Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE) and cognitive domain scores.

Z- and p-values are for the statistical tests of significance of the pooled values.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pmed.1002261.s002

(TIF)

S1 Table. Number of assessment waves, time since baseline, and number of individuals assessed with the MMSE for baseline and each follow-up wave.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pmed.1002261.s003

(DOCX)

S2 Table. Time (y) since baseline and number of individuals assessed with the Mini-Mental State Examination for the 16 annual Bambui study assessment waves.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pmed.1002261.s004

(DOCX)

S3 Table. Time (y, mean ± SD, and range) since baseline and number of individuals assessed with the Blessed Information Memory Concentration test for each of the 16 EAS assessment waves.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pmed.1002261.s005

(DOCX)

S4 Table. Ethics approvals for the individual contributing studies.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pmed.1002261.s006

(DOCX)

S5 Table. Tests from each contributing study used to represent the cognitive domains investigated.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pmed.1002261.s007

(DOCX)

S6 Table. Numbers and percentages of missing and deleted test scores.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pmed.1002261.s008

(DOCX)

S7 Table. Meta-analyses of the fixed effects of age on MMSE and cognitive domain scores.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pmed.1002261.s009

(DOCX)

S8 Table. Meta-analyses of the fixed effects of age2 on MMSE and cognitive domain scores.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pmed.1002261.s010

(DOCX)

S9 Table. For predominantly white studies, meta-analyses of the fixed effects of age on MMSE and cognitive domain scores.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pmed.1002261.s011

(DOCX)

S10 Table. For predominantly white studies, meta-analyses of the fixed effects of age2 on MMSE and cognitive domain scores.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pmed.1002261.s012

(DOCX)

S11 Table. For predominantly Asian studies, meta-analyses of the fixed effects of age on MMSE and cognitive domain scores.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pmed.1002261.s013

(DOCX)

S12 Table. For predominantly Asian studies, meta-analyses of the fixed effects of age2 on MMSE and cognitive domain scores.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pmed.1002261.s014

(DOCX)

S13 Table. Meta-analyses of the fixed effects of sex on MMSE and cognitive domain scores.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pmed.1002261.s015

(DOCX)

S14 Table. Meta-analyses of sex by age interactions on MMSE and cognitive domain scores.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pmed.1002261.s016

(DOCX)

S15 Table. Meta-analyses of the fixed effects of education on MMSE and cognitive domain scores.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pmed.1002261.s017

(DOCX)

S16 Table. Meta-analyses of education by age interactions on MMSE and cognitive domain scores.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pmed.1002261.s018

(DOCX)

S17 Table. Meta-analyses of the fixed effects of APOE*4 carrier status on MMSE and cognitive domain scores.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pmed.1002261.s019

(DOCX)

S18 Table. Meta-analyses of APOE*4 carrier status by age interactions on MMSE and cognitive domain scores.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pmed.1002261.s020

(DOCX)

S1 Text. Analysis plan for COSMIC paper on age-related cognitive decline in diverse international regions.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pmed.1002261.s021

(DOCX)

Acknowledgments

The authors’ affiliations with the contributing COSMIC studies are as follows: Bambui: MFLC (study leader), ECC; CFAS: CB (study leader), FEM, BCMS; EAS: RBL, MJK (joint study leaders); ESPRIT: KR (joint study leader), JSc, MLA (joint study leader); HELIAD: NS (study leader), MY, ED; HK-MAPS: LCWL (study leader), CHYW, AWTF; Invece.Ab: AG (study leader), RV, AD; KLOSCAD: KWK (study leader), JWH, THK; PATH: KJA (study leader), NC, PB; SPAH: MS (study leader); SGS: SK (study leader), SC, KN; SLASI: TPN (study leader), QG; Sydney MAS: DML, PSS (joint study leader), JDC, RD, AT, NAK, GA, SR, HB (joint study leader); ZARADEMP: AL (study leader), RLA, JSa.

The authors of particular studies acknowledge: EAS: the contributions of Molly Zimmerman and Carol Derby; ESPRIT: the further study member Isabelle Carriere; HELIAD: the further study members Mary Kosmidis, George M Hadjigeorgiou, Pararskevi Sakka, Elena Margioti, Xanthi Arabatzi, and the other contributors to the study; HK-MAPS: the contributions of Grace T.Y. Leung, Wai Chi Chan, Department of Health of Hong Kong SAR, the participants and the social centres for their assistance in the assessment, and thank Novartis and Astra Zeneca for their sponsorship of souvenirs for the participants in the baseline study; Invece.Ab: the further study members Letizia Polito, Simona Abbondanza, Eleonora Valle, Mauro Colombo, Silvia Francesca Vitali, Silvia Fossi, Daniele Zaccaria and the contributions of Gianluigi Forloni,”Mario Negri” Institute for Pharmacological Research, Milan, Italy and Simona Villani, University of Pavia, Pavia, Italy, and are also grateful to the relative’s association, “Federazione Alzheimer Italia”, Milan, Italy, for supporting the study. PATH: the further study members Helen Christensen, Andrew MacKinnon, Simon Easteal, Project Managers Trish Jacomb, Karen Maxwell, participants and the NHMRC. SLASI: and gratefully thank the help and support of the following voluntary welfare organizations: Geylang East Home for the Aged, Presbysterian Community Services, Thye Hua Kwan Moral Society (Moral Neighbourhood Links), Yuhua Neighbourhood Link, Henderson Senior Citizens’ Home, NTUC Eldercare Co-op Ltd, Thong Kheng Seniors Activity Centre (Queenstown Centre), Redhill Moral Seniors Activity Centre, SARAH Seniors Activity Centre, and Training, Research Academy at Jurong Point (TaRA@JP). Sydney MAS: the contributions of members of the MAS Team: Allison Bowman, Kim Burns, Anthony Broe, Joula Dekker, Louise Dooley, Michele de Permentier, Sarah Fairjones, Janelle Fletcher, Therese French, Cathy Foster, Emma Nugent-Cleary-Fox, Chien Gooi, Evelyn Harvey, Rebecca Helyer, Sharpley Hsieh, Laura Hughes, Sarah Jacek, Mary Johnston, Donna McCade, Samantha Meeth, Eveline Milne, Angharad Moir, Ros O’Grady, Kia Pfaeffli, Carine Pose, Laura Reuser, Amanda Rose, Peter Schofield, Zeeshan Shahnawaz, Amanda Sharpley, Claire Thompson, Wiebke Queisser, and Sam Wong. ZARADEMP: the further study members Guillermo Marcos, Concepción De-la-Cámara, Pedro Saz, Tirso Ventura, Miguel Angel Quintanilla, and Elena Lobo, and the contributions of the ZARADEMP Workgroup who participated in the study.

Author Contributions

  1. Conceptualization: PSS DML JDC NAK SK CB FEM BCMS CHYW LCWL MFLC ECC KJA AL NS ED MY RBL MJK GA KWK MS.
  2. Data curation: DML JDC RD AT AWTF NC SK SC KN TPN QG CHYW LCWL JSc CB FEM BCMS THK MFLC ECC KJA RLA JSa MJK JWH KWK PB MS.
  3. Formal analysis: RD AT SC NAK CHYW LCWL MFLC ECC RLA JSa NS MY JWH KWK MS FEM BCMS.
  4. Funding acquisition: PSS.
  5. Investigation: AWTF NC SK SC KN TPN QG CHYW LCWL AG RV AD MLA HB THK NAK MFLC ECC KJA AL RLA JSa NS ED MY KR RBL MJK SR JWH GA KWK PB MS CB FEM BCMS.
  6. Methodology: JDC AT SK SC KN TPN QG CB FEM LCWL KJA NS ED MY KWK MS.
  7. Project administration: DML.
  8. Software: JSa MJK KWK MS FEM BCMS.
  9. Supervision: PSS.
  10. Validation: CB FEM BCMS LCWL JSc MLA MFLC ECC KJA AL RLA JSa RBL MJK JWH KWK MS.
  11. Visualization: AT.
  12. Writing – original draft: DML JDC PSS.
  13. Writing – review & editing: DML PSS JDC RD AT NAK GA MFLC ECC CB FEM BCMS RBL MJK KR JSc MLA NS MY ED LCWL CHYW AWTF AG RV AD KWK JWH THK KJA NC PB MS KS CS KN TPN QG SR HB AL RLA JSa.

References

  1. 1. Prince M, Bryce R, Albanese E, Wimo A, Ribeiro W, Ferri CP. The global prevalence of dementia: a systematic review and metaanalysis. Alzheimers Dement. 2013;9(1):63–75 e2. pmid:23305823
  2. 2. Sachdev PS, Lipnicki DM, Kochan NA, Crawford JD, Thalamuthu A, Andrews G, et al. The Prevalence of Mild Cognitive Impairment in Diverse Geographical and Ethnocultural Regions: The COSMIC Collaboration. PLoS ONE. 2015;10(11):e0142388. pmid:26539987
  3. 3. Skirbekk V, Loichinger E, Weber D. Variation in cognitive functioning as a refined approach to comparing aging across countries. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2012;109(3):770–4. pmid:22184241
  4. 4. Lines LM, Sherif NA, Wiener JM. Racial and ethnic disparities among individuals with Alzheimer's disease in the United States: A literature review (RTI Press publication No. RR-0024-1412). Research Triangle Park (NC): 2014.
  5. 5. Early DR, Widaman KF, Harvey D, Beckett L, Park LQ, Farias ST, et al. Demographic predictors of cognitive change in ethnically diverse older persons. Psychol Aging. 2013;28(3):633–45. pmid:23437898
  6. 6. Wilson RS, Capuano AW, Sytsma J, Bennett DA, Barnes LL. Cognitive aging in older Black and White persons. Psychol Aging. 2015;30(2):279–85. pmid:25961876
  7. 7. Gross AL, Mungas DM, Crane PK, Gibbons LE, MacKay-Brandt A, Manly JJ, et al. Effects of education and race on cognitive decline: An integrative study of generalizability versus study-specific results. Psychol Aging. 2015;30(4):863–80. pmid:26523693
  8. 8. Wolinsky FD, Bentler SE, Hockenberry J, Jones MP, Weigel PA, Kaskie B, et al. A prospective cohort study of long-term cognitive changes in older Medicare beneficiaries. BMC Public Health. 2011;11:710. pmid:21933430
  9. 9. Castora-Binkley M, Peronto CL, Edwards JD, Small BJ. A longitudinal analysis of the influence of race on cognitive performance. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci. 2015;70(4):512–8. pmid:24184780
  10. 10. Vasquez E, Botoseneanu A, Bennett JM, Shaw BA. Racial/Ethnic Differences in Trajectories of Cognitive Function in Older Adults: Role of Education, Smoking, and Physical Activity. J Aging Health. 2015.
  11. 11. Piccinin AM, Muniz-Terrera G, Clouston S, Reynolds CA, Thorvaldsson V, Deary IJ, et al. Coordinated analysis of age, sex, and education effects on change in MMSE scores. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci. 2013;68(3):374–90. pmid:23033357
  12. 12. Wu Y, Zhang D, Pang Z, Oksuzyan A, Jiang W, Wang S, et al. Gender-specific patterns in age-related decline in general health among Danish and Chinese: a cross-national comparative study. Geriatr Gerontol Int. 2012;12(3):431–9. pmid:22212497
  13. 13. Corder EH, Saunders AM, Strittmatter WJ, Schmechel DE, Gaskell PC, Small GW, et al. Gene dose of apolipoprotein E type 4 allele and the risk of Alzheimer's disease in late onset families. Science. 1993;261(5123):921–3. pmid:8346443
  14. 14. Artero S, Ancelin ML, Portet F, Dupuy A, Berr C, Dartigues JF, et al. Risk profiles for mild cognitive impairment and progression to dementia are gender specific. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 2008;79(9):979–84. pmid:18450788
  15. 15. Crean S, Ward A, Mercaldi CJ, Collins JM, Cook MN, Baker NL, et al. Apolipoprotein E epsilon4 prevalence in Alzheimer's disease patients varies across global populations: a systematic literature review and meta-analysis. Dement Geriatr Cogn Disord. 2011;31(1):20–30. pmid:21124030
  16. 16. Sachdev PS, Lipnicki DM, Kochan NA, Crawford JD, Rockwood K, Xiao S, et al. COSMIC (Cohort Studies of Memory in an International Consortium): an international consortium to identify risk and protective factors and biomarkers of cognitive ageing and dementia in diverse ethnic and sociocultural groups. BMC Neurol. 2013;13:165. pmid:24195705
  17. 17. Lima-Costa MF, Firmo JO, Uchoa E. Cohort profile: the Bambui (Brazil) Cohort Study of Ageing. Int J Epidemiol. 2011;40(4):862–7. pmid:20805109
  18. 18. Brayne C, McCracken C, Matthews FE. Cohort profile: the Medical Research Council Cognitive Function and Ageing Study (CFAS). Int J Epidemiol. 2006;35(5):1140–5. pmid:16980700
  19. 19. Katz MJ, Lipton RB, Hall CB, Zimmerman ME, Sanders AE, Verghese J, et al. Age-specific and sex-specific prevalence and incidence of mild cognitive impairment, dementia, and Alzheimer dementia in blacks and whites: a report from the Einstein Aging Study. Alzheimer Dis Assoc Disord. 2012;26(4):335–43. pmid:22156756
  20. 20. Ritchie K, Carriere I, Ritchie CW, Berr C, Artero S, Ancelin ML. Designing prevention programmes to reduce incidence of dementia: prospective cohort study of modifiable risk factors. BMJ. 2010;341:c3885. pmid:20688841
  21. 21. Dardiotis E, Kosmidis MH, Yannakoulia M, Hadjigeorgiou GM, Scarmeas N. The Hellenic Longitudinal Investigation of Aging and Diet (HELIAD): rationale, study design, and cohort description. Neuroepidemiology. 2014;43(1):9–14. pmid:24993387
  22. 22. Wong CH, Leung GT, Fung AW, Chan WC, Lam LC. Cognitive predictors for five-year conversion to dementia in community-dwelling Chinese older adults. Int Psychogeriatr. 2013;25(7):1125–34. pmid:23544873
  23. 23. Guaita A, Colombo M, Vaccaro R, Fossi S, Vitali SF, Forloni G, et al. Brain aging and dementia during the transition from late adulthood to old age: design and methodology of the "Invece.Ab" population-based study. BMC Geriatr. 2013;13:98. pmid:24063518
  24. 24. Kim TH, Park JH, Lee JJ, Jhoo JH, Kim BJ, Kim JL, et al. Overview of the Korean Longitudinal Study on Cognitive Aging and Dementia [abstract]. Alzheimers Dement. 2013;9(Suppl 4):626–7.
  25. 25. Anstey KJ, Christensen H, Butterworth P, Easteal S, Mackinnon A, Jacomb T, et al. Cohort profile: the PATH through life project. Int J Epidemiol. 2012;41(4):951–60. pmid:21349904
  26. 26. Scazufca M, Menezes PR, Araya R, Di Rienzo VD, Almeida OP, Gunnell D, et al. Risk factors across the life course and dementia in a Brazilian population: results from the Sao Paulo Ageing & Health Study (SPAH). Int J Epidemiol. 2008;37(4):879–90. pmid:18583392
  27. 27. Narazaki K, Nofuji Y, Honda T, Matsuo E, Yonemoto K, Kumagai S. Normative data for the montreal cognitive assessment in a Japanese community-dwelling older population. Neuroepidemiology. 2013;40(1):23–9. pmid:23075757
  28. 28. Feng L, Gwee X, Kua EH, Ng TP. Cognitive function and tea consumption in community dwelling older Chinese in Singapore. J Nutr Health Aging. 2010;14(6):433–8. pmid:20617284
  29. 29. Sachdev PS, Brodaty H, Reppermund S, Kochan NA, Trollor JN, Draper B, et al. The Sydney Memory and Ageing Study (MAS): methodology and baseline medical and neuropsychiatric characteristics of an elderly epidemiological non-demented cohort of Australians aged 70–90 years. Int Psychogeriatr. 2010;22(8):1248–64. pmid:20637138
  30. 30. Lobo A, Saz P, Marcos G, Día J, De-la-Cámara C, Ventura T, et al. The ZARADEMP Project on the incidence, prevalence and risk factors of dementia (and depression) in the elderly community: II. Methods and first results. Eur J Psychiatry. 2005;19:40–54.
  31. 31. Folstein MF, Folstein SE, McHugh PR. "Mini-mental state". A practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. J Psychiatr Res. 1975;12(3):189–98. pmid:1202204
  32. 32. Thal LJ, Grundman M, Golden R. Alzheimer's disease: a correlational analysis of the Blessed Information-Memory-Concentration Test and the Mini-Mental State Exam. Neurology. 1986;36(2):262–4. pmid:3945395
  33. 33. Strauss E, Sherman EMS, Spreen O. A Compendium of Neuropsychological Tests: Administration, Norms, and Commentary. 3rd ed. New York, NY: Oxford University Press; 2006.
  34. 34. Lezak MD, Howieson DB, Loring DW. Neuropsychological Assessment. 4 ed. New York, NY: Oxford University Press; 2004.
  35. 35. Weintraub S, Salmon D, Mercaldo N, Ferris S, Graff-Radford NR, Chui H, et al. The Alzheimer's Disease Centers' Uniform Data Set (UDS): the neuropsychologic test battery. Alzheimer Dis Assoc Disord. 2009;23(2):91–101. pmid:19474567
  36. 36. Ganguli M, Snitz BE, Lee CW, Vanderbilt J, Saxton JA, Chang CC. Age and education effects and norms on a cognitive test battery from a population-based cohort: the Monongahela-Youghiogheny Healthy Aging Team. Aging Ment Health. 2010;14(1):100–7. pmid:20155526
  37. 37. Lim J, Oh IK, Han C, Huh YJ, Jung IK, Patkar AA, et al. Sensitivity of cognitive tests in four cognitive domains in discriminating MDD patients from healthy controls: a meta-analysis. Int Psychogeriatr. 2013;25(9):1543–57. pmid:23725644
  38. 38. Higgins JPT, Green S. Cochrane Handbook for Systematic Reviews of Interventions Version 5.1.0 [updated March 2011]: The Cochrane Collaboration; 2011. Available from: www.handbook.cochrane.org.
  39. 39. Castro-Costa E, Fuzikawa C, Uchoa E, Firmo JO, Lima-Costa MF. Norms for the mini-mental state examination: adjustment of the cut-off point in population-based studies (evidences from the Bambui health aging study). Arq Neuropsiquiatr. 2008;66(3A):524–8. pmid:18813712
  40. 40. Venables WN, Ripley BD. Modern Applied Statistics with S. 4th ed. New York, NY: Springer; 2002.
  41. 41. Viechtbauer W. Conducting meta-analyses in R with the metafor package. J Stat Softw. 2010;36(3):1–48.
  42. 42. Finkel D, Reynolds CA, McArdle JJ, Pedersen NL. Age changes in processing speed as a leading indicator of cognitive aging. Psychol Aging. 2007;22(3):558–68. pmid:17874954
  43. 43. Salthouse TA. The processing-speed theory of adult age differences in cognition. Psychol Rev. 1996;103(3):403–28. pmid:8759042
  44. 44. Proust-Lima C, Amieva H, Dartigues JF, Jacqmin-Gadda H. Sensitivity of four psychometric tests to measure cognitive changes in brain aging-population-based studies. Am J Epidemiol. 2007;165(3):344–50. pmid:17105962
  45. 45. Hayat SA, Luben R, Moore S, Dalzell N, Bhaniani A, Anuj S, et al. Cognitive function in a general population of men and women: a cross sectional study in the European Investigation of Cancer-Norfolk cohort (EPIC-Norfolk). BMC Geriatr. 2014;14:142. pmid:25527303
  46. 46. Jack CR Jr., Wiste HJ, Weigand SD, Knopman DS, Vemuri P, Mielke MM, et al. Age, Sex, and APOE epsilon4 Effects on Memory, Brain Structure, and beta-Amyloid Across the Adult Life Span. JAMA Neurol. 2015;72(5):511–9. pmid:25775353
  47. 47. Lee J, Shih R, Feeney K, Langa KM. Gender disparity in late-life cognitive functioning in India: findings from the longitudinal aging study in India. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci. 2014;69(4):603–11. pmid:24622150
  48. 48. Lei X, Hu Y, McArdle JJ, Smith JP, Zhao Y. Gender Differences in Cognition among Older Adults in China. J Hum Resour. 2012;47(4):951–71. pmid:24347682
  49. 49. Ancelin ML, Ritchie K. Lifelong endocrine fluctuations and related cognitive disorders. Curr Pharm Des. 2005;11(32):4229–52. pmid:16375742
  50. 50. Sundermann EE, Maki PM, Rubin LH, Lipton RB, Landau S, Biegon A, et al. Female advantage in verbal memory: Evidence of sex-specific cognitive reserve. Neurology. 2016;87(18):1916–24. pmid:27708128
  51. 51. Proust-Lima C, Amieva H, Letenneur L, Orgogozo JM, Jacqmin-Gadda H, Dartigues JF. Gender and education impact on brain aging: a general cognitive factor approach. Psychol Aging. 2008;23(3):608–20. pmid:18808250
  52. 52. Mielke MM, Vemuri P, Rocca WA. Clinical epidemiology of Alzheimer's disease: assessing sex and gender differences. Clin Epidemiol. 2014;6:37–48. pmid:24470773
  53. 53. Cagney KA, Lauderdale DS. Education, wealth, and cognitive function in later life. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci. 2002;57(2):P163–72. pmid:11867664
  54. 54. Schneider AL, Sharrett AR, Patel MD, Alonso A, Coresh J, Mosley T, et al. Education and cognitive change over 15 years: the atherosclerosis risk in communities study. J Am Geriatr Soc. 2012;60(10):1847–53. pmid:23013064
  55. 55. Wisdom NM, Callahan JL, Hawkins KA. The effects of apolipoprotein E on non-impaired cognitive functioning: a meta-analysis. Neurobiol Aging. 2011;32(1):63–74. pmid:19285755
  56. 56. Corbo RM, Scacchi R. Apolipoprotein E (APOE) allele distribution in the world. Is APOE*4 a 'thrifty' allele? Ann Hum Genet. 1999;63(Pt 4):301–10. pmid:10738542
  57. 57. Spencer RJ, Wendell CR, Giggey PP, Katzel LI, Lefkowitz DM, Siegel EL, et al. Psychometric limitations of the mini-mental state examination among nondemented older adults: an evaluation of neurocognitive and magnetic resonance imaging correlates. Exp Aging Res. 2013;39(4):382–97. pmid:23875837
  58. 58. Stein J, Luppa M, Maier W, Wagner M, Wolfsgruber S, Scherer M, et al. Assessing cognitive changes in the elderly: reliable change indices for the Mini-Mental State Examination. Acta Psychiatr Scand. 2012;126(3):208–18. pmid:22375927
  59. 59. Lamar M, Resnick SM, Zonderman AB. Longitudinal changes in verbal memory in older adults: distinguishing the effects of age from repeat testing. Neurology. 2003;60(1):82–6. pmid:12525723