Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

Levers for Language Growth: Characteristics and Predictors of Language Trajectories between 4 and 7 Years

  • Cristina McKean ,

    cristina.mckean@ncl.ac.uk

    Affiliations Newcastle University, Newcastle upon Tyne, United Kingdom, Murdoch Children’s Research Institute, Melbourne, Australia

  • Fiona K. Mensah ,

    Contributed equally to this work with: Fiona K. Mensah, Sheena Reilly

    Affiliations Murdoch Children’s Research Institute, Melbourne, Australia, University of Melbourne, Melbourne, Australia, The Royal Children’s Hospital, Melbourne, Australia

  • Patricia Eadie ,

    ‡ These authors also contributed equally to the work.

    Affiliation University of Melbourne, Melbourne, Australia

  • Edith L. Bavin ,

    ‡ These authors also contributed equally to the work.

    Affiliation La Trobe University, Melbourne, Australia

  • Lesley Bretherton ,

    ‡ These authors also contributed equally to the work.

    Affiliations The Royal Children’s Hospital, Melbourne, Australia, Murdoch Children’s Research Institute, Melbourne, Australia, University of Melbourne, Melbourne, Australia

  • Eileen Cini ,

    ‡ These authors also contributed equally to the work.

    Affiliation Murdoch Children’s Research Institute, Melbourne, Australia

  • Sheena Reilly

    Contributed equally to this work with: Fiona K. Mensah, Sheena Reilly

    Affiliations Menzies Health Institute Queensland, Queensland, Australia, Murdoch Children’s Research Institute, Melbourne, Australia

Abstract

Background

Evidence is required as to when and where to focus resources to achieve the greatest gains for children’s language development. Key to these decisions is the understanding of individual differences in children’s language trajectories and the predictors of those differences. To determine optimal timing we must understand if and when children’s relative language abilities become fixed. To determine where to focus effort we must identify mutable factors, that is those with the potential to be changed through interventions, which are associated with significant differences in children’s language scores and rate of progress.

Methods

Uniquely this study examined individual differences in language growth trajectories in a population sample of children between 4 and 7 years using the multilevel model for change. The influence of predictors, grouped with respect to their mutability and their proximity to the child (least-mutable, mutable-distal, mutable-proximal), were estimated.

Results

A significant degree of variability in rate of progress between 4 and 7 years was evident, much of which was systematically associated with mutable-proximal factors, that is, those factors with evidence that they are modifiable through interventions with the child or family, such as shared book reading, TV viewing and number of books in the home. Mutable-distal factors, such as family income, family literacy and neighbourhood disadvantage, hypothesised to be modifiable through social policy, were important predictors of language abilities at 4 years.

Conclusions

Potential levers for language interventions lie in the child’s home learning environment from birth to age 4. However, the role of a family’s material and cultural capital must not be ignored, nor should the potential for growth into the school years. Early Years services should acknowledge the effects of multiple, cascading and cumulative risks and seek to promote child language development through the aggregation of marginal gains in the pre-school years and beyond.

Introduction

Well-developed language abilities enable children to negotiate the social-relational demands of school life and provide the foundational knowledge upon which literacy and other academic skills are built [1]. Without such skills children are at increased risk of literacy difficulties [2] academic failure [3,4], social and emotional difficulties [5] and, as adults, of unemployment [6] and poor mental health [7]

Between 7–16% of 5 year olds have poor language development, that is falling 1–1.25 SD below the mean on norm-referenced language tests, with higher rates reported in more socially disadvantaged groups [8,9]. In recent years, the importance of promoting oral language development has been recognised in policy for Early Years provision internationally [10,11]. However as children transition into more formal schooling, this focus recedes. If potential remains to narrow the gap for children with poor language as they transition into school this may be an opportunity lost.

Language Trajectories

Longitudinal studies suggest that in the pre-school years language development is highly variable, with ‘natural resolution’ of poor language occurring spontaneously for many children [1214] but those children who reach 5 years with poor language are unlikely to ever catch up [3,1517]. However, recent longitudinal studies of population cohorts suggest that the degree of fluidity and heterogeneity that exists in child language pathways has been underestimated [1214,18].

The nature of language trajectories is poorly understood. Only five population studies of child language have considered the nature of language growth [1822]. Two of these focussed on the crucial period of transition into formal schooling between 4 and 7 years but based their investigation on receptive vocabulary (i.e. knowledge of the from and meaning of individual words) [18,22]. Taylor, Christenson (18) explored the effect of child, maternal and family risk factors on vocabulary development from 4 to 8 years. This breadth of measurement aligns with current models of language development that acknowledge the influence of multiple and interacting influences on the language acquisition process. Similar to many other epidemiological samples, Taylor et al found that a range of child, maternal and family risks, predicted receptive vocabulary development at age 4. Vocabulary growth between 4 and 8 however was predicted by a much smaller number of factors. Speaking English as a second language (ESL), low school readiness and maternal mental health distress were all associated with lower scores at age 4 but also with ‘catch-up’ growth, although this was not sufficient to close the gap by 8 years. Furthermore children living in areas of socio-economic disadvantage did not have significantly poorer vocabulary scores at age 4 but did have lower rates of vocabulary growth between 4 and 8 years than their more advantaged peers.

‘Language’ is a complex developmental skill comprised not only of vocabulary knowledge but also grammatical morphology (i.e. markers which signal grammatical properties such as plurality or tense) and syntax (i.e. word order in sentences which, in conjunction with grammatical morphology, signals the nature of the relationships between components within sentences). Although receptive vocabulary has been highly correlated with broader language abilities cross-sectionally [23] the shape and rate of its developmental trajectory may not be representative of growth in syntax and morphology [24] since sensitive or optimal periods for development differ between language components [25]. Thus there may be greater variability in children’s growth rates during the optimal period for one component, and, conversely, variance in growth will decrease when a child’s relative ability in that component reaches maturation. Further research is therefore required to uncover the nature of individual differences in the broader language trajectory.

Levers of language growth

Language development is the result of complex interactions between the child’s biological make-up, their family, school and community and the social and cultural context [11]. These interactions are hypothesised to involve cascading effects wherein distal factors, (those temporally or spatially distant from the child) indirectly affect child outcomes through their influence on more proximal factors which directly affect the child [26]. In public health, preventative interventions involve the removal or reduction of risk factors and/or the promotion of protective factors [27]. Preventing or ameliorating poor language development could, therefore, involve targeting any or all of these factors. Evidence is required as to where to focus effort, and hence resources, in order to gain maximal effect. Key to determining whether a factor should be targeted would be understanding both the scale of effect which it might yield, and also the degree to which it has the capacity to be changed; that is, its ‘mutability’. Factors vary in their mutability and hence their suitability as targets for preventative interventions, from least mutable, such as gender, which may confer risk but would not be a candidate for intervention, to most mutable, such as a family’s access to developmentally appropriate toys, which could readily be addressed through intervention. This study has two main aims. The first is to identify factors that are potentially modifiable by interventions that could reduce children’s risk of poor language development up to the age of 4 years and of poor language growth between 4 and 7 years. The second is to quantify the amount of variance these factors explain in order to estimate the potential gains that might be achieved.

Clearly factors fall on a continuum of how amenable they may be to change from most to least mutable and characterisation of risks as mutable or otherwise is not always simple. For the purposes of this study we developed a conceptual framework in which we grouped factors into three groups. ‘Least-mutable’ being those risks which are resistant to modification or inappropriate choices for intervention, ‘mutable-proximal’ as those which may be modified by direct interventions with the child or family, and ‘mutable-distal’, as those with indirect effects, which would require interventions delivered at a population level, through social policy rather than with individual families or schools. This framework allows us to build a series of models in a systematic and theoretically motivated sequence [28], and allows us to estimate the variability explained by each category of predictors in addition to the effects of the other categories.

This study is unique in exploring individual differences in children’s language abilities at 4 years and the rate at which their language grows between 4 and 7 years. By examining individual differences in growth after the age of 4 years and identifying mutable predictors of both language ability at 4 years and rate of progress, this paper seeks to inform decisions regarding where and when to focus resources to achieve the greatest gains for children’s language outcomes. To that end we ask the following research questions:

To what extent are children’s language trajectories fixed by age 4 and is there heterogeneity in children’s rate of progress between 4 and 7 years?

  1. Which factors predict a child’s language at age 4 and which predict their rate of progress between 4 and 7?
  2. How much of the variability in language abilities at age 4 and children’s rate of progress between 4 and 7 years is explained by mutable factors, either proximal or distal

Materials and Method

Participants and sampling

The participants were recruited into the Early Language in Victoria Study (ELVS), a community ascertained, prospective observational longitudinal study. Infants (mean age 8 months), were recruited in 2003–4 in metropolitan Melbourne, Victoria, Australia using a sampling frame stratified by the Australian Index of Social Disadvantage (Socio-economic Indexes for Areas—SEIFA) [29]. Data included in this paper were collected in 8 waves, from 8 months to 7 years. At baseline, 1910 children were recruited (50.5% boys), excluding children with serious disabilities or developmental delays (e.g. Down syndrome) and parents who did not speak and/or understand English. Further details are reported in Reilly et al. [30].

Ethics statement.

Ethical approval was obtained from the Royal Childrens Hospital (#23018 and #27078) and La Trobe University, Human Ethics Committee (#03–32). All parents provided written, informed consent.

Attrition.

The analyses were conducted on the subset of children who had completed language assessments at 4, 5 and 7 years. Children with complete (n = 883) and incomplete (n = 1027) outcome data, were compared. The groups were not significantly different with respect to gender (p = .1) but those with incomplete data had higher levels of social disadvantage (p < .001), lower language (4 years: p < .001, 5 years: p < .01, and 7 years: p < .001) and lower IQ (4 years: p < .001). This sample is more advantaged than the Australian population and so provides a conservative estimate of the effects of social disadvantage and associated factors in the wider population.

Language Measures

Language was measured using direct assessment by trained research assistants, using the Australian adaptation of the Clinical Evaluation of Language Fundamentals-Preschool, Second Edition (CELF-P2) [31] at age 4 and the CELF-4 Australian Standardisation at 5 and 7 years [32]. To ensure that differences between the tests did not affect estimates of change over time, the CELF ‘core language’ raw scores were scaled to a mean 0 and SD of 1 (z scores) at each time point from for the sample of 883 children. The CELF is an omnibus language measure and tests children’s abilities to use and understand syntax (word order rules), semantics (word and sentence meanings) and grammatical morphology (markers of grammatical relationships such as plural—s and past tense—ed).

Predictor Measures

Predictors were derived from direct assessment of non-verbal ability at 4 and parent report at 8 months, 1, 2, 3, and 4 years. Previous population studies were reviewed and potential risk and protective factors for language or vocabulary outcomes were identified in children between 4 and 7 years [13,15,19,3337]. The final set of 22 predictors were chosen after preliminary analyses (see below), and were categorised as either ‘least mutable’, ‘mutable-distal’ or ‘mutable-proximal’.

Least-mutable factors.

For the purposes of this study we considered factors to fall into the ‘least mutable’ category where the risks cannot be removed through the action of an intervention, either at the level of social policy or through an intervention delivered directly with the child or family. Factors were categorized as least- mutable if they fulfilled one or more of the following criteria: 1) cannot be modified through intervention as the factor is biologically driven (e.g. family history, gender); 2) comorbid diagnoses such as ADHD, ASD or Learning Disability whose symptoms can be ameliorated to a degree but which cannot be removed entirely 3) evidence for high levels of stability over development (e.g. Temperament); and 4) it would be unethical or impracticable to target in an intervention (e.g. exposure to non-dominant language in the home (see Table 1)).

thumbnail
Table 1. Least-mutable predictors: measures, age of measurement, derivation and criteria and evidence for categorisation.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0134251.t001

Mutable-distal factors.

For the purposes of this study we defined mutable-distal factors as potentially modifiable factors that exert indirect effects on child language and which, in order to be modified, would require interventions delivered at a population level through social policy rather than through individual families or schools (see Table 2).

thumbnail
Table 2. Mutable-distal predictors: measures, age of measurement, and derivation.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0134251.t002

Mutable-proximal factors.

Mutable-proximal factors were defined as those that have the potential to be modified by direct interventions with the child or family and which have robust supporting evidence that they can be substantially ameliorated through intervention (see Table 3).

thumbnail
Table 3. Mutable-proximal predictors: measures, age of measurement, derivation and evidence for potential to be modified through intervention.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0134251.t003

Statistical analysis

We used individual growth trajectory analysis to investigate change in language ability over time using the multilevel model for change [28], which is comprised of two levels.

Level 1 represents the individual growth model, which specifies the intercept, slope and shape of trajectory for each individual and captures the residual variance, or scatter, of the observed data around the individual’s hypothesised trajectory. In this case the intercept was the child’s scaled language score at age 4 years, the slope was their rate of change in scaled language score between 4 and 7 years, and the shape was hypothesised to be linear, as the three data points available in this data did not allow for specification of curvilinear changes [28].

Level 2 represents the nature of the associations between predictors and inter-individual differences in the change trajectories. This is comprised of fixed effects and variance components. The fixed effects capture systematic inter-individual differences in intercept and slope according to different values of the predictors. The variance components capture the residual variance in intercept and slope and the covariance between the intercept and slope after controlling for the effect of the predictors. Consideration of fixed effects allow us to uncover the effect of individual predictors on a child’s starting point at 4 years and their rate of progress between 4 and 7 years. Comparison of variance components across models with differing predictors allow us to estimate the degree of variance in intercept and slope explained by those predictors [28]. Models were fitted using maximum likelihood estimation in Stata [38]. The CELF scaled score was the response variable and age in years, centred on 4 years, and corrected for prematurity if necessary, was the unit of time. These models allowed the exploration of inter-individual differences in children’s language scores at 4 years (the intercept) and their rate of change in relative position between 4 and 7 (the slope).

Correlation between candidate predictor variables was examined and preliminary analyses of candidate predictors were conducted and those included in the final multivariate analyses were chosen if preliminary bivariate growth trajectory modelling demonstrated a significant association with language intercept and/or slope at the p< .1 level; and no difficulties of collinearity between predictors arose.

We then fitted 4 models: an unconditional model containing only level-one predictors (the child’s age as a measure of time) and three conditional models that introduced level-two predictors (the ‘risk’ factors). These models were comprised of 1) least mutable factors 2) least mutable and mutable-distal factors and 3) least mutable, mutable-distal and mutable-proximal factors. Differences between these models in terms of the amount of variance in intercept and slope explained, were explored through consideration of the model variance components and also through calculation of pseudo R2 measure of model fit (Table 1) [28]. This allowed the estimation of the amount of additional variance in intercept and slope explained by the addition of each category of predictors. Consideration of the fixed effects in each model allowed for the estimation of the association between the individual predictors and the children’s language at 4 (the intercept) and the rate of change in relative position between 4 and 7 (slopes), whilst controlling for the effects of the other predictors in the model.

Results

Table 4 presents comparisons between the three conditional models with respect to their variance components and pseudo R2 measures of model fit, and between each conditional model and the relevant unconditional model. This allows for the estimation of how much variance in intercept and slope was explained with the addition of each category of predictors: least-mutable, mutable-proximal and mutable-distal.

thumbnail
Table 4. Multivariate growth trajectory models 1, 2 and 3: variance components for Mean CELF scaled standard score at 4 years (intercept) Growth rate per year 4–7 years (slope) and Pseudo R2.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0134251.t004

Table 5 summarises the fixed effects in the models and presents the coefficients, 95% CI and p values for each of the predictors in the three conditional models (1) least-mutable predictors 2) least-mutable and mutable-distal and 3) least-mutable, mutable-distal and mutable-proximal). The proportion of the sample falling into each level of the predictors is also shown. The response variable, CELF scaled scores, had a mean of 0 and SD of 1. The unit of measurement for the intercept was therefore 1 SD and, for the slope SD change per year over the 3-year period studied. We can therefore interpret the coefficients as effect sizes, that is, reflecting differences between risks and reference categories in terms of number of SDs and for the slope, the numbers of SDs lost or gained per year if a child presents with a specific risk.

thumbnail
Table 5. Least-mutable and Mutable (proximal and distal) predictors of language growth: multivariate growth trajectory model coefficients Mean CELF scaled standard score at 4 years (intercept) Growth rate per year 4–7 years (slope).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0134251.t005

To what extent are children’s language trajectories fixed by age 4 and is there heterogeneity in children’s rate of progress between 4 and 7 years?

The average intercept and slope in scaled CELF score were close to zero in the unconditional model (intercept M = .02; slope M = .00) reflecting the fact that the language score had been scaled and converted to Z scores. The between person variance around the intercept was .87 and around the slope was .11 (Table 4). This variance in slope indicates a SD of .33. This means that over the 3 years studied, 68% of the sample have rates of progress which vary between approximately +1 and -1 SD per 3 years in language scores and 95% have changes between + 2 and– 2 SD per 3 years. A substantial degree of variability in slope therefore continues to be present in this age range (see Fig 1).

thumbnail
Fig 1. Scatterplot of the relationship between Language Z score at 4 years (intercept) and rate of change in Language Z scores per year from 4–7 years (slope).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0134251.g001

The covariance between intercept and slope was negative suggesting there was a general trend for children with lower scores at intercept to ‘catch up’ and those with higher scores to drop in relative position over time (Table 4 and Fig 1). Figs 2 and 3 depict subsamples of the empirical growth plots (Fig 1 representing 1 child in 10 and Fig 2 representing a sample of 20 children ranked from low to high according to their intercept). These Figures demonstrate the substantial individual differences in intercept and the smaller but significant individual differences in slope that existed in the sample. Furthermore visual inspection of the empirical plots (Fig 3) appears to support the assumption of linearity of our statistical models.

thumbnail
Fig 2. Random sample of ‘empirical growth’ plots—Individual OLS trajectories for 1 child in 10 (N = 83).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0134251.g002

thumbnail
Fig 3. Sample of ‘empirical growth’ plots—sample of 20 individual OLS trajectories ordered from low to high score at 4 years (intercept).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0134251.g003

Which factors predict a child’s language at age 4?

Of the 22 predictors in the final model, 13 made a statistically significant contribution to the variability in the children’s scores at 4 years whilst controlling for the other predictors in the model (Table 5). We note that there was a significant level of correlation between many of the predictors considered (Table 6) which offers explanation for the attenuation of effects seen between Model 1 and Model 3. Significant ‘least-mutable’ predictors were: ESL, non-verbal IQ, diagnosis of a developmental disorder, shy/high approach score, and family history of speech and language difficulties. Significant mutable-distal factors were family literacy, high birth position, social disadvantage and family income. Significant mutable-proximal factors were number of children’s books in the home, and frequency of reading to the child. Having a low prosocial score and a Speech Sound Disorder (SSD) were also significant but only at the p < .1 level.

thumbnail
Table 6. Correlation between measures examined as predictors of language growth.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0134251.t006

Which factors predict a child’s rate of progress between 4 and 7 years?

Six predictors made a statistically significant, unique contribution to the variability in children’s slopes (rate of change between 4 and 7 years) in the final model. Significant least-mutable predictors were: ESL, with rapid ‘catch up’ growth evident that was sufficient to make up the disadvantage found at 4 years by 7 years of age; and children with a low birth weight, evincing a small but significant rate of ‘falling behind’ despite having average levels of language at 4 years. The remaining four were mutable-proximal factors. The language abilities of the children with low pro-social scores (outside of the typical range for this domain) appeared to ‘catch up’ with their peers over the course of the study. Children with fewer than 10 children’s books in the home, were also catching up. However, at the observed rate of progress, it would be 5 years before they made up their disadvantage in language at age 4. This result was significant at the p< .1 level. Those with 20–30 or 10–20 books did not exhibit catch up growth. Children with the lowest frequency of shared book reading (those whose parents consistently report reading only ‘sometimes’ from 8 months to 4 years) also did not exhibit catch up growth, so their disadvantage at 4 would appear to persist until at least 7 years. Those in the second and third quartile caught up over time, making up their disadvantage over approximately 3 years. Children who watched an average of more than 3 hours TV per day fell behind their peers .07 of a SD per year despite the fact that they did not begin with poorer language at age 4.

How much of the variability in children’s language abilities at 4 and their rate of progress between 4 and 7 years is explained by mutable factors?

Pseudo R2 values suggest the final model, with all 22 predictors, explains 48% of the variability in intercept and 67% of the variability in slope (rate of progress between 4 and 7 years). The least-mutable factors alone explain 34% of the variability in children’s language abilities at 4 years, mutable distal factors an additional 9% and mutable proximal an additional 5% (Table 4). For the slopes, ‘least mutable’ factors explain 40% of the variability, mutable distal an additional 4% and mutable proximal an additional 23%. Factors which we hypothesised could be changed through interventions therefore explain a large proportion of the variance in slope.

Multiple imputation

Analyses were completed on participants with complete data for the predictor variables. To test whether the reduced sample size due to missing values of the predictors affected the results, we used multiple imputation to restore the sample to the original 883 children with complete outcome data. There were no substantive differences between growth trajectory analysis results on the imputed and non-imputed data. We concluded that our results could be considered robust.

Discussion

This paper sought to inform decisions regarding where and when to best focus resources to achieve the greatest gains for children’s language outcomes through an examination of individual differences in language trajectory between 4 and 7 years and of the predictors of those trajectories. Up to the age of 4 individual differences in language abilities were explained, in the most part, by factors that are not suitable targets for interventions such as a child’s gender or a positive family history of speech and language difficulties. However, factors which we hypothesised could be changed through interventions explained a small but significant additional proportion of the variability in language outcomes at 4. Of particular importance up to age 4 years were mutable-distal factors relating to a family’s material and cultural ‘capital’ such as family literacy, high birth position, social disadvantage index and family income [26]. Between the ages of 4 and 7 years individual differences in the rate of language growth were also explained in the most part by the least-mutable group of predictors; in this case ESL and low birth weight. However, a large proportion of variability in growth was also explained by mutable-proximal factors: pro-social scores at 4 years; number of children’s books in the home at 2 years, frequency of shared book reading from 8 months to 4 years and TV viewing at 4 years. There is robust evidence that each of these factors can be modified through interventions with the child and family [3943] therefore opening up the possibility of early preventative interventions targeting these risks.

This study also demonstrates that children’s relative position in language abilities is not absolutely fixed by age 4 and some movement does still occur between 4 and 7 years, much of which is associated with specific predictors. Our results suggest that the home learning environment up to the age 4 years continues to influence language growth between 4 and 7 years, hence prioritising interventions during the early years of development appear to be supported. These findings support Shonkoff and colleagues’ assertion that “what happens during the first months and years of life matters a lot, not because this period of development provides an indelible blueprint for adult well-being, but because it sets either a sturdy or fragile stage for what follows” [11] p. 5. However, our findings also suggest that substantial changes in a child’s relative position in language ability can still occur after 4 years and that much of this variability remains unexplained, suggesting that scope may still exist to modify children’s language trajectories after the early years. This would suggest that early years interventions should not therefore be prioritised to the exclusion of later interventions.

The effect of the least-mutable factors, in the most part, reflected previous research, with children with low non-verbal IQ, diagnosis of a developmental disorder, shy temperament and positive family history all being associated with significantly lower scores at 4 years. Uniquely this study was able to further demonstrate that these risks are not associated with rate of growth between 4 and 7 years.

Although being from a ESL background exerted the highest level of disadvantage on English language scores at age 4, it was also associated with the fastest rate of progress; by the age of 7 this group had caught up with their English-speaking peers. This trajectory was adjusted for other disadvantages, such as low income or high levels of neighbourhood disadvantage. This result would suggest that where poor progress is found for this group of children, the source is unlikely to be their exposure to a minority language at home but rather the broader social risks often experienced by these communities [44].

A further important finding was that, while children with low birth weight did not have lower language scores at 4 years, they experienced a small but significant decline between 4 and 7; a finding which aligns with theories of cumulative vulnerability over time in this group [45]. This result must be interpreted cautiously due to the small numbers with this risk in the cohort however it indicates the importance of the consideration of growth in studies of language development as a cross-sectional methodology would not have revealed any differences between this group and their peers.

Factors that could be addressed through social policy such as neighbourhood disadvantage, low income, high birth position and family literacy affected children’s 4-year language scores but were not associated with growth. These factors, which can be broadly conceptualised as a family’s resource, are hypothesised to affect the child indirectly through their effects on proximal processes such as parent-child interaction and the home learning environment [26]. Their importance in the final model, where proximal processes were also included, together with the fact that children do not ‘catch up’ from these disadvantages in the early school years, underline the importance of addressing these more structural inequalities during the very early years.

Factors closest to the child explained a large proportion of the variability in children’s language growth. Counter-intuitively, children with low pro-social scores at 4 years demonstrated catch-up growth in their language abilities between 4 and 7 years. Given that access to good quality early years provision has been shown to have long lasting effects on children’s pro-social scores [39] perhaps access to kindergarten and school addresses these issues for many children and thence their language abilities. More importantly, the child’s home learning environment in the first 4 years of life was associated with children’s language at 4 and had lasting influences into the early school years. The negative effects associated with having fewer children’s books in the home and of low levels of shared book reading in the early years persisted until the age of 7 or, if catch up occurred, would take between 3 and 5 years to be compensated; years during which crucial academic skills are learned. High levels of TV viewing were associated with a negative slope such that children who watched an average of more than 3 hours of TV per day fell behind their peers, perhaps through the displacement of other activities that would benefit language development [46].

When considering the importance of the identified risk factors it is important to remember that many children living with disadvantage are likely to be exposed to multiple risks, thus experiencing cumulative effects [26] which can readily reach clinically significant levels. For example, 5% of this sample was children who were in homes with fewer than 10 children’s books (Coefficient = -.58) and had the lowest levels of shared book reading (Coefficient = -.38). Taking account of both of these risk factors this group would, on average, start kindergarten .98 of a SD below the mean in language scores, with likely negative consequences extending into adulthood [3,6,7]. Indeed, even children with less severe levels of risk, if aggregated across a number of factors, readily reach similarly poor language levels.

Limitations and next steps

Due to higher levels of attrition and greater difficulties in recruitment in socially disadvantaged groups this sample does not reflect the more socially disadvantaged groups in the Australian population and so is likely to underestimate the proportion of children at risk. However, as one of the few community samples which include language measures repeated at multiple waves, the cohort provides a rare opportunity to explore language growth. The contrast between those factors found in the current study to be associated with language growth and those found by Taylor et al. [18] with respect to vocabulary growth (maternal mental health, school readiness, and socio-economic area disadvantage), would suggest that measures of language development and vocabulary growth tap distinct abilities with distinct risk and protective factors. We therefore urge future cohort studies to include measures of language at multiple data waves into adulthood in order to identify more clearly if gaps between children at high and low risk widen, narrow or persist over childhood and to identify risk and protective factors for those trajectories.

The model estimates reported in this study were limited by the measures available in ELVS and the timing of their measurement. For example measures of the quality of childcare/kindergarten provision accessed by the children were not available, nor was detail regarding the school attended by each child. Given the proportion of a child’s day spent in educational provision between 4 and 7 years, we recommend that future work be carried out to determine whether differences exist between schools with respect to children’s language growth. Also, although measures of children’s pragmatic skills were available, they were measured after the age of 4 years and so could not be included. The final model, with 22 predictors, is the product of an initial analysis of 36 potential predictors and includes only those factors found to significantly predict language outcomes in a bivariate analyses and which did not present issues with respect to collinearity. The model cannot be entirely comprehensive but we are confident that it captures the majority of relevant factors that the current “state of the science” allows us to measure within a longitudinal cohort design.

It is, of course, essential to note that the direction of causality in observational studies such as this is, inevitably, moot. Hence low language abilities may be the cause of, for example, poor peer relationships rather than the result. Indeed reciprocal developmental relationships between language and a number of the risks identified here are likely (e.g. TV viewing, shared book reading). This study does not prove causality however, given that effective interventions already exist to modify each of the mutable-proximal factors identified as significant in this study, it does clearly signpost potential areas for intervention research. That is to determine whether child language outcomes can be improved through the modification of the associated risks of shared book reading, number of books in the home and TV viewing. It is essential to acknowledge that these risks are likely the most readily measured features of a broader set of responsive and stimulating parenting behaviours. Intervention studies should therefore test the promotion of a wider set of parenting behaviours in the early home environment that may promote children’s language and consider the effect of distal factors, such as family resources, on a family’s ability to provide these experiences.

Conclusion

Language development is the result of complex interactions between the child’s biological make-up, and their environment [11].Whilst a substantial proportion of the variance in both language at 4 and language growth between 4 and 7 is explained by the factors that are least amenable to change, scope remains to make clinically meaningful differences to children’s language progress through the manipulation of mutable factors. Both proximal factors affecting the home learning environment and those relating to wider social risks were important in creating optimal conditions for the child. The significant and cumulative effects of, shared book reading, books in the home and TV viewing, point to the promotion of a set of parenting behaviours which could bolster language and literacy development, all of which have proven to be modifiable through interventions [3942]. Preventing or ameliorating literacy difficulties in parents would also appear to be an important goal, with the potential perhaps, to address the intergenerational transmission of language and literacy difficulties [43]. A family’s resource, in terms of its material and cultural capital, which has such a large influence on language development up to the age of 4 years, must not be ignored in the design of interventions [26]. However, the effect of parenting behaviours found over and above other risks, suggest that what parents do with their child also matters.

Preventative interventions in the early years should therefore continue to be a priority and should acknowledge both the quality of the home learning environment and wider social risks. However it is also clear that substantial change can still occur in children’s language growth from 4 to 7 and that much of this growth is associated with mutable factors. Combining interventions for these environmental factors with more traditional, language-focussed approaches could increase the effectiveness of services for children with poor language development. The findings suggest that child language intervention programs should not look for a ‘silver bullet’, rather we should acknowledge the effects of multiple, cascading and cumulative risks and seek to promote child language development through the aggregation of marginal gains in the pre-school years and beyond.

Acknowledgments

The authors acknowledge the support of the wider team in the Centre of Research Excellence in Child Language and especially thank the Early Language Victoria Study research team and all the participating families.

Author Contributions

Conceived and designed the experiments: CM SR PE ELB LB. Analyzed the data: CM FKM EC. Wrote the paper: CM FKM PE ELB LB EC.

References

  1. 1. Duncan G, Dowsett C, Claessens A, Magnusson K, Huston A, Klebanov P, et al. (2007) School readiness and later achievement. Developmental Psychology 43: 1428–1446. pmid:18020822
  2. 2. Catts HW, Fey ME, Tomblin JB, Zhang XY (2002) A longitudinal investigation of reading outcomes in children with language impairments. Journal of Speech Language and Hearing Research 45: 1142–1157.
  3. 3. Johnson CJ, Beitchman JH, Brownlie EB (2010) Twent-year follow-up of children with and without speech-language impairments: Family, educational, occupational, and quality of life outcomes. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology 9: 51–65.
  4. 4. Tomblin B (2008) Validating diagnostic standards for specific language impairment using adolescent outcomes. In: Norbury CF, Tomblin B, Bishop DVM, editors. Understanding developmental language disorders: from theory to practice. Hove: Psychology Press.
  5. 5. Durkin K, Conti-Ramsden G (2010) Young people with specific language impairment: A review of social and emotional functioning in adolescence. Child Language Teaching and Therapy 26: 105–121.
  6. 6. Law J, Rush R, Schoon I, Parsons S (2009) Modeling developmental language difficulties from school entry into adulthood: Literacy, mental health, and employment outcomes. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research 52: 1401–1416. pmid:19951922
  7. 7. Schoon I, Parsons S, Rush R, Law J (2010) Children's language ability and psychosocial development: A 29-year follow-up study. Pediatrics 126: e73–e80. pmid:20587683
  8. 8. Law J, Todd L, Clark J, Mroz M, Carr J (2013) Early language delays in the UK. London: Save the Children.
  9. 9. Tomblin JB, Records NL, Buckwalter P, Zhang X, Smith E, O'Brien M. (1997) Prevalence of specific language impairment in kindergarten children. Journal of Speech & Hearing Research 40: 1245–1260.
  10. 10. Field F (2010) The Foundation Years: preventing poor children becoming poor adults. In: Cabinet Office, editor. London: HM Government.
  11. 11. National Research Council and Institute of Medicine (2000) From Neurons to Neighborhoods: The Science of Early Childhood Development. In: Shonkoff JP, Phillips DA, editors. Washington, D. C.: The National Academies Press.
  12. 12. Reilly S, Wake M, Ukoumunne OC, Bavin E, Prior M, Cini E, et al. (2010) Predicting language outcomes at 4 years of age: Findings from the Early Language in Victoria Study. Pediatrics 126: e1530–e1537. pmid:21059719
  13. 13. Rice ML, Taylor CL, Zubrick SR (2008) Language outcomes of 7-year-old children with or without a history of late language emergence at 24 months. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research 51: 394–407. pmid:18367685
  14. 14. Ukoumunne OC, Wake M, Carlin J, Bavin EL, Lum J, Skeat J, et al. (2012) Profiles of language development in pre-school children: a longitudinal latent class analysis of data from the Early Language in Victoria Study. Child: care, health and development 38: 341–349.
  15. 15. Beitchman JH, Jiang H, Koyama E, Johnson CJ, Escobar M, Atkinson L, et al. (2008) Models and determinants of vocabulary growth from kindergarten to adulthood. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines 49: 626–634.
  16. 16. Law J, Tomblin JB, Zhang X (2008) Characterizing the growth trajectories of language-impaired children between 7 and 11 years of age. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research 51: 739–749. pmid:18506047
  17. 17. Tomblin JB, Zhang X, Buckwalter P, O'Brien M (2003) The stability of primary language disorder: Four years after kindergarten diagnosis. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research 46: 1283–1296. pmid:14700355
  18. 18. Taylor CL, Christensen D, Lawrence D, Mitrou F, Zubrick SR (2013) Risk Factors for Children's Receptive Vocabulary Development from Four to Eight Years in the Longitudinal Study of Australian Children. PLOS one 8: e73046. pmid:24039856
  19. 19. Luyten H, ten Bruggencate G (2011) The presence of Matthew effects in Dutch primary education, development of language skills over a six year period. Journal of Learning Disabilities 44: 444–458. pmid:21772060
  20. 20. Fergusson DM, Horwood LJ (1993) The effect of lead levels on the growth of word recognition in middle childhood International Journal of Epidemiology 22: 891–897.
  21. 21. Farkas G, Beron K (2004) The detailed age trajectory of oral vocabulary knowledge: Differences by class and race. Social Science Research 33: 464–497.
  22. 22. Peisner-Feinberg ES, Burchinal MR, Clifford RM, Culkin ML, Howes C, Kagan SL, et al. (2001) The relation of preschool child-care quality to children's cognitive and social developmental trajectories through second grade. Child Development 72: 1534–1553. pmid:11699686
  23. 23. Dionne G, Dale PS, Boivin M, Plomin R (2003) Genetic Evidence for Bidirectional Effects of Early Lexical and Grammatical Development. Child Development 74: 394–412. pmid:12705562
  24. 24. Rice ML (2013) Language growth and genetics of specific language impairment. International Journal of Speech-Language Pathology 15: 223–233. pmid:23614332
  25. 25. Werker JF, Hensch TK (in press) Critical periods in Speech Perception: New directions. Annual Review of Psychology.
  26. 26. Braveman P, Egerter S, Williams DR (2011) The social determinants of health: coming of age. The Annual Review of Public Health 32: 381–398. pmid:21091195
  27. 27. Mrazek PJ, Heggarty RJ (1994) Reducing risks of mental disorder: frontiers for preventative intervention research. Washington, DC: National Academy Press.
  28. 28. Singer JD, Willett JB (2003) Applied longitudinal data analysis. New York: Oxford University Press.
  29. 29. Australian Bureau of Statistics (2001) Socio-Economic Indexes for Areas. Canberra: Australian Bureau of Statistics.
  30. 30. Reilly S, Eadie P, Bavin EL, Wake M, Prior M, Williams J, et al. (2006) Growth of infant communication between 8 and 12 months: A population study. Journal of Paediatrics and Child Health 42: 764–770. pmid:17096710
  31. 31. Wiig EH, Secord WA, Semel E (2006) Clinical Evaluation of Language Fundamentals—Preschool: Australian Standardised Edition. Sydney, Australia: Harcourt Assessment.
  32. 32. Semel E, Wiig EH, Secord WA (2006) Clinical Evaluation of Language Fundamentals—Fourth Edition, Australian Standardised Edition Sydney, NSW: Psychological Corporation.
  33. 33. Beitchman JH, Brownlie EB, Wilson B (1996) Linguistic impairment and psychiatric disorder: pathways to outcome. In: Beitchman JH, Cohen NJ, Konstantareas MM, Tannock R, editors. Language, learning and behaviour disorders: Developmental biological and clinical perspectives. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. pp. 493–515.
  34. 34. Law J, Rush R, Anandan C, Cox M, Wood R (2012) Predicting language change between 3 and 5 Years and its implications for early identification. Pediatrics 130: 132–137.
  35. 35. Silva PA, Williams S, McGee R (1987) A longitudinal study of children with developmental language delay at age three: Later intelligence, reading and behaviour problems. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology 29: 630–640. pmid:2444484
  36. 36. Miniscalco C, Nygren G, Hagberg B, Kadesjö B, Gillberg C (2006) Neuropsychiatric and neurodevelopmental outcome of children at age 6 and 7 years who screened positive for language problems at 30 months. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology 48: 361–366. pmid:16608544
  37. 37. Taylor CL, Zubrick SR (2009) Predicting children's speech, language and reading impairment over time. International Journal of Speech-Language Pathology 11: 341–343. pmid:21841946
  38. 38. StataCorp. (2013) Stata Statistical Software: Release 13. College Station, TX: StataCorp LP.
  39. 39. Sylva K, Melhuish E, Sammons P, Siraj-Blatchford I, Taggart B (2008) Effective Pre-school and Primary Education 3–11 Project (EPPE 3–11) Final report from the primary phase: Pre-school, school and family influences on children’s development during Key Stage 2. Nottingham: DCSF. DCSF-RR061 DCSF-RR061.
  40. 40. High P, LaGasse L, Becker S, Ahlgren L, Gardner A (2000) Literacy promotion in primary care pediatrics: Can we make a difference? Pediatrics, 105, 927–934. pmid:10742349
  41. 41. Golova N, Alario A, Vivier P, Rodriguez M, High P (1999) Literacy promotion for Hispanic families in a primary care setting: A randomized, controlled trial. Journal of the Americal Academy of Pediatrics 103.
  42. 42. Dennison BA, Russo TJ, Burdick PA, Jenkins PL (2004) An intervention to reduce television viewing by preschool children. Archives of Pediatric Adolescent Medicine 158: 170–176.
  43. 43. Nutbrown C, Hannon P, Morgan A (2005) Early literacy practice with families: policy, practice and research. London: Sage Publications.
  44. 44. Wake M, Sanson A, Berthelsen D, Hardy P, Misson S, Smith K, et al. (2008) How well are Australian infants and children aged 4 to 5 years doing? Findings from the Longitudinal Study of Australian Children Wave 1. Australian Government: Department of Families, Housing, Community Services and Indigenous Affairs.
  45. 45. Barker DJ (2003) The developmental origins of adult disease. European Journal of Epidemiology 18: 733–736. pmid:12974544
  46. 46. Wright JC, Huston AC, Murphy KC, St. Peters M, Piñon M, Scantlin R, et al. (2001) The Relations of Early Television Viewing to School Readiness and Vocabulary of Children from Low-Income Families: The Early Window Project. Child Development 72: 1347–1366. pmid:11700636