Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

Education of staff in preschool aged classrooms in child care centers and child outcomes: A meta-analysis and systematic review

  • Olesya Falenchuk ,

    Roles Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Project administration, Resources, Validation, Visualization, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing

    michal.perlman@utoronto.ca (MP); olesya.falenchuk@utoronto.ca (OF)

    Affiliation Applied Psychology and Human Development, University of Toronto/OISE, Toronto, Ontario, Canada

  • Michal Perlman ,

    Roles Conceptualization, Formal analysis, Funding acquisition, Investigation, Methodology, Project administration, Resources, Supervision, Validation, Visualization, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing

    michal.perlman@utoronto.ca (MP); olesya.falenchuk@utoronto.ca (OF)

    Affiliation Applied Psychology and Human Development, University of Toronto/OISE, Toronto, Ontario, Canada

  • Evelyn McMullen,

    Roles Conceptualization, Formal analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Project administration, Resources, Validation, Visualization, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Applied Psychology and Human Development, University of Toronto/OISE, Toronto, Ontario, Canada

  • Brooke Fletcher,

    Roles Conceptualization, Methodology

    Affiliation Department of Gastroenterology, Alberta Children's Hospital, Calgary, Alberta, Canada

  • Prakesh S. Shah

    Roles Conceptualization, Formal analysis, Funding acquisition, Methodology, Resources, Supervision, Visualization, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliations Department of Pediatrics, Mount Sinai Hospital, Toronto, Ontario, Canada, Department of Pediatrics, University of Toronto, Toronto, Ontario, Canada, Institute of Health Policy, Management and Evaluation, University of Toronto, Toronto, Ontario, Canada

Abstract

Staff education is considered key to quality of early childhood education and care (ECEC) programs. However, findings about associations between staff education and children’s outcomes have been inconsistent. We conducted a systematic review and meta-analysis of associations between ECEC staff education and child outcomes. Searches of Medline, PsycINFO, and ERIC, websites of large datasets and reference sections of all retrieved articles were conducted. Eligible studies provided a statistical link between staff education and child outcomes for preschool-aged children in ECEC programs. Titles, abstracts and paper reviews as well as all data extraction were conducted by two independent raters. Of the 823 studies reviewed for eligibility, 39 met our inclusion criteria. Research in this area is observational in nature and subject to the inherent biases of that research design. Results from our systematic review were hampered by heterogeneity in how staff education was defined, variability in whose education was measured and the child outcomes that were assessed. However, overall the qualitative summary indicates that associations between staff education and childhood outcomes are non-existent to very borderline positive. In our meta-analysis of more homogeneous studies we identified certain positive, albeit very weak, associations between staff education and children’s language outcomes (specifically, vocabulary and letter word identification) and no significant association with a mathematics outcome (WJ Applied Problems). Thus, our findings suggest that within the range of education levels found in the existing literature, education is not a key driver of child outcomes. However, since we only explored levels of education that were reported in the literature, our findings cannot be used to argue for lowering education standards in ECEC settings.

Introduction

Over 60% of children under age six regularly attend some type of out-of-home child care program in the USA [13]]. Exposure to early childhood education and care (ECEC) is thought to positively influence children’s pre-academic skills including improvements in cognitive, language and social/emotional abilities [4] Furthermore, exposure to ECEC programs can reduce gaps in academic performance caused by social inequalities [5], as exposure to high quality environments has been shown to be most beneficial for children who come from at-risk backgrounds. However, research findings suggest that these benefits are only realized when quality of care is good [6]. For example, children exposed to better quality ECEC programs scored higher on measures of numeracy, receptive vocabulary and school readiness when compared to children who had been exposed to lower quality care [7]. Others have shown links between the quality of care and positive social outcomes for children [8,9]. Unfortunately, research on quality of care suggests that child care quality is generally mediocre at best [10,11]. High ECEC utilization rates and the frequently inadequate quality of programs have raised questions about which aspects of ECEC programs are important to child outcomes.

Quality in the ECEC context is comprised of structural and process characteristics. Structural quality characteristics include constructs such as staff/child ratios, group size and staff education. [12] These are generally quantifiable and therefore easier to regulate by government. Structural quality is thought to set the stage for the kinds of processes that take place in ECEC settings. Process quality consists of the different interactions that children experience with staff and other children in their ECEC program. Thus, it is thought to impact children directly [13].

Staff education is a characteristic of structural quality that is often listed as an indicator of high-quality child care [14]. It is thought to be an important driver of the types of interactions and activities (e.g., level of cognitive stimulation) that children experience directly in ECEC programs, thereby influencing their outcomes. In keeping with the idea that structural quality drives process quality, there is evidence that higher levels of staff education are associated with higher quality interactions between staff and children [15]. However, in terms of findings about the impact of staff education on child outcomes results have been less consistent. While, a positive relationship between staff education and child outcomes has been reported in some studies [16,17], other studies did not find such associations [1821].

Despite the lack of consistency in findings from studies that link staff education to child outcomes, education is still considered to be an influential quality indicator. For example, the American Academy of Pediatrics and the American Public Health Association recommend that early childhood educators have a minimum of a BA, a bachelor’s degree. They add that “at least 50% of all assistant teachers and teacher aides must have or be working on either a Child Development Associate (CDA) credential or equivalent, or an associate’s or higher degree in early childhood education/child development or equivalent.” (p. 12) [22]. This is consistent with recommendations by the National Association for the Education of Young Children [23] that early childhood educators should have a minimum of 4 to 5 years of post-secondary education. Finally, the UNICEF ‘Report Card 8’[24] sets 50% of staff having three years of higher education as the minimum quality benchmark for staff education in ECEC settings. Thus, major organizations in this sector highlight the importance of staff education and generally stipulate that staff working in ECEC settings should have undergraduate levels of education.

The perceived importance of staff education is further evidenced by the inclusion of staff education as part of Quality Ratings and Improvement Systems (QRISs), which are accountability systems used in the U.S. to monitor and improve ECEC program quality [25]. Programs receive a single amalgamated score based on number of characteristics (e.g., staff/child ratios, environmental rating scales etc.). In general staff education is one of the characteristics that is aggregated to create the program score.

Inconsistency in past findings makes it difficult to extract patterns of results from the literature on staff education. One reason for this is that education can be defined in different ways. Approaches to the measurement of staff education include: 1) years of education; 2) scales based on completed degrees; 3) defining thresholds or “levels” of education (e.g., BA/No-BA) and 4) rating participants in terms of whether or not they adhere to local quality standards for staff education. While these are sensible approaches, the variety in how education is operationalized makes it challenging to integrate findings across studies. In addition, ECEC classrooms are staffed by multiple adults. Different researchers can adopt different strategies with regards to whose education to include. For example, one strategy is to collect education information from different members of the staff in a classroom, another is to focus on the “lead” staff member (which can be an arbitrary distinction when ECEC programs adopt a team-teaching approach), and yet another is to simply collect education information for the staff member who is available when data collection takes place. Thus, while there is a growing body of research that examines the associations between staff education and child outcomes, it is difficult for individual stakeholders to extract conclusions or policy directives from this research. The complexities of this literature point to the need for a systematic review and meta-analysis of the association between staff education and child outcomes. To our knowledge such a review has not been published to date. Moreover, payment to staff is the major driver of costs in ECEC programs, and better-educated staff requires higher levels of remuneration. Thus, it is important to know whether there is a reliable relationship between ECEC staff education and child outcomes.

We set out to review whether higher levels of staff education are associated with better child outcomes. We explored whether there are more associations between staff education and child outcomes for at-risk children when compared to non at-risk children. Finally, we explored whether the pattern of associations differed when the education levels of multiple staff in a room were collected vs. those of the lead staff member only.

Methods

Search strategy

Electronic databases (PsycINFO, Medline, and ERIC) were searched for studies published until July 3, 2015. Two different search strategies were used to identify the eligible studies: 1) searching for education-related terms and child outcomes and, 2) a global approach that involved searching for a large number of quality indicators simultaneously. The second strategy allowed us to identify papers in which education was one of the control variables in the analyses. Thus, it ensured that we would capture studies that contribute to the current effort even if education was not the primary goal of the original study. Search terms are provided in Tables A-D in S1 File. The websites for the following large databases of ECEC quality and child outcomes were searched to locate research reports: Cost, Quality and Outcomes Study (CQO) [26]; Early Childhood Longitudinal Study (ECLS) [27]; Effective Provision of Pre-School Education (EPPE) Project [28]; Head Start Impact Study (HS) [29]; National Center for Early Development and Learning (NCEDL) Multi-State Study of Pre-Kindergarten [30]; State-Wide Early Education Program Study (SWEEP) [31]; Family and Child Experiences Survey (FACES) [32]; and the National Institute of Child Health and Human Development (NICHD) Study of Early Child Care and Youth Development [33]. Finally, reference sections of all retrieved studies were searched to locate additional studies. Finally, the search was limited to studies published in the English language only.

Inclusion criteria

Types of studies.

This review focused on cohort, cross-sectional and longitudinal studies reporting statistical associations between staff education in preschool classrooms to children’s academic competence (e.g., language and math) as well as cognitive, physical and social-emotional development outcomes. Studies that used a combination of staff education and other measures to create an overall quality composite that could not be disaggregated were excluded. (See Table 1 for a more detailed description of study selection criteria and rationale).

thumbnail
Table 1. Inclusion criteria for systematic review and rationale.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0183673.t001

Types of participants and settings.

We focused only on studies of preschool age children (30 to 72 months) as this age group serves the largest number of children and much of the research has been conducted on this age group [34,35]. Only the studies conducted in center-based ECEC settings were included (preschool, pre-kindergarten and Head Start programs, nursery schools, and child care centers). Studies examining only home-based child care or a mixture of home-based and center-based child care that could not be disentangled were not included.

Outcomes

We cast a wide net regarding the child outcomes we included reflecting an understanding of the classroom context as having an impact on children that goes “beyond achievement tests” [36]. Thus, we compiled studies that used a broad range of outcomes and included any measure of academic competence (e.g., language and math) as well as cognitive, physical and social-emotional development outcomes. See Table 2 for a general description of each of the dimensions within the domains that were eligible for inclusion [37]. Also see S3 File for the list of the child outcome measures within each domain. The data in the reviewed studies were collected from staff, parents, and/or direct assessment of children. Measures that focus on dyads (e.g., attachment) were excluded as child and adult effects are difficult to disentangle in such measures.

Selection strategy

The selection of eligible studies was performed in two steps, each conducted by a pair of independent raters: 1) the titles and abstracts of the documents were screened for relevance; 2) full review of the relevant documents was conducted to determine if they met inclusion criteria for this study. In both steps pairs of trained raters included graduate students and authors of this paper (EM, OF and MP) in the Department of Applied Psychology and Human Development at the university of Toronto. Discrepancies between the raters were resolved through discussion or in consultation with the research methodologist (one of the authors–OF) who made the final decision. A systematic review protocol and data extraction form were developed by the research team and are available upon request from the first author.

Data analysis

All studies that met our inclusion criteria were included in the systematic review. Subsets of studies that could be meta-analyzed together were also identified. In order to be meta-analyzed, studies had to report child outcomes measured with the same instrument and have staff education operationalized in the same way. While a meta-analysis can be conducted with as few as two studies [38] there is little empirical guidance in terms of the minimum number of studies required to conduct a meta-analysis. One source of information is a comprehensive review of all meta-analyses included in the Cochrane’s database, which found that the median number of studies included in meta-analyses was three [39]. Based on this finding we adopted three as the minimum for the number of studies required to conduct a meta-analysis in this review.

To increase homogeneity among studies that were meta-analyzed, only studies that ensured children’s exposure to the program were included. To do so, we only meta-analyzed studies that used child pre-scores as a covariate, used gain scores in analyses, or in which the authors stated explicitly that children had been in the program for a period of time prior to their assessment.

To avoid dependency issues when multiple studies were based on secondary analyses of subsamples drawn from the same dataset, only the study with the largest sample size was selected for inclusion in meta-analyses. [40]

For ethical and logistical reasons, all of the studies included in this review have a correlational/observational design, and, therefore, there is little variability in their scores on standard measures of study quality used for meta-analysis [41,42]. We did not proceed with these measures assessing study quality, as they did not allow us to differentiate between studies. In addition, due to sample size constraints we were not able to directly test for study quality as a moderator in our analyses. While we were not able to rate and test the impact of quality of study directly, we do note when papers were peer reviewed and provide readers with very detailed information about studies in the systematic review. Thus, we address the issue of study quality in this more qualitative way. It is worth noting that our selection criteria for the meta-analyses resulted in relatively stronger papers being included. This is because we required outcomes to have been used in three or more papers and this resulted in only the relatively psychometrically stronger measures being included. Only statistics that accounted for covariates (e.g., child and family characteristics) were combined within a single meta-analysis, which also pulled for the inclusion of the better quality research. Finally, when multiple studies drew from the same sample, we included the study with the largest sample size, which also pulled for the inclusion of studies with stronger methodologies.

For each meta-analysis, the I2 index was used to test statistical heterogeneity [43]. Large I2 values (>70%) indicate high heterogeneity in findings from different studies and reduce the reliability of the pooled results. Meta-analyses were performed using the random-effects models with the Comprehensive Meta-Analysis software (Version 3, see S2 File for conversion formulas) [44].

Results

Description of studies

Results from the search and study selection are provided in Fig 1. Of the 823 potential studies related to the staff education, 784 were excluded because they did not meet all of the inclusion criteria. Thus, 39 studies were eligible for the current review. Of the 39 studies selected for this review, there are 26 peer reviewed journal articles, 10 reports and 3 books.

thumbnail
Fig 1. Flow diagram for study selection.

Adapted from Moher et al. [45]

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0183673.g001

The characteristics of the 39 eligible studies are presented in Table 3 (the acronyms used in Table 3 can be found in S5 File). Thirty-four of the studies report the results for a single sample, 3 studies [19,46,47] provide the results for two samples of data, one study [48] has data for three samples of children, and one study [49] reports results from seven datasets of preschool programs. Table 3 contains a separate record for each of the 50 independent samples identified through our searches.

thumbnail
Table 3. Description of studies meeting inclusion criteriaa.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0183673.t003

One study was conducted in Canada [68] (covering 4 Atlantic provinces, New Brunswick, Prince Edward Island, Newfoundland and Nova Scotia) and one study used a sample collected across multiple countries [70] (including Finland, Hong Kong, Ireland, Italy, Poland, Thailand, and the United States). The rest of the studies were conducted in the U.S.

Many of the studies had overlapping samples. Nine studies contained samples that were drawn from both the NCEDL’s Multi-State Study and SWEEP study [19,34,48,49,59,66,72,73,81], two used the Head Start FACES 2000 Cohort sample [79,80], three utilized the 2003 Head Start FACES sample [16,49,74], and two studies were based on the 2006 Head Start FACES cohort sample [50,76]. Also, two studies included samples drawn from the NICHD Study of Early Child Care [49,71], and two studies included samples drawn from the Preschool Curriculum Evaluation Research (PCER) Program [49,62]. Furthermore, three studies were based on data from the Georgia Early Childhood Study [49,81,65].

Description of participants

Teachers and programs.

The data were collected from the samples of 15 to 887 staff members (median = 242.5) working in 14 to 887 classrooms (median = 257.5) from 16 to 704 child care centers (median = 135). In 14% of the samples used in this study education data were collected from all staff in the classroom and in 66% of the samples education data were collected from one primary caregiver, usually the head staff member (20% of the studies did not report on the staff surveyed). Between 12% and 100% of staff in the study samples reported having a BA or higher (n = 27, median = 55%), or between 13.8 and 16 years of education (n = 7, median = 15.67%). Statistics for the remaining 16 samples were not reported.

Children and families.

Sample sizes of the included studies ranged from 51 to 3584 children (median = 945), involving 33,175 children overall for non-overlapping samples (i.e., for this purpose we only counted children from each of the databases used by multiple studies once). Between 43% and 56% of children in the study samples are males (median = 50%). Mean age of children across the samples ranges between 36 and 56 months (median = 51 months). The samples contained between 5.5% and 100% of non-Caucasian children (median = 59%). Children from minority ethnic backgrounds were primarily African American, Hispanic and Latino.

Of the 30 non-overlapping samples in this review (i.e., no child counted more than once), 20 authors identified their sample of children as being “at-risk”, 8 indicated that the sample was not “at-risk” and 2 did not provide this type information. The specific details of the SES index used to determine “at-risk” status was rarely reported. Authors indicated that families were poor, came from low-income households, had incomes below a poverty threshold, or were receiving child care subsidies. The percentage of children considered “at-risk” in these samples ranged from 50% and 100% (median = 69%), with the exception of one study (i.e., 38%).

Operationalization of staff education.

In the studies used in this review, staff education was operationalized in a variety of ways. Detailed information about the way staff education was operationalized in each study included in this review is provided in the Quality Measures column of Table 3. For example, some studies compared staff members with and without an Associate of Arts (AA) or Higher degree [50,52,79,80], a Bachelor of Arts (BA) degree [49,62,65,66], or a BA or Higher degree [16,18,34]. Other studies used the number of years of education, e.g., 1 to16, where a value of 16 would be equivalent to a post graduate degree [54,5860]. Ordinal scales were also used to capture the level of education measured on 4- to 7-point scales, such as (1) Some college only, (2) AA or 2 year, (3) BA, (4) At least 1 year beyond BA, and (5) MA and above) [46,48,60]. Some studies operationalized staff education in multiple ways and reported separate analyses for each [34,49]. In addition, as ECEC classrooms are staffed with multiple staff, some studies selected only the lead staff [56,57], while other studies collected education data from all staff in the classroom [18,48]. To reflect the different ways in which staff education was operationalized across the studies, we organized the tables for the systematic review into sections based on the type of operationalization that was used in the analysis. Meta-analyses were also conducted separately for each type of operationalization.

Outcomes

Studies selected for this review provided associations between staff education and the scores of 112 measures of academic competence (e.g., language and math) as well as cognitive, physical and social-emotional development outcomes. All measures are listed in S3 File. The majority of the measures were reported in a single study only. The measures used in the largest number of studies captured receptive language (using the Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test; PPVT) (21 studies, 28 samples) and early math skills (using the Woodcock Johnson-Applied Problems; WJ-AP) (19 studies, 27 samples).

Systematic review

Data summarizing all of the findings from the 39 eligible studies (50 samples of data) are presented Tables A-H in S4 File (A snapshot summary of just those outcomes analyzed in three or more samples is also provided in Figs 2 to 5). The first column in all these tables and figures indicates the type of operationalization of staff education used in the reviewed studies. The results of statistical analyses are presented in these tables using a variety of symbols indicating the type of analysis and its statistical significance. To ensure the comprehensiveness of our review, all models tested in each of the papers are included. Each symbol represents a unique model. This allows the reader to assess how many effects were significant given the total number of different models/analyses authors reported in each of these studies. For example, in Fig 2, Aikens (2010) [50], conducted linear regressions for 6 different models, reporting Beta scores for each.

thumbnail
Fig 2. Systematic review of the associations between staff education (AA or Higher; BA or Higher) and child outcomes.

a Abbreviations: AA = Associate’s Degree; BA = Bachelor’s Degree. Symbols bolded are significant and positive, symbols bolded and italicized are significant and negative, and symbols in grey are non-significant. Star = Zero Order Pearson’s Correlation, Unfilled circle = Beta, Filled square = Unstandardized Coefficient, Downward arrow = Effect Size. aTo improve the readability of this complex table, eleven papers [17,21,5557,61,63,67,70,71,82] that had an outcome that appeared in only that one paper were omitted from this table. Several analyses from other papers that had idiosyncratic outcomes are also excluded. For a comprehensive display of all of the data for all of the child outcomes see Tables A-H in S4 File. bThis paper is one of a series of Meta-Analyses and Systematic Reviews assessing the relationship between child care quality and children’s outcomes; therefore, superscript letters below are in reference to various large databases that samples in these papers were drawn from. These letters have been kept consistent across the series for our readers. cSamples within papers are described in more detail in Table 3 in the manuscript. dAcronyms for child outcomes are listed in S3 File and for journals, large samples and covariates are in S5 File. eIdentifying Letters (also refers to as Alphabet Recognition Test, Naming Letters, and Letter-Naming Test). ANational Center for Early Development and Learning Dataset (NCEDL, 2002, 2004); BHead Start Family and Children Experiences Survey (FACES, 2006 Cohort); KHead Start Family and Children Experiences Survey (FACES, 2000 Cohort); LHead Start Family and Children Experiences Survey (FACES, 2003 Cohort); MHead Start Family and Children Experiences Survey (FACES, 2009 Cohort).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0183673.g002

thumbnail
Fig 3. Systematic review of the associations between staff education (Has a BA) and child outcomes.

a Abbreviations: BA = Bachelor’s Degree. Symbols bolded are significant and positive, symbols bolded and italicized are significant and negative, and symbols in grey are non-significant. Star = Zero Order Pearson’s Correlation, Unfilled circle = Beta, Filled square = Unstandardized Coefficient, Downward arrow = Effect Size, Filled circle = F-Ratio. aTo improve the readability of this complex table, eleven papers [17,21,5557,61,63,67,70,71,82] that had an outcome that appeared in only that one paper were omitted from this table. Several analyses from other papers that had idiosyncratic outcomes are also excluded. For a comprehensive display of all of the data for all of the child outcomes see Tables A-H in S4 File. bThis paper is one of a series of Meta-Analyses and Systematic Reviews assessing the relationship between child care quality and children’s outcomes; therefore, superscript letters below are in reference to various large databases that samples in these papers were drawn from. These letters have been kept consistent across the series for our readers. cSamples within papers are described in more detail in Table 3 in the manuscript. dAcronyms for child outcomes are listed in S3 File and for journals, large samples and covariates are in S5 File. eIdentifying Letters (also refers to as Alphabet Recognition Test, Naming Letters, and Letter-Naming Test). ANational Center for Early Development and Learning Dataset (NCEDL, 2002, 2004); FGeorgia Early Childhood Study (GECS, 2002); HEarly Head Start (EHS, 2001–2003 Cohort); LHead Start Family and Children Experiences Survey (FACES, 2003 Cohort); MHead Start Family and Children Experiences Survey (FACES, 2009 Cohort); QNational Institute of Child Health and Human Development (NICHD, 1995–1996); UPreschool Curriculum Evaluation Research (PCER, 1999–2003);YAMore is Four North Carolina Study (2002–2003 Cohort); YBMore is Four North Carolina Study (2003–2004) Cohort; ZColorado QRIS.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0183673.g003

thumbnail
Fig 4. Systematic review of the associations between staff education (Years of Education) and child outcomes.

a Abbreviations: BA = Bachelor’s Degree. Symbols bolded are significant and positive, symbols bolded and italicized are significant and negative, and symbols in grey are non-significant. Star = Zero Order Pearson’s Correlation, Unfilled circle = Beta, Filled square = Unstandardized Coefficient, Key clover = Partial Correlation, Downward arrow = Effect Size. aTo improve the readability of this complex table, eleven papers [17,21,5557,61,63,67,70,71,82] that had an outcome that appeared in only that one paper were omitted from this table. Several analyses from other papers that had idiosyncratic outcomes are also excluded. For a comprehensive display of all of the data for all of the child outcomes see Tables A-H in S4 File. bThis paper is one of a series of Meta-Analyses and Systematic Reviews assessing the relationship between child care quality and children’s outcomes; therefore, superscript letters below are in reference to various large databases that samples in these papers were drawn from. These letters have been kept consistent across the series for our readers. cSamples within papers are described in more detail in Table 3 in the manuscript. dAcronyms for child outcomes are listed in S3 File and for journals, large samples and covariates are in S5 File. eIdentifying Letters (also refers to as Alphabet Recognition Test, Naming Letters, and Letter-Naming Test). ANational Center for Early Development and Learning Dataset (NCEDL, 2002, 2004); DCost, Quality and Outcomes Study (CQO, 1993–1994); NEarly Childhood Longitudinal Study (ECLS-B, 2001–2006 Birth Cohort); S8-County Region of North-Central Indiana (Year NR).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0183673.g004

thumbnail
Fig 5. Systematic review of the associations between staff education (Level of Education, Ordinal) and child outcomes.

a Abbreviations: BA = Bachelor’s Degree. Symbols bolded are significant and positive, symbols bolded and italicized are significant and negative, and symbols in grey are non-significant. Star = Zero Order Pearson’s Correlation, Unfilled circle = Beta, Filled square = Unstandardized Coefficient, Key clover = Partial Correlation, Downward arrow = Effect Size, Filled circle = F-Ratio. aTo improve the readability of this complex table, eleven papers[17,21,5557,61,63,67,70,71,82] that had an outcome that appeared in only that one paper were omitted from this table. Several analyses from other papers that had idiosyncratic outcomes are also excluded. For a comprehensive display of all of the data for all of the child outcomes see Tables A-H in S4 File. bThis paper is one of a series of Meta-Analyses and Systematic Reviews assessing the relationship between child care quality and children’s outcomes; therefore, superscript letters below are in reference to various large databases that samples in these papers were drawn from. These letters have been kept consistent across the series for our readers. cSamples within papers are described in more detail in Table 3 in the manuscript. dAcronyms for child outcomes are listed in S3 File and for journals, large samples and covariates are in S5 File. eIdentifying Letters (also refers to as Alphabet Recognition Test, Naming Letters, and Letter-Naming Test). ANational Center for Early Development and Learning Dataset (NCEDL, 2002, 2004); FGeorgia Early Childhood Study (GECS, 2002); HEarly Head Start (EHS, 2001–2003 Cohort); JHead Start Family and Children Experiences Survey (FACES, 1997 Cohort); LHead Start Family and Children Experiences Survey (FACES, 2003 Cohort); QNational Institute of Child Health and Human Development (NICHD, 1995–1996); S8-County Region of North-Central Indiana (Year NR); UPreschool Curriculum Evaluation Research (PCER, 1999–2003);YAMore is Four North Carolina Study (2002–2003 Cohort); YBMore is Four North Carolina Study (2003–2004) Cohort.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0183673.g005

To facilitate interpretation, these tables are grouped based on the types of outcomes reported in the study. Across all eligible studies, 477 distinct statistical analyses quantifying the association between staff education and child outcomes were reported. These statistical analyses included 112 unique child outcome measures associated with the different ways of operationalizing staff education described above (see S3 File).

Approach to learning outcomes.

Two studies [61,78] reported an association between staff education and approach to learning outcomes. Zellman (2008) [78] used five subscales of Child Behavior Inventory measure and Epstein (1993) [61] used the Initiative subscale of Child Observation Record measure. Both studies showed a non-significant association between this type of child outcomes and staff education.

Cognitive outcomes.

Fifteen studies reported an association between staff education and cognitive outcomes with 17 different measures (listed in S3 File). Most of these measures were used in a single study with the exception of Intelligence subscale of the Child behavior Inventory (2 studies), the Color Naming test (2 studies), and the Preschool Inventory (3 studies). The majority of these studies found no association between staff education and this type of child outcome. Only 3 studies [17,47,71] reported significant relationships. However, the results of these studies are mixed: while two studies [47,71] show a positive relationship between staff education and school readiness and intelligence, the study by Mashburn (2004) [17] found a negative relationship between staff education and child academic skills and school readiness.

Language outcomes.

The associations between staff education and child language outcomes were reported for 43 samples drawn from all 39 studies included in this systematic review. The results for 35 different measures of language development were reported. These measures were used to evaluate language development as a whole (e.g., Language subscales from Child Observation Record, Academic Rating Scale, and DIAL-R) or some aspects of language development, such as letter recognition, vocabulary, phonological awareness, and rhyming. Most of these measures were used in a single study. Only 8 measures were used in 2 or more studies. The Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test (PPVT) was reported in the largest number of studies [31].

The vast majority of the results in these studies showed no significant association between staff education and child language outcomes. A small number of significant results showed a positive relationship between the two types of indicators. However, these significant results came from the studies that report multiple child language outcomes and mixed findings across the outcomes (e.g., Howes 2008, Sabol 2013) [66,73].

Mathematics outcomes.

Association between staff education and 7 different math outcomes was reported for 32 samples in 23 studies. The WJ Applied Problems measure of child competency in mathematics is the only outcome that was reported in a large number of studies (n = 19). The rest of the math outcomes were reported in 1to 3 studies. Most of the results reported across these studies suggest a lack of association between staff education and math outcomes. A few significant results indicated a positive association and are reported in two papers only [34,49].

Physical health and development outcomes.

Two studies reported the relationship between staff education and child physical development outcomes measured with three different indicators. None of the results in these studies showed a significant association.

Social-emotional: Positive behavior outcomes.

The results of statistical analyses investigating the relationship between staff education and child positive behavior were reported for 25 different outcomes in 9 studies. Most of these outcomes were used in a single study. Each study involved multiple indicators of positive behavior and multiple ways of operationalizing staff education. Two outcomes (Social Skills Rating System and Teacher Child Rating Scale) were reported in 4 studies each.

The studies included in this systematic review show mixed results about the association between staff education and positive behavior. For most indicators reported in a single study the results are not significant. However, for the two indicators reported in multiple studies (Social Skills Rating System and Teacher Child Rating Scale), two out of four studies showed a significant positive association between staff education and positive behavior.

Social-emotional: Problem behavior outcomes.

Problem behavior was measured in 17 samples used in 15 studies with 15 different outcome variables. With exception of Behavior Problems subscale from the Teacher Child Rating Scale that was used in three studies, all other outcomes were used in a single study. Most studies reported a single problem behavior outcome.

The majority of the reported results showed no significant association between staff education and problem behavior outcomes. A few significant results show positive relationship between higher levels of staff education and decreases in child behavior problems during the school year [47,71,80].

Overall, the vast majority of the results reported in the 39 studies (50 samples) we reviewed suggest small or no associations between staff education and children’s academic competence (e.g., language, mathematics), as well as cognitive, physical and social-emotional outcomes. The characteristics of studies that reported significant associations were compared with studies that did not. For example, we qualitatively explored whether studies conducted more recently showed more/fewer associations between education and child outcomes than older studies. We adopted a similar strategy to exploring study design (e.g., cross-sectional vs. longitudinal), operationalization of staff education (e.g., years of education vs. degrees) and whether authors reported statistics that did or did not account for covariates (i.e., beta coefficients from regressions vs. Pearson correlations and F-ratios). The small number of significant effects that were found were reported across studies with these different characteristics. This suggests that these characteristics do not explain why a very small number of studies that reported significant effect did so, while the others did not.

Meta-analyses

We identified that 3 outcomes were used in studies with the similar operationalization of education fulfilling the criteria for meta-analyses. These were: one math outcome (WJ Applied Problems) and two language outcomes (PPVT and WJ Letter Word ID). Meta-analyses in these studies were based on three to four studies. The results of meta-analyses relating teachers’ BA degree with these outcomes are presented in Fig 6 and those relating teachers’ level of education to these outcomes are presented in Fig 7.

thumbnail
Fig 6. Meta-analysis results for the associations between staff education measured as a dichotomy, having a BA or not, and child outcomes.

Significant findings are noted with asterisks.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0183673.g006

thumbnail
Fig 7. Meta-analysis results for the association between staff education measured as level of education (ordinal) and child outcomes.

Significant findings are noted with asterisks.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0183673.g007

The meta-analysis identified a small positive relationship between the PPVT and staff education (pooled r = 0.04, 95% confidence interval 0.02 to 0.06, I2 = 0%). Similarly, we identified a weak positive association between this WJ-Letter Word ID outcome and staff education (pooled r = 0.04, 95% confidence interval 0.02 to 0.07, I2 = 0%). However, pooled results for the mathematics outcome (WJ Applied Problems, based on 4 studies, 7113 children) showed no association between these variables (pooled r = 0.03, 95% confidence interval 0.00 to 0.05, I2 = 77.2%).

Two meta-analyses were conducted to evaluate associations between level of staff education measured by ordinal categories, such as having high school diploma, AA, BA, or a Graduate degree [49] with child outcomes (see Fig 7). Pooled results for the PPVT (based on 3 studies, 4709 children) showed a significant positive association between these two variables (pooled r = 0.05, 95% confidence interval 0.02 to 0.08, I2 = 0.0%). The analysis of WJ-Applied Problems was based on three studies (5253 children). The results showed no significant association between this outcome and staff education (pooled r = 0.05, 95% confidence interval—0.02 to 0.07, I2 = 53.2%).

Discussion

The goal of this comprehensive systematic review was to identify associations between staff education and academic competence (e.g., language, mathematics), as well as cognitive, physical and social-emotional outcomes of pre-school children in ECEC settings. We found that some studies reported weak positive associations between staff education and certain outcomes whereas the majority of studies reported no association. Marked heterogeneity in assessment of education and outcomes was identified and is discussed below. A few studies that defined education and reported outcomes in a similar manner were meta-analyzed and revealed weak positive associations between staff education and receptive language and letter identification. It is important to note that since much of the variability in child outcomes is driven by child and family factors, even small effects associated with ECEC programs, particularly when family factors are accounted for, may be meaningful. This is especially true when language is the outcome as it has been shown that effects on language outcome can have long lasting effects [83] that spillover into multiple domains of a child’s life including social, emotional and cognitive outcomes [84,85]. The finding that staff education is associated only with language outcomes may reflect recent emphasis on language in literacy in ECEC staff training programs or it may reflect the predispositions of the individuals who are drawn to careers in ECEC settings, who may be more oriented towards language than math. However, the results from our meta-analysis represent a small minority of selected studies and should be viewed with caution and as hypothesis generating. The lack of associations with other outcomes (e.g., classroom management styles and children’s behavioral outcomes) is unexpected and requires further research to identify avenues for improvement.

We explored whether including all staff in the room was associated with a different pattern of results than including the education of the lead staff members only. A substantially higher proportion of studies that used only the lead teacher’s education in the analyses reported one or more significant effect, relative to studies that used aggregated education level of all staff in a classroom. This is surprising given that exploring the education of head-teachers only provides a partial picture of the education of staff in a given classroom. One possible explanation is that the lead teachers’ education is more important in terms of driving children’s experiences in the classroom and therefore their outcomes than the education levels of the other staff. Another, is that the methods researchers use to aggregate the data when they collected education levels from all staff (e.g., averaging or weighing some staff more than others) result in a classroom level index that did not reflect children’s experiences in the classrooms. Unfortunately, reporting of the method of aggregation is highly inconsistent across studies and did not allow us to explore this issue. It is important to note, however, that only 7 studies assessed the education of all staff in the room while 32 assessed the lead teachers’ education only. Given these low numbers, the above analysis needs to be interpreted with caution.

We also explored whether the pattern of results was different in studies in which the sample was considered “at-risk” (see Table 3). To do this we compared the number of samples that reported one or more significant effects in studies in which the sample was not “at-risk” (as described by the authors) vs. studies where the sample consisted of 50% or more children considered “at-risk” (based on poverty). Overall, the proportion of samples that reported significant results was similar regardless of the sample “at-risk” status. This suggests that the differential susceptibility hypothesis is not at play for staff education in the literature included in this review. However, this qualitative analysis, based on a small number of samples must be interpreted with caution.

Heterogeneity in research on associations between staff education and child outcomes

The body of research we reviewed is very heterogeneous in terms of the operationalization of staff education, reported statistics, covariates used in the analyses, and child outcome measures used. Our review identified 112 distinct outcome measures that spanned a wide range of abilities. These were collected through direct assessment of children and through surveys of parents and ECEC staff. Thus, there was substantial heterogeneity in what was measured and how the information was gathered. Studies relied on different analyses and therefore reported different statistics. Some studies reported zero-order correlations, others reported only the results of regression analyses. Where possible we converted statistics to maximize the number of studies that could be meta-analyzed but the information needed to do this was not always available in the papers. When researchers conducted regressions the number and nature of covariates they included varied. Finally, staff education was operationalized in a variety of different ways across the reviewed studies (number of years of education, attainment of a particular level/degree, levels of education measured with ordinal categories).

Reporting of study methodology and results is inconsistent and at times incomplete and limited our ability to integrate across papers. For example, some of the studies did not report the demographics of the sample making it hard to understand the generalizability of the findings [52,61]. Other studies did not report the descriptive statistics for quality indicators and/or child outcomes [46,57,61]. Clearly, greater consistency in methodologies and reporting is needed to facilitate integration across studies in the future.

We dealt with this substantial heterogeneity by being inclusive in our systematic review while taking a conservative approach to ensure that the studies we meta-analyzed were sufficiently similar to one another. Specifically, for the meta-analyses we selected outcomes that were used in three or more papers, which meant that the outcome measures that were meta-analyzed tended to be strong psychometrically. We did not combine statistics that did and did not include covariates and we reduced heterogeneity in exposure to ECEC programs by only including studies that ensured that children had had at least some exposure to their ECEC program. The downside of this approach is that it limited the number of papers that could be meta-analyzed. The upside is that we reduced the heterogeneity of what we meta-analyzed thereby increasing our confidence that the studies we meta-analyzed were sufficiently similar to be combined.

Methodological limitations of research on associations between staff education and child outcomes

A major methodological issue in this area of research is the reliance on observational/correlational studies based on samples that are not randomly selected. Researchers and policy makers need to work together to look for opportunities to study the effects of staff education in naturally occurring settings such as changes to regulation with regard to staff education. Another issue is that children and staff are nested in classrooms that are nested in centers. Yet, the lack of independence between units of analyses is often not accounted for in the statistics researchers in this area use. In addition, there is often a mismatch between the unit of analysis of staff education (generally measured at the staff or classroom level) and child outcomes (which are measured at the child level). With a few exceptions, the studies included in this review were based on U.S. samples. Future research needs to expand on the status of this important aspect of ECEC quality in other cultures.

Conceptual explanations of the weak associations between staff education and child outcomes

Several conceptual factors may explain the limited association between staff education and child outcomes that we identified. Other staff characteristics such as the area of specialization, years of experience, professional development opportunities, and knowledge of child development may need to be considered simultaneously. Having a particular level of education may not result in the assumed knowledge that would help staff support children’s development. This may be because of a potential disconnect between the content covered in formal education programs and what makes for good quality experiences for children. Alternatively, even if staff are taught (and retain what they are taught) about best practices for supporting child development, they might not implement what they learn. For example, researchers have found that educators’ practices are driven by previous beliefs, knowledge, past practices [86] as well as their own childhood experiences [87], rather than best practices. A focus on what staff do may be more productive than focusing on their education as a proxy of their behavior. There is some support for this idea from the parenting literature. Findings from the Effective Provision of Preschool Education Project based on a short interview asking parents about what they do with their children identified that, “While other family factors such as parents' education and SES are also important, the extent of home learning activities exerts a greater and independent influence on educational attainment” (p. 106) [88]. Perhaps this is also true of staff in the ECEC sector. The increasing focus in the literature on staff-child interactions is consistent with this emphasis. However, our recent review of staff/child interactions (the Classroom Assessment Scoring System) [89] also identified a few associations with child outcomes [90]. Finally, the differential susceptibility hypothesis suggests that some children are more sensitive to variability in the quality of their environments [91]. Thus, having better-educated staff may be important for some (e.g., at-risk) children. While we did not find such a pattern in this review, due to the limited number of studies available for meta-analysis, we were not able to test this statistically. Despite adding to the complexity of an already highly complex research area, these conceptual issues cannot be ignored for the field to move forward.

Limitations of the current study

One of the main limitations of the current study is the methodological limitations of the literature we covered which were described above. In addition, the small number and heterogeneous nature of the studies included in this paper severely limited our ability to meta-analyze across papers. Further, due to the small number of studies included in our meta-analyses, the effects of possible moderators on the relationship between staff education and child outcomes could not be examined. Moderating variables (e.g., does education matter when staff/child ratios are poorer?) should be explored once more scientific evidence is collected and presented in more homogeneous way.

Conclusion

Results from our systematic review were hampered by heterogeneity in the definition of staff education, variability in whether all or only some staff’s education was measured, as well as variability in the child outcomes that were collected. However, overall the qualitative summary indicates that associations between staff education and childhood outcomes are, at best, non-existent to very borderline positive. In our meta-analysis of more homogeneous studies, we identified certain positive, albeit very weak, associations between staff education and children’s language outcomes (specifically, vocabulary and letter word identification) while no significant associations were found with a mathematics outcome (WJ Applied Problems). Nonetheless, by compiling the existing literature in a systematic way, this study has highlighted a number of important methodological issues that need to be addressed in future research. While the meta-analyses revealed a few associations, the lack of associations is meaningful as it draws researchers, policy makers and other stakeholders’ attention to other avenues for improving quality in ECEC settings. These include examining practices in settings that train early childhood educators and the professional development opportunities available in this area, as well as assessing how staff interact with children rather than using structural variables such as education as measures of ECEC program quality.

Supporting information

S1 File. Search syntax for staff education.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0183673.s001

(PDF)

S3 File. Child outcomes for staff education.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0183673.s003

(PDF)

S4 File. Systematic review tables for staff education.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0183673.s004

(PDF)

Acknowledgments

Special thanks to Charles Pascal, Elaine Baxter-Trahair, Debra Mathews, Christa Japel, Lynn Karoly, Deborah Phillips and Elizabeth Uleryk. We thank Jenaya Webb, OISE/UT’s Public Services Librarian for her invaluable assistance throughout this project and to Joseph Beyene for his statistical support. Thank you also to Stephanie Aglipay, Teresa Pietropaolo, Anastasia Bukowski Nadina Mahadeo, Natasha Musrap, and Sonny Yeh for all of their help with the huge organizational task associated with this project.

References

  1. 1. Capizzano J, Main R. Many young children spend long hours in child care [Internet]. Washington, DC: U.S. Urban Institute; 2005. http://www.urban.org/sites/default/files/alfresco/publication-pdfs/311154-Many-Young-Children-Spend-Long-Hours-in-Child-Care.PDF
  2. 2. Sinha M. Childcare in Canada [Internet]. Ottawa, ON; 2014. http://www.statcan.gc.ca/pub/89-652-x/89-652-x2014005-eng.pdf.
  3. 3. U.S. Department of Labor, Women’s Bureau. 20 Facts on Women Workers [Internet]. Washington DC; 2000. http://babel.hathitrust.org/cgi/pt?id=uiug.30112048171083
  4. 4. NICHD ECCRN. Early child care and children’s development prior to school entry: results from the NICHD study of early child care. Am Educ Res J. 2002;39: 133–164.
  5. 5. Laurin J, Geoffroy M, Boivin M, Japel C, Raynault M, Tremblay R, et al. Child care services, socioeconomic inequalities, and academic performance. Pediatrics. 2015;136. pmid:26598459
  6. 6. National Institute of Child Health and Human Development Early Child Care Research Network. The relation of child care to cognitive and language development. Child Dev. 2000;71: 960–80. pmid:11016559
  7. 7. Côté S, Mongeau C, Japel C, Xu Q, Séquin J, Tremblay R. Child care quality and cognitive development: trajectories leading to better preacademic skills. Child Dev. 2013;84: 752–66. pmid:23083205
  8. 8. Goelman H, Zdaniuk B, Boyce W, Armstrong J, Essex M. Maternal mental health, child care quality, and children’s behavior. J Appl Dev Psychol. 2014;35: 347–356.
  9. 9. Gialamas A, Mittinty M, Sawyer M, Zubrick S, Lynch J. Child care quality and children’s cognitive and socio-emotional development: An Australian longitudinal study. Early Child Dev Care. 2013;184: 977–997.
  10. 10. Helburn SW. Cost, quality and child outcomes in child care centers [Internet]. Denver, CO: University of Colorado at Denver; 1995. http://www.researchconnections.org/childcare/resources/1459
  11. 11. Vermeer H, van IJzendoorn M, Cárcamo R, Harrison L. Quality of child care using the environment rating scales: A meta-analysis of international studies. Int J Early Child. 2016;48: 33–60.
  12. 12. Vandell DL, Wolfe B. Child care quality: Does it matter and does it need to be improved? [Internet]. Washington, DC: Department of Health and Human Services; 2000. https://www.purdue.edu/hhs/hdfs/fii/wp-content/uploads/2015/07/s_wifis17c01.pdf
  13. 13. NICHD Early Child Care Research Network. Child care structure--> process--> outcome: direct and indirect effects of child care quality on young children’s development. Psychol Sci. 2002;13: 199–206. pmid:12009038
  14. 14. American Academy of Pediatrics, Committee on Early Childhood, Adoption, and Dependent Care. Quality early education and child care from birth to kindergarten. Pediatrics. 2005;115: 187–191. pmid:15630001
  15. 15. McEntire N. The impact of teacher education on outcomes in center-based early childhood education programs: A meta-analysis. Child Educ. 2011;87: 374–375.
  16. 16. Kim S, Chang M, Kim H. Does teacher educational training help the early math skills of English language learners in Head Start? Child Youth Serv Rev. 2011;33: 732–740.
  17. 17. Mashburn A, Henry G. Assessing school readiness: validity and bias in preschool and kindergarten teachers’ ratings. Educ Meas Issues Pract. 2004;23: 16–30.
  18. 18. Cameron C, Morrison F. Teacher activity orienting predicts preschoolers academic and self regulatory skills. Early Educ Dev. 2011;22: 620–648.
  19. 19. Downer J, López M, Grimm K, Hamagami A, Pianta R, Howes C. Observations of teacher-child interactions in classrooms serving Latinos and dual language learners: applicability of the classroom assessment scoring system in diverse settings. Early Child Res Q. 2012;27: 21–32.
  20. 20. Hindman A, Skibbe L, Miller A, Zimmerman M. Ecological contexts and early learning: contributions of child, family, and classroom factors during head start, to literacy and mathematics growth through first grade. Early Child Res Q. 2010;25: 235–250.
  21. 21. Mashburn A, Downer J, Hamre B, Justice L, Pianta R. Consultation for teachers and children’s language and literacy development during pre-kindergarten. Appl Dev Sci. 2010;14: 179–196.
  22. 22. American Academy of Pediatrics, National Resource Center for Health, Safety in Child Care (US), American Public Health Association, United States. Maternal, & Child Health Bureau. Caring for our children: National health and safety performance standards: Guidelines for out-of-home child care. American Academy of Pediatrics; 2002.
  23. 23. National Association for the Education of Young Children. NAEYC standards for early childhood professional preparation: Initial licensure programs. [Internet]. 2001. http://www.naeyc.org/faculty/pdf/2001.pdf
  24. 24. UNICEF. The child care transition: A league table of early childhood education and care in economically advanced countries [Internet]. Florence, Italy: The United Nations Children’s Fund; 2008 pp. 1–36. http://www.unicef-irc.org/publications/pdf/rc8_eng.pdf
  25. 25. Tout K, Starr R, Soli M, Moodie S, Kirby G, Boller K. Compendium of quality rating systems and evaluations [Internet]. Mathematica Policy Research; 2010. http://www.acf.hhs.gov/sites/default/files/opre/qrs_compendium_final.pdf
  26. 26. Cost, Quality and Outcomes Study (CQO). Web site. In: Frank Porter Graham Child Development Institute [Internet]. [cited 8 Jul 2015]. http://fpg.unc.edu/search/apachesolr_search/costqualityoutcome
  27. 27. Early Childhood Longitudinal Program (ECLS). Web site. In: National Center for Education Statistics [Internet]. [cited 1 Jul 2015]. http://nces.ed.gov/ecls/
  28. 28. Effective Pre-School, Primary and Secondary Education (EPPSE). Web site. In: Institute of Education, University College London [Internet]. [cited 1 Jul 2015]. http://www.ioe.ac.uk/research/153.html
  29. 29. Head Start Impact Study (HS). Web site. In: Administration for Children and Families [Internet]. [cited 1 Jul 2015]. http://www.acf.hhs.gov/programs/opre/research/project/head-start-impact-study-and-follow-up
  30. 30. National Center for Early Development & Learning (NCEDL) multi-state study of pre-kindergarten. Web site. In: Frank Porter Graham Child Development Institute [Internet]. [cited 8 Jul 2015]. http://fcd-us.org/resources/pre-kindergarten-11-states-ncedls-multi-state-study-pre-kindergarten-and-study-statewide-e?destination=resources%2Fsearch%3Fpage%3D27
  31. 31. State-Wide Early Education Program Study (SWEEP). Web site. In: Frank Porter Graham Child Development Institute [Internet]. [cited 8 Jul 2015]. http://fpg.unc.edu/search/apachesolr_search/Sweep
  32. 32. Head Start Family And Child Experiences Survey (FACES) series. Web site. In: Inter-University Consortium for Political and Social Research [Internet]. [cited 1 Jul 2015]. https://www.icpsr.umich.edu/icpsrweb/ICPSR/series/236
  33. 33. Study of Early Child Care and Youth Development (NICHD). Web site. In: U.S. Department of Health and Human Services, National Institutes of Health [Internet]. 2015 [cited 8 Jul 2015]. http://www.nichd.nih.gov/research/supported/seccyd/Pages/datasets.aspx
  34. 34. Early D, Bryant D, Pianta R, Clifford R, Burchinal M, Ritchie S, et al. Are teachers’ education, major, and credentials related to classroom quality and children’s academic gains in pre-kindergarten? Early Child Res Q. 2006;21: 174–195.
  35. 35. Burchinal M, Howes C, Pianta R. Predicting child outcomes at the end of kindergarten from the quality of pre-kindergarten teacher-child interactions and instruction. Appl Dev Sci. 2008;12: 140–153.
  36. 36. Cappella E, Aber J, Kim H. Teaching beyond achievement tests: Perspectives from developmental and education science. In Gitomer D. H. & Bell C. A. (Eds.). Handbook of research on teaching. Washington, DC: American Educational Research Association; 2016. pp. 249–347.
  37. 37. Department of Health and Human Services, Administration for Children and Families, Office of Planning, Research and Evaluation. The head start leaders guide to positive child: strategies to support child positive outcomes. [Internet]. 2003. http://eclkc.ohs.acf.hhs.gov/hslc/hs/resources/ECLKC_Bookstore/PDFs/HeadStartGuidePositiveChildOutcomes.pdf.
  38. 38. Valentine J, Pigott T, Rothstein H. How many studies do you need? A primer on statistical power for meta-analysis. J Educ Behav Stat. 2010;35: 215–247.
  39. 39. Davey J, Turner R, Clarke M, Higgins J. Characteristics of meta-analyses and their component studies in the Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews: a cross-sectional, descriptive analysis. BMC Med Res Methodol. 2011;11: 160. pmid:22114982
  40. 40. Wood J. Methodology for dealing with duplicate study effects in a meta-analysis. Organ Res Methods. 2008;11: 79–95.
  41. 41. National Center on Child Care Quality Improvement. Use of ERS and other program assessment tools in QRIS (No.131) [Internet]. Administration for Children & Families Office of Child Care; 2013. https://occqrisguide.icfwebservices.com/files/QRIS_Program_Assess.pdf
  42. 42. Wells G, Shea B, O’Conell D, Peterson J, Welch V, Losos M. The Newcastle-Ottawa Scale (NOS) for assessing the quality of nonrandomized studies in meta-analyses [Internet]. Ottawa, ON; 2009. http://www.ohri.ca/programs/clinicalepidemiology/oxford.htm
  43. 43. Higgins J, Thompson S, Deeks J, Altman D. Measuring inconsistency in meta-analyses. Br Med J. 2003;327: 557–560. http://dx.doi.org/10.1136/bmj.327.7414.557
  44. 44. Comprehensive meta-analysis. Version 2.0. Engelwood, NJ: Biostat; 2005.
  45. 45. Moher D, Liberati A, Tetzlaff J, Altman D, The PRISMA Group. Preferred reporting items for systematic reviews and meta-analyses: the PRISMA statement. PLoS Med. 2009;6. pmid:19621072
  46. 46. Barnett W, Yarosz D, Thomas J, Jung K, Blanco D. Two-way and monolingual english immersion in preschool education: an experimental comparison. Early Child Res Q. 2007;22: 277–293.
  47. 47. Research Triangle Institute. A report on two national samples of Head Start classes: some aspects of child development participants in full year 1967–68 and 1968–69 programs [Internet]. Durham, NC: Research Triangle Institute; 1972. http://files.eric.ed.gov/fulltext/ED085407.pdf
  48. 48. Chang F, Crawford G, Early D, Bryant D, Howes C, Burchinal M, et al. Spanish-speaking children’s social and language development in pre-kindergarten classrooms. Early Educ Dev. 2007;18: 243–269.
  49. 49. Early D, Maxwell K, Burchinal M, Alva S, Bender R, Bryant D, et al. Teachers’ education, classroom quality, and young children’s academic skills: results from seven studies of preschool programs. Child Dev. 2007;78: 558–580. pmid:17381790
  50. 50. Aikens N, Tarullo L, Husley L, Ross C, West J, Xue Y. A year in head start: children, families and programs [Internet]. Washington, DC: U.S. Department of Health and Human Services, Administration for Children and Families, Office of Planning, Research and Evaluation; 2010. http://www.acf.hhs.gov/sites/default/files/opre/year_final.pdf
  51. 51. Hulsey L, Aikens N, Xue Y, Tarullo L, West J. ACF-OPRE Report: Data Tables for FACES 2006: A Year In Head Start Report [Internet]. Washington, DC: U.S. Department of Health and Human Services, Administration for Children and Families, Office of Planning, Research and Evaluation; 2010. http://www.acf.hhs.gov/sites/default/files/opre/year_data_tables.pdf
  52. 52. Aikens N, Moiduddin E, Xue Y, Tarullo L, West J. Data tables for child outcomes and classroom quality in FACES 2009 report [Internet]. Washington, DC: U.S. Department of Health and Human Services, Administration for Children and Families, Office of Planning, Research and Evaluation; 2012. http://www.acf.hhs.gov/sites/default/files/opre/data_tables_for_child_outcomes_and_classroom_quality_in_faces_2009.pdf
  53. 53. Moiduddin E, Aikens N, Tarullo L, West J, Xue Y. Child outcomes and classroom quality in FACES 2009 [Internet]. Washington, DC: Office of Planning, Research and Evaluation, Administration for Children and Families, U.S. Department of Health and Human Services; 2012. http://eric.ed.gov/?id=ED539265
  54. 54. Burchinal M, Nelson L. Family selection and child care experiences: implications for studies of child outcomes. Early Child Res Q. 2000;15: 385–411.
  55. 55. Burchinal M, Roberts J, Riggins R Jr, Zeisel S, Neebe E, Bryant D. Relating quality of center-based child care to early cognitive and language development longitudinally. Child Dev. 2000;71: 339–357. pmid:10834469
  56. 56. Choi J, Dobbs-Oates J. Childcare quality and preschoolers’ math development. Early Child Dev Care. 2014;184: 915–932.
  57. 57. Clarke-Stewart K, Gruber C, Fitzgerald L. Children at home and in day care. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc; 1994.
  58. 58. Colwell N, Gordon R, Fujimoto K, Kaestner R, Korenman S. New evidence on the validity of the Arnett Caregiver Interaction Scale: results from the early childhood longitudinal study-birth cohort. Early Child Res Q. 2013;28: 218–233. pmid:24058264
  59. 59. Dotterer A, Burchinal M, Bryant D, Early D, Pianta R. Universal and targeted pre-kindergarten programmes: a comparison of classroom characteristics and child outcomes. Early Child Dev Care. 2013;183: 931–950.
  60. 60. Dunn L. Proximal and distal features of day care quality and children’s development. Early Child Res Q. 1993;8: 167–192.
  61. 61. Epstein A. Training for quality improving early childhood programs through systematic inservice training. Ypsilanti, Michigan: High/ Scope Press; 1993.
  62. 62. Guo Y, Tompkins V, Justice L, Petscher Y. Classroom age composition and vocabulary development among at-risk preschoolers. Early Educ Dev. 2014;25: 1016–1034. pmid:27660399
  63. 63. Hamre B, Hatfield B, Pianta R, Jamil F. Evidence for general and domain-specific elements of teacher–child interactions: associations with preschool children’s development. Child Dev. 2014;85: 1257–1274. pmid:24255933
  64. 64. Henry G, Gordon C, Henderson L, Ponder B. Georgia pre-K longitudinal study: final report, 1996–2001 [Internet]. Atlanta, GA: Andrew Young School of Policy Studies, Georgia State University; 2003. http://files.eric.ed.gov/fulltext/ED481260.pdf
  65. 65. Henry G, Rickman D, Ponder B, Henderson L, Mashburn A, Gordon C. The Georgia early childhood study, 2001–2004, final report [Internet]. Atlanta, GA: Andrew Young School of Policy Studies, Georgia State University; 2005. http://citeseerx.ist.psu.edu/viewdoc/download?doi=10.1.1.177.2555&rep=rep1&type=pdf
  66. 66. Howes C, Burchinal M, Pianta R, Bryant D, Early D, Clifford R, et al. Ready to learn? children’s pre-academic achievement in pre-kindergarten programs. Early Child Res Q. 2008;23: 27–50.
  67. 67. Kaiser A, Cai X, Hancock T, Foster E. Teacher-reported behavior problems and language delays in boys and girls enrolled in head start. Counc Child Behav Disord. 2002;28: 23–39.
  68. 68. Lyon M, Canning P. Atlantic day care study. Halifax: Mount Saint Vincent University; 1995.
  69. 69. Mashburn AJ, Pianta RC, Hamre BK, Downer JT, Barbarin OA, Bryant D, et al. Measures of classroom quality in prekindergarten and children’s development of academic, language, and social skills. Child Dev. 2008;79: 732–749. pmid:18489424
  70. 70. Montie J, Xiang Z, Schweinhart L. Preschool experience in 10 countries: cognitive and language performance at age 7. Early Child Res Q. 2006;21: 313–331.
  71. 71. NICHD Early Child Care Research Network. Child outcomes when child care center classes meet recommended standards for quality. Am J Public Health. 1999;89: 1072–1077. pmid:10394318
  72. 72. Reid J, Ready D. High-quality preschool: the socioeconomic composition of preschool classrooms and children’s learning. Early Educ Dev. 2013;24: 1082–1111.
  73. 73. Sabol T, Hong S, Pianta R, Burchinal M. Can rating pre-k programs predict children’s learning? Science. 2013;341: 845–846. pmid:23970684
  74. 74. Son S, Kwon K, Jeon H, Hon S. Head Start classrooms and children’s school readiness benefit from teachers’ qualifications and ongoing training. Child Youth Care Forum. 2013;42: 525–553.
  75. 75. Travers J, Goodson B, Singer J, Connell D. Research Results of the National Day Care Study. Final Report of the National Day Care Study. Volume II [Internet]. Cambridge, MA: Abt Associates, Inc.; 1980. http://eric.ed.gov/?id=ED195336
  76. 76. West J, Malone L, Hulsey L, Aikens N, Tarullo L. Head start children go to kindergarten [Internet]. Washington, DC: U.S. Department of Health and Human Services, Administration for Children and Families, Office of Planning, Research and Evaluation; 2010. http://www.acf.hhs.gov/sites/default/files/opre/hs_kindergarten.pdf
  77. 77. Malone L, Hulsey L, Aikens N, West J, Tarullo L. ACF-OPRE report data tables for FACES 2006: head start children go to kindergarten report [Internet]. Washington, DC: U.S. Department of Health and Human Services, Administration for Children and Families, Office of Planning, Research and Evaluation; 2010 pp. 1–164. http://www.acf.hhs.gov/sites/default/files/opre/hs_kinder_data.pdf
  78. 78. Zellman G, Perlman M, Le V, Setodji C. Assessing the validity of the qualistar early learning quality rating and improvement system as a tool for improving child-care quality [Internet]. Santa Monica, CA: RAND Corporation; 2008. http://www.rand.org/content/dam/rand/pubs/monographs/2008/RAND_MG650.pdf
  79. 79. Zill N, Resnick G, Kim K, O’Donnell K, Sorongon A, McKey R, et al. Head start FACES 2000: a whole-child perspective on program performance [Internet]. Washington, DC: Administration for Children and Families, U.S. Department of Health and Human Services; 2003. http://www.acf.hhs.gov/sites/default/files/opre/faces00_4thprogress.pdf
  80. 80. Zill N, Resnick G, Kim K, O’Donnell K, Sorongon A, Ziv Y, et al. Head start performance measures center family and child experiences survey (FACES 2000): Technical report [Internet]. Washington, DC: Office of Planning, Research and Evaluation, Administration for Children and Families, U.S. Department of Health and Human Services; 2006. http://www.acf.hhs.gov/sites/default/files/opre/tech2k_final2.pdf
  81. 81. Mashburn A. Quality of social and physical environments in preschools and children’s development of academic, language and literacy skills. Appl Dev Sci. 2008;12: 103–127.
  82. 82. Gordon R, Fujimoto K, Kaestner R, Korenman S, Abner K. An assessment of the validity of the ECERS–R with implications for measures of child care quality and relations to child development. Dev Pscyhology. 2013;49: 146–160. pmid:22468567
  83. 83. Johnson C, Beitchman J, Brownlie E. Twenty-year follow-up of children with and without speech-language impairments: Family, educational, occupational, and quality of life outcomes. Am J Speech Lang Pathol. 2010;19: 51–65. pmid:19644128
  84. 84. Bornstein M, Hahn C, Suwalsky J. Language and internalizing and externalizing behavioral adjustment: Developmental pathways from childhood to adolescence. Dev Psychopathol. 2013;25: 857–878. pmid:23880396
  85. 85. Salmon K, O’Kearney R, Reese E, Fortune C. The role of language skill in child psychopathology: implications for intervention in the early years. Clin Child Fam Psychol Rev. 2016;19: 352–367. pmid:27678011
  86. 86. Bellanca J. Designing professional development for change: a guide for improving classroom instruction. Thousand Oaks CA: Corwin Press; 2009.
  87. 87. Heisner M, Lederberg A. The impact of Child Development Associate training in the beliefs and practices of teachers. Early Child Res Q. 2011;26: 227–236.
  88. 88. Melhuish E, Phan M, Sylva K, Sammons P, Siraj-Blatchford I, Taggart B. Effects of the home learning environment and preschool center experience upon literacy and numeracy development in early primary school. J Soc Issues. 2008;64: 95–114.
  89. 89. Pianta R, La Paro K, Hamre B. Classroom Assessment Scoring System (CLASS) Pre-k Version. Baltimore, MD: Brookes Publishing; 2007.
  90. 90. Perlman M, Falenchuk O, Fletcher B, McMullen E, Beyene J, Shah P. A systematic review and meta-analysis of a measure of staff/child interaction quality (the classroom assessment scoring system) in early childhood education and care settings and child outcomes. PLOS One. 2016;11: e0167660. pmid:28036333
  91. 91. Ellis B, Boyce W, Belsky J, Bakermans-Kranenburg M, Van Ijzendoorn M. Differential susceptibility to the environment: An evolutionary–neurodevelopmental theory. Dev Psychopathol. 2011;23: 7–28. pmid:21262036