Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

Effects of laughter therapy on quality of life in patients with cancer: An open-label, randomized controlled trial

  • Toshitaka Morishima ,

    Roles Data curation, Formal analysis, Funding acquisition, Investigation, Resources, Visualization, Writing – original draft

    morishima.t@oici.jp

    Affiliation Cancer Control Center, Osaka International Cancer Institute, Osaka, Japan

  • Isao Miyashiro,

    Roles Conceptualization, Data curation, Investigation, Project administration, Resources, Validation, Writing – original draft

    Affiliation Cancer Control Center, Osaka International Cancer Institute, Osaka, Japan

  • Norimitsu Inoue,

    Roles Data curation, Investigation, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliations Department of Tumor Immunology, Research Center, Osaka International Cancer Institute, Osaka, Japan, Department of Molecular Genetics, Wakayama Medical University, Wakayama, Japan

  • Mitsuko Kitasaka,

    Roles Data curation, Investigation, Resources, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Department of Nursing Services, Osaka International Cancer Institute, Osaka, Japan

  • Takashi Akazawa,

    Roles Investigation, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Department of Cancer Drug Discovery and Development, Research Center, Osaka International Cancer Institute, Osaka, Japan

  • Akemi Higeno,

    Roles Data curation, Investigation, Resources, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Department of Clinical Laboratory, Osaka International Cancer Institute, Osaka, Japan

  • Atsushi Idota,

    Roles Data curation, Investigation, Resources, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Department of Clinical Laboratory, Osaka International Cancer Institute, Osaka, Japan

  • Akira Sato,

    Roles Data curation, Validation, Writing – original draft

    Affiliation Cancer Control Center, Osaka International Cancer Institute, Osaka, Japan

  • Tetsuya Ohira,

    Roles Funding acquisition, Supervision, Validation, Writing – original draft

    Affiliation Department of Epidemiology, Fukushima Medical University, Fukushima, Japan

  • Masato Sakon,

    Roles Conceptualization, Project administration, Supervision, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Osaka International Cancer Institute, Osaka, Japan

  • Nariaki Matsuura

    Roles Project administration, Supervision, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Osaka International Cancer Institute, Osaka, Japan

Abstract

Background

Few randomized controlled trials have assessed the effects of laughter therapy on health-related quality of life (QOL) in cancer patients. This study aimed to evaluate these effects as an exploratory endpoint in cancer patients as part of a randomized controlled trial conducted at a single institution in Japan.

Methods

The Initiative On Smile And CAncer (iOSACA) study was an open-label randomized controlled trial conducted in 2017 in which participants aged 40–64 years with cancer were randomly assigned to either an intervention group (laughter therapy) or control group (no laughter therapy). Each participant in the intervention group underwent a laughter therapy session once every two weeks for seven weeks (total of four sessions). Each session involved a laughter yoga routine followed by Rakugo or Manzai traditional Japanese verbal comedy performances. We assessed QOL as a secondary endpoint in this intention-to-treat population using the European Organisation for Research and Treatment of Cancer Quality of Life Questionnaire Core 30 (EORTC QLQ-C30). The questionnaire was completed at baseline (Week 0) and at Weeks 3 and 7. Mixed-effects models for repeated measures were developed to compare time-dependent changes in each QOL domain from baseline between the intervention and control groups.

Results

Four participants retracted consent and one participant was retrospectively excluded from analysis due to unmet inclusion criteria. The analysis was conducted using 56 participants, with 26 in the intervention group and 30 in the control group. Questionnaire completion rates were high (>90%), with similar QOL scores reported at baseline in both groups. The mixed-effects models showed that the intervention group had significantly better cognitive function and less pain than the control group for a short period.

Conclusion

Laughter therapy may represent a beneficial, noninvasive complementary intervention in the clinical setting. Further studies are needed to verify the hypotheses generated from this exploratory study.

Introduction

Over the past 20 years, widespread screening and advances in treatment options have allowed individuals to live longer after cancer diagnosis [1]. This has led to a continuously growing population of cancer survivors living with various sequelae [2]. The impact of cancer on health-related quality of life (QOL) during the acute diagnostic and treatment phases is well documented [3]. Moreover, attention has also been directed toward cancer’s longer-term physical and psycho-social effects. Cancer survivors are more likely to suffer from secondary health problems such as impaired cognitive function, fatigue, pain, anxiety, and depression [46]. This may have an impact on their QOL, which is generally lower than that of the general population [7].

Effective interventions are therefore needed to reduce QOL impairment in cancer patients. In addition to conventional treatments, complementary and alternative therapies have been developed to improve QOL and manage symptoms [8]. Laughter therapy (which often includes laughter yoga, comedy performances, clown performances, and jokes) has been applied as a complementary intervention since the 1970s [9]. Laughter therapy can benefit health through various mechanisms, including muscular exercise, increased respiration and blood circulation, improvements to digestion, and emotional catharsis [10]. Researchers have since investigated the therapeutic efficacy of this therapy, and reported that it can have positive and quantifiable effects on a variety of medical conditions, such as depression, anxiety, stress, dementia, and pain without deleterious effects on health [1124]. Furthermore, researchers have also reported that laughter therapy can reduce the levels of depression, anxiety, and stress in cancer patients [2529]. However, the causal effects of laughter therapy in cancer patients remain unclear because the majority of previous studies were conducted without comparisons or randomized treatment allocation. Several randomized controlled clinical trials have been conducted to examine the therapeutic efficacy of laughter therapy in patients with breast cancer, gastrointestinal cancer, and leukemia [3034], but these have generally focused on psychological outcomes rather than physiological ones.

The goal of active cancer therapy is to improve survival and QOL in patients [35]. QOL reflects the impact of long-term physical and psychological sequelae in cancer survivors. The American Cancer Society, which defines cancer survivorship as beginning at cancer diagnosis and continuing for the balance of life, views QOL as a key outcome of survivorship [36]. When examining the effects of integrative, alternative, and complementary therapies on cancer patients, QOL is recommended in the US National Cancer Institute’s Physician Data Query cancer information summaries as a scientifically strong endpoint for clinical trials [37]. Nevertheless, few randomized controlled studies have investigated the effects of laughter therapy on QOL [34]. To address this issue, we examined, as part of a randomized controlled trial, whether laughter therapy would improve QOL in cancer survivors. As this study was exploratory, the analysis was conducted without a specific a priori hypothesis.

Methods

Study design and participants

The Initiative On Smile And CAncer (iOSACA) study was a single-center, open-label, cross-over, randomized controlled trial designed to evaluate the effects of laughter therapy in cancer patients. The iOSACA study also included a prospective single-arm trial to evaluate the feasibility of four-month-long laughter therapy in cancer patients, and a randomized clinical trial to evaluate the psychological effects on work stress in hospital staff; these aspects of the study will be reported elsewhere. Patients receiving active treatment or follow-up for cancer were recruited from outpatient clinics at Osaka International Cancer Institute (OICI), Osaka, Japan in April 2017. The eligibility criteria for participants included having histologic and/or cytologic evidence of any malignancy; being aged 40 to 64 years; having an Eastern Cooperative Oncology Group (ECOG) performance status (PS) of 0 (indicating a fully active patient), 1 (indicating an ambulatory patient with restrictions on strenuous activities), or 2 (indicating an ambulatory patient capable of self-care but unable to work); and having an anticipated survival time of at least one year. Patients were excluded if they did not understand Japanese or had plans to undergo cancer surgery.

Prospective participants were informed of the risks and benefits of study participation, and provided written informed consent before enrollment. Participants were randomly assigned to two groups (Group A and Group B) in a 1:1 ratio according to a computer-generated sequence. Randomization with minimization was performed with stratifications for sex, cancer site (breast, gastrointestinal, lung, urological or gynecological, and others), and whether a patient was receiving chemotherapy or radiotherapy at the time of enrollment [38]. This study was approved by the institutional review board of OICI (Approval number: 1702246289) and registered in the UMIN Clinical Trial Registry (UMIN000026831) on April 3, 2017 (https://upload.umin.ac.jp/cgi-open-bin/ctr/ctr_view.cgi?recptno=R000030790). The CONSORT checklist and protocol for this trial are available as supporting information (S1S3 Files).

Intervention

Table 1 outlines the schedule of the intervention and assessments. This study consisted of two phases: a seven-week initial phase (including the baseline measurement) and a six-week cross-over phase. During the initial phase, Group A was designated the intervention group and the participants underwent laughter therapy in Weeks 1, 3, 5, and 7. Participants in Group B, which served as the control group during this phase, received no laughter therapy. The initial phase was followed by the cross-over phase from Weeks 9 through 15, during which participants in Group B crossed over to receive laughter therapy in Weeks 9, 11, 13, and 15. Group A served as the control group during the cross-over phase. The initial phase started in May 2017, and the cross-over phase ended in August 2017. The primary interest of this analysis was the initial phase, in which we investigated the effects of laughter therapy that Group A (intervention) participants received when compared with Group B (control) participants. The cross-over phase was designed to enable further analyses to determine if the effects of laughter therapy were maintained after treatment cessation and to detect any differences between carry-over effects in Group A and current effects in Group B.

Participants assigned to the intervention group underwent a one-hour laughter therapy session every two weeks over the course of seven weeks, with each participant receiving a total of four sessions. Each session was performed at OICI, and began with a laughter yoga routine (a group practice involving voluntary laughter, with a body exercise which includes stretching, clapping, and body movement). This was followed by live performances of Rakugo (a form of Japanese verbal comedy performed by a lone storyteller sitting on stage) or Manzai (a traditional Japanese style of stand-up comedy involving jokes traded at high speed between two performers) by locally well-known professional entertainers.

QOL assessments

This study was conducted to investigate the effects of laughter therapy on QOL as a secondary endpoint of the iOSACA study. The primary endpoint was self-efficacy, which is not addressed in this paper. QOL was measured using the European Organisation for Research and Treatment of Cancer Quality of Life Questionnaire Core 30 version 3.0 (EORTC QLQ-C30), a validated questionnaire designed to assess both disease-related functions and symptoms in addition to their impact on everyday life [39]. The time frame for each assessment was the immediately preceding week, which may be of particular relevance to clinical trials.

The QLQ-C30 is a 30-item, cancer-specific, multi-dimensional, self-administered questionnaire that has been validated in cross-cultural settings [40]. It comprises a two-item global health status domain; five multi-item functional domains (physical functioning, role functioning, emotional functioning, cognitive functioning, and social functioning); three multi-item symptom domains (fatigue, pain, and nausea and vomiting); and six single-item domains for the assessment of additional symptoms commonly reported by cancer patients (dyspnea, appetite loss, sleep disturbance, constipation, and diarrhea) and the perceived financial impact of the disease and treatment. Most items are answered based on a four-point Likert scale ranging from 1 (not at all) to 4 (very much). Responses to the two items assessing global health status range from 1 (very poor) to 7 (excellent). Items were scaled and scored according to the EORTC Scoring Manual [41]. Raw scores were transformed to a linear scale ranging from 0 to 100 [41]. For scores measuring global health status and functional domains, a higher score represents a ‘better’ level of status or functioning; in contrast, a higher score for symptom domains represents a ‘worse’ level of symptoms.

Questionnaires were completed on Week 0 (baseline, one week before the start of the intervention) and every four weeks during both the initial and cross-over phases. Participants undergoing the intervention self-administered the questionnaire immediately after a laughter therapy session at OICI, whereas control participants self-administered the questionnaire when visiting OICI during the same scheduled time points.

Statistical analyses

We calculated that 30 participants would be required for each group based on a sample size estimation for the iOSACA study’s primary endpoint (i.e., self-efficacy). Questionnaire completion rates for each group were calculated as the percentage of all subjects who completed a questionnaire at each scheduled time point. Primary analyses were performed using three assessments of the QOL domains obtained at baseline and during the initial phase. Statistical analyses of all 15 QOL domain scores were used to evaluate the differences between the groups with respect to changes from the baseline scores to those obtained at each assessment point as follows: changes from baseline to Week 3 in Group A versus Group B and changes from baseline to Week 7 in Group A versus Group B. Between-group differences in these time-dependent changes from baseline and their 95% confidence intervals (CIs) were analyzed using mixed-effects models for repeated measures in which the covariates were laughter therapy during each phase (receiving laughter therapy or not), time point, therapy phase (receiving laughter therapy in the initial or cross-over phase), therapy-by-time point interactions, and the three predefined stratification factors (sex, cancer site, and receiving chemotherapy or radiotherapy at the time of enrollment) as fixed effects [4245]. Unstructured covariance matrices were used for these analyses. No correlation structures of residual error terms were specified.

A secondary analysis of within-group differences was performed for each QOL domain to examine if the effects of laughter therapy were maintained after the intervention was stopped. For these analyses, changes from the end of the initial phase (i.e., the score of Week 7) to the cross-over phase (i.e., the score of Week 11 and the score of Week 15) in Group A were examined. Another secondary analysis was conducted to detect between-group differences in each QOL domain for the carry-over effects and current effects of laughter therapy. For these analyses, the between-group comparisons of changes from baseline to each assessment point during the cross-over phase were conducted as follows: changes from baseline to Week 11 in Group A versus Group B and changes from baseline to Week 15 in Group A versus Group B. All comparisons between the groups were based on intention-to-treat analyses in which participants were analyzed according to their assigned group. These analyses were not pre-specified in the trial protocol. All statistical analyses were performed using SAS version 9.4 (SAS Institute, Cary, NC, USA). All statistical tests were two-sided, and P values below 0.05 were considered statistically significant.

Results

Participant characteristics

A flow diagram of this clinical trial is presented in Fig 1. Sixty-one patients provided consent to be enrolled in the trial. Thirty participants were randomized to Group A and the remaining 31 participants formed Group B. However, four participants withdrew their consent. One participant was retrospectively excluded from the analyses because (s)he was found not to have met the age inclusion criterion through data curation after the trial. In total, 56 participants were enrolled (26 allocated to Group A and 30 to Group B) and included in the analyses.

The demographic and medical characteristics of the 56 participants are summarized in Table 2. These characteristics were generally well balanced between the two groups. The participants comprised a higher proportion of women (75%) than men. Breast cancer (48%) was the most common cancer site. Each group was allocated a similar proportion of participants who were receiving chemotherapy or radiotherapy at study enrollment (approximately 34%). None of the participants had an ECOG PS of 2. Group A had a lower proportion of participants with an ECOG PS of 0 than Group B (50% versus 63%, respectively).

Laughter therapy and QOL questionnaire completion

Compliance with the laughter therapy sessions and QOL assessments is presented in Table 3. Among the participants in Group A, 25 participated in all sessions from Weeks 1 through 7, with one participant missing a session in Week 3. Among the participants in Group B, 27 participated in all sessions from Weeks 9 through 15, with one participant missing each post-baseline session.

thumbnail
Table 3. Completion of laughter therapy sessions and the EORTC QLQ-C30 assessment.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0219065.t003

All participants in both groups completed the baseline QLQ-C30 assessment, and most participants (84%) completed all post-baseline questionnaires. Completion rates, as expressed by the percentage of eligible participants who were expected to complete the QOL questionnaires at each specific time point, ranged between 90% and 100%.

Baseline QOL scores

The two groups had similar baseline QOL scores in all domains (Table 4). The greatest impairment was seen in global health status (mean scores of 72.1 and 77.5 in Group A and Group B, respectively), cognitive functioning (75.0 and 80.6, respectively) and emotional functioning (77.6 and 80.3, respectively). The symptoms with the highest mean baseline scores were fatigue (mean scores of 35.9 and 28.5 in Group A and Group B, respectively), insomnia (21.8 and 22.2, respectively), financial difficulties (19.2 and 15.6, respectively), and dyspnea (21.8 and 10.0, respectively).

thumbnail
Table 4. Baseline quality of life in the study participants by group.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0219065.t004

Primary QOL analyses

Fig 2 illustrates the time-dependent changes in the mean scores for each QLQ-C30 domain from baseline. Primary QOL analyses were performed for three assessments obtained from baseline up to Week 7 to compare these changes between the intervention group (i.e., Group A) and the control group (i.e., Group B) in the initial phase. In Group A, the global health status score had worsened by 0.3 points at Week 3 and improved by 1.3 points at Week 7 relative to the baseline score. In Group B, this score had worsened by 2.1 points at Week 3 and by 1.1 point at Week 7 relative to the baseline score. These scores did not demonstrate statistically significant between-group differences at any week (95% CI: -7.6 to 11.1 points for Week 3 and -6.9 to 11.6 points for Week 7). For functional domains, the cognitive functioning score improved by 5.8 points from baseline to Week 7 in Group A, but worsened by 5.3 points in Group B. This resulted in a significant between-group difference that favored Group A over Group B (95% CI: 3.2 to 19.0 points; P = 0.006). There were no significant between-group differences in the other functional domains.

thumbnail
Fig 2. Time-dependent changes in 15 quality of life domains.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0219065.g002

For symptom domains, the pain score improved by 3.9 points from baseline to Week 3 in Group A, but worsened by 4.5 points in Group B. This resulted in a significant between-group difference that favored Group A over Group B (95% CI: -16.4 to -0.5 points; P = 0.037). Although a similar between-group difference of 5.1 points in the pain score was observed at Week 7, the difference did not reach statistical significance (95% CI: -12.9 to 2.7 points; P = 0.20). The nausea and vomiting score was generally stable over time in Group A, but worsened in Group B. Although this finding appeared to favor Group A, the observed between-group differences were not statistically significant (95% CI: -6.4 to 4.9 points for Week 3 and -7.8 to 3.4 points for Week 7). Neither group showed improvements or statistically significant between-group differences for the other symptom domains.

Secondary QOL analyses

Fig 2 also illustrates the mean score changes in the QOL domains from the initial phase to the cross-over phase, which indicate the carry-over effects after the cessation of laughter therapy in Group A. For the global health status and functional domains, the cognitive functioning score did not change significantly from Week 7 to Week 11 (-3.7 points; 95% CI: -10.6 to 3.2 points; P = 0.29) or from Week 7 to Week 15 (1.0 point; 95% CI: -5.8 to 7.7 points; P = 0.78). Also, pain score did not change significantly from Week 7 to Week 11 (4.2 points; 95% CI: -1.7 to 10.0 points; P = 0.160) or from Week 7 to Week 15 (3.8 points; 95% CI: -1.8 to 9.5 points; P = 0.183). However, one of the symptom domains showed deterioration in Group A after the cessation of laughter therapy. The appetite loss score increased significantly from Week 7 to Week 11 by 8.2 points (95% CI: 0.5 to 15.9 points; P = 0.037). There were no significant changes in mean scores for the other domains from the initial phase to the cross-over phase.

Participants who did not receive laughter therapy in the initial phase (i.e., Group B) were given the opportunity to do so in the cross-over phase, whereas participants who received laughter therapy in the initial phase (i.e., Group A) ceased to receive this therapy in the cross-over phase. This design enabled us to compare the carry-over effects and current effects of laughter therapy. The results showed no significant between-group differences in the changes in any QOL domain score from baseline to Weeks 11 or 15.

Across the two phases, there were no significant between-group differences that favored the current control group over the current intervention group.

Discussion

To the best of our knowledge, this is the first randomized controlled trial that evaluates the effects of laughter therapy on QOL in adult cancer patients. Our analyses detected differences in specific measures of QOL between the intervention and control groups during the initial phase using the well-established EORTC QLQ-C30.

The results showed no significant differences in global health status between the two groups at Weeks 3 or 7. However, significant between-group differences were found in two domains, although it should be noted that multiple testing increases the risk of Type I errors. First, the intervention group showed statistically significant improvements in cognitive functioning relative to baseline during the initial phase when compared with the control group. Second, participants receiving laughter therapy also had consistently greater mean benefits for pain within 3 to 7 weeks after baseline when compared with the control group. Notably, participants in the control group experienced worsening in these domains during the same period, although this may simply be due to regression to the mean. These findings suggest that laughter therapy may have physical and psychological effects in cancer patients. In addition, no negative impact of laughter therapy was detected by the QLQ-C30 scale, and the completion rates of the questionnaires were high in both groups. Laughter therapy therefore represents a potential non-pharmacological, complementary treatment for cancer patients with little or no expected harmful effects. However, these results must be considered exploratory since they were formed with no a priori hypothesis as the iOSACA study’s secondary endpoint. Furthermore, no adjustments were made for multiple statistical tests assessing a large number of domain changes.

With regard to the mechanism of cognitive function improvement, the positive emotions induced or accompanied by laughter may have enabled patients to reduce the stress response and ease tension by decreasing stress-making hormones such as cortisol, epinephrine, and growth hormone; this in turn can have a positive effect on the cognitive functioning of patients [18]. For the alleviation of pain, previous studies have reported that laughter therapy increases pain tolerance and reduces pain perception through physiological mechanisms for analgesia involving the release of endorphins [2124, 46]. Our findings from the primary QOL analyses using results obtained during the four laughter sessions are supported by an earlier study demonstrating that even a single one-hour session of laughter therapy was able to reduce pain after a week [23].

It is interesting that participants originally randomized to laughter therapy and had experienced less appetite loss scores during the initial phase showed subsequently poorer scores in these domains at Week 11 after crossing over to the control group. The phenomenon where Group A participants showed a return to baseline QOL scores after treatment cessation suggests that the improved or stable QOL statuses may be transient, particularly in the symptom domains. This indicates that the effects of laughter therapy may be short term, although patients who continue to undergo therapy could demonstrate better preservation or slower degradation of QOL. In contrast, it is also possible that the carry-over effects are not short term. This is because no between-group differences were observed in any of the domains during the cross-over phase, where current effects and carry-over effects of laughter therapy would be observed. Notably, the slopes of Groups A and B in the score changes for emotional functioning, role functioning, and cognitive functioning between Weeks 11 and 15 were approximately parallel. A possible explanation for these findings is that the present trial design did not provide a break period after the initial phase. Also, carry-over effects may be more likely to occur in functional domains than symptom domains. We also suggest that patients may not benefit from an earlier intervention with laughter therapy soon after cancer diagnosis.

Our analysis found only two statistically significant between-group differences in the functional and symptom domains, and no significant differences in global health status. These may be explained by several reasons. First, the participants enrolled in this trial exhibited relatively good QOL status and stable disease at baseline. Such patients are likely to have ceiling effects in which improvements in QOL are less common. Second, the EORTC QLQ-C30 may not be sensitive enough to detect changes in QOL over time in patients with stable disease. For example, this questionnaire contains items related to side effects of cancer treatment (e.g., nausea and vomiting) that would have little relevance to a relatively healthy cohort of cancer survivors. As previously noted, the QLQ-C30 may not be appropriate for survivors as it was designed to capture the immediate effects of cancer treatment rather than issues related to re-integration and the long-term sequelae of treatment [47]. Third, the uneven participant characteristics between the two groups in this study should be examined in further detail. More participants in Group A had poorer baseline physical status than in Group B. This is because the study participants were not stratified with respect to PS, which may have resulted in imbalances within this small subset of participants. However, it is difficult to judge which group the imbalances favored.

Our study has several limitations that should be noted. First, as in any trial assessing patient-reported outcomes such as QOL, there may be a response shift where patients revise their internal standards or understanding of the outcomes after undergoing changes in health states. Such response shifts can affect or distort QOL outcome measurements. Second, there may be a recall bias in favor of laughter therapy. The QLQ-C30 requests patients to base their evaluations on QOL statuses in the preceding week. Because recall is influenced most strongly by a person’s current status [48], study participants may tend toward under-reporting the severity of past problems. This tendency to emphasize the current status over the past is of particular concern when evaluating outcomes immediately after an intervention. Third, there may be a social desirability bias due to the open-label nature of this study. The bias can take the form of participants tending to answer questions in a way that will be viewed favorably by others. Fourth, we were unable to determine if the effects of laughter therapy were attributable to laughter yoga or the Rakugo and Manzai performances because the study was not designed to distinguish between the effects of each type of therapy. Fifth, we did not quantitatively measure laughing during the therapy sessions. The measurement of participants’ laughter should be included in further studies for a more detailed analysis of the effects of laughter therapy. Sixth, although laughter therapy appeared to have positive effects, it should be recognized that these results may have been influenced by regression to the mean. Finally, the generalizability of the findings may be limited due to self-selection bias as many participants may have been more amenable to laughing than the overall population of cancer patients. We cannot conclude that laughter therapy would have positive effects on all cancer patients as there is scarce evidence to show these effects on individuals with a more negative disposition. Furthermore, there may be a selection bias with regard to sex distribution as most of the participants were women. The uneven sex distribution may be because men (who have a much higher employment rate than women in Japan) had more difficulty participating in the laughter therapy sessions, which were performed on weekday afternoons. This may therefore affect the generalizability of our findings to men. A previous study reported that women tend to have a greater response to laughter therapy than men [20], which may be because women have a higher tendency to laugh [49].

Conclusions

This exploratory study provided some evidence that laughter therapy may improve specific domains of QOL and symptoms in cancer survivors. As laughter therapy has few, if any, harmful side effects, we propose that it can be implemented as a complementary therapy for cancer patients even if the beneficial effects are subtle. Additional analyses on the economic costs (including the opportunity costs of other activities) of this therapy will provide further insight into its value. However, QOL constitutes one of the secondary endpoints of this trial, and further studies are needed to verify the hypotheses generated from this exploratory study.

Supporting information

S3 File. Study protocol (English translation).

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0219065.s003

(DOCX)

S4 File. Relevant data underlying the findings described in manuscript.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0219065.s004

(CSV)

S5 File. Laughter therapy session protocol.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0219065.s005

(PDF)

Acknowledgments

We would like to express our gratitude to Hiroshi Arikawa for his coordination of the laughter therapy sessions, and to Takeshi Ueda, Tomoko Hatanaka, Isao Kadowaki, Hiroshi Yamazaki, Mitsuyo Matsuda, Nami Nishio, and Kanako Yoshimura at OICI for their clerical assistance.

References

  1. 1. Allemani C, Matsuda T, Di Carlo V, Harewood R, Matz M, Nikšić M, et al. Global surveillance of trends in cancer survival 2000–14 (CONCORD-3): analysis of individual records for 37 513 025 patients diagnosed with one of 18 cancers from 322 population-based registries in 71 countries. Lancet. 2018;391:1023–1075. pmid:29395269
  2. 2. Miller KD, Siegel RL, Lin CC, Mariotto AB, Kramer JL, Rowland JH, et al. Cancer treatment and survivorship statistics, 2016. CA Cancer J Clin. 2016;66:271–289. pmid:27253694
  3. 3. Stark D, Kiely M, Smith A, Velikova G, House A, Selby P. Anxiety disorders in cancer patients: their nature, associations, and relation to quality of life. J Clin Oncol. 2002;20:3137–3148. pmid:12118028
  4. 4. Tannock IF, Ahles TA, Ganz PA, Van Dam FS. Cognitive impairment associated with chemotherapy for cancer: report of a workshop. J Clin Oncol. 2004;22:2233–2239. pmid:15169812
  5. 5. Loge JH, Abrahamsen AF, Ekeberg , Kaasa S. Fatigue and psychiatric morbidity among Hodgkin’s disease survivors. J Pain Symptom Manage. 2000;19:91–99. pmid:10699536
  6. 6. Findley PA, Sambamoorthi U. Preventive health services and lifestyle practices in cancer survivors: a population health investigation. J Cancer Surviv. 2009;3:43–58. pmid:19067178
  7. 7. Jansen L, Koch L, Brenner H, Arndt V. Quality of life among long-term (≥5 years) colorectal cancer survivors—systematic review. Eur J Cancer. 2010;46:2879–2888. pmid:20605090
  8. 8. Ernst E. The role of complementary and alternative medicine in cancer. Lancet Oncol. 2000;1:176–180. pmid:11905656
  9. 9. Cousins N. Anatomy of an illness (as perceived by the patient). N Engl J Med. 1976;295:1458–1463. pmid:995143
  10. 10. Goldstein JH. A laugh a day: Can mirth keep disease at bay? Sciences (New York). 1982;22:21–25.
  11. 11. Shahidi M, Mojtahed A, Modabbernia A, Mojtahed M, Shafiabady A, Delavar A, et al. Laughter yoga versus group exercise program in elderly depressed women: a randomized controlled trial. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2011;26:322–327. pmid:20848578
  12. 12. Walter M, Hänni B, Haug M, Amrhein I, Krebs-Roubicek E, Müller-Spahn F, et al. Humour therapy in patients with late-life depression or Alzheimer’s disease: a pilot study. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry. 2007;22:77–83. pmid:16977676
  13. 13. Ghodsbin F, Sharif Ahmadi Z, Jahanbin I, Sharif F. The effects of laughter therapy on general health of elderly people referring to jahandidegan community center in shiraz, iran, 2014: a randomized controlled trial. Int J Community Based Nurs Midwifery. 2015;3:31–38. pmid:25553332
  14. 14. Dolgoff-Kaspar R, Baldwin A, Johnson MS, Edling N, Sethi GK. Effect of laughter yoga on mood and heart rate variability in patients awaiting organ transplantation: a pilot study. Altern Ther Health Med. 2012;18:61–66. pmid:22894892
  15. 15. Bukutu C, Le C, Vohra S. Asthma: a review of complementary and alternative therapies. Pediatr Rev. 2008;29:e44–e49. pmid:18676576
  16. 16. Bennett MP, Zeller JM, Rosenberg L, McCann J. The effect of mirthful laughter on stress and natural killer cell activity. Altern Ther Health Med. 2003;9:38–45.
  17. 17. Takeda M, Hashimoto R, Kudo T, Okochi M, Tagami S, Morihara T, et al. Laughter and humor as complementary and alternative medicines for dementia patients. BMC Complement Altern Med. 2010;10:28. pmid:20565815
  18. 18. Berk LS, Tan SA, Fry WF, Napier BJ, Lee JW, Hubbard RW, et al. Neuroendocrine and stress hormone changes during mirthful laughter. Am J Med Sci. 1989;298:390–396. pmid:2556917
  19. 19. Hirosaki M, Ohira T, Kajiura M, Kiyama M, Kitamura A, Sato S, et al. Effects of a laughter and exercise program on physiological and psychological health among community-dwelling elderly in Japan: randomized controlled trial. Geriatr Gerontol Int. 2013;13:152–160. pmid:22672359
  20. 20. Weisenberg M, Raz T, Hener T. The influence of film-induced mood on pain perception. Pain. 1998;76:365–375. pmid:9718255
  21. 21. Weisenberg M, Tepper I, Schwarzwald J. Humor as a cognitive technique for increasing pain tolerance. Pain. 1995;63:207–212. pmid:8628586
  22. 22. Zweyer K, Velker B, Ruch W. Do cheerfulness, exhilaration, and humor production moderate pain tolerance? A FACS study. Humor. 2004;17:85–119.
  23. 23. Yoshino S, Fujimori J, Kohda M. Effects of mirthful laughter on neuroendocrine and immune systems in patients with rheumatoid arthritis. J Rheumatol. 1996;23:793–794. pmid:8730156
  24. 24. Dunbar RI, Baron R, Frangou A, Pearce E, van Leeuwen EJ, Stow J, et al. Social laughter is correlated with an elevated pain threshold. Proc Biol Sci. 2012;279:1161–1167. pmid:21920973
  25. 25. Christie W, Moore C. The impact of humor on patients with cancer. Clin J Oncol Nurs. 2005;9:211–218. pmid:15853164
  26. 26. Penson RT, Partridge RA, Rudd P, Seiden MV, Nelson JE, Chabner BA, et al. Laughter: the best medicine? Oncologist. 2005;10:651–660. pmid:16177290
  27. 27. Borod M. SMILES—toward a better laughter life: a model for introducing humor in the palliative care setting. J Cancer Educ. 2006;21:30–34. pmid:16918287
  28. 28. Lengacher CA, Bennett MP, Kip KE, Keller R, LaVance MS, Smith LS, et al. Frequency of use of complementary and alternative medicine in women with breast cancer. Oncol Nurs Forum. 2002;29:1445–1452. pmid:12432415
  29. 29. Kong M, Shin SH, Lee E, Yun EK. The effect of laughter therapy on radiation dermatitis in patients with breast cancer: a single-blind prospective pilot study. Onco Targets Ther. 2014;7:2053–2059. pmid:25395864
  30. 30. Kim SH, Kim YH, Kim HJ. Laughter and stress relief in cancer patients: a pilot study. Evid Based Complement Alternat Med. 2015;2015:864739. pmid:26064177
  31. 31. Kim SH, Kook JR, Kwon M, Son MH, Ahn SD, Kim YH. The effects of laughter therapy on mood state and self-esteem in cancer patients undergoing radiation therapy: a randomized controlled trial. J Altern Complement Med. 2015;21:217–222. pmid:25875938
  32. 32. Farifteh S, Mohammadi-Aria A, Kiamanesh A, Mofid B. The impact of laughter yoga on the stress of cancer patients before chemotherapy. Iran J Cancer Prev. 2014;7:179–183. pmid:25628838
  33. 33. Phipps S, Barrera M, Vannatta K, Xiong X, Doyle JJ, Alderfer MA. Complementary therapies for children undergoing stem cell transplantation: report of a multisite trial. Cancer. 2010;116:3924–3933. pmid:20626016
  34. 34. Phipps S, Peasant C, Barrera M, Alderfer MA, Huang Q, Vannatta K. Resilience in children undergoing stem cell transplantation: results of a complementary intervention trial. Pediatrics. 2012;129:e762–e770. pmid:22311995
  35. 35. Peppercorn JM, Smith TJ, Helft PR, Debono DJ, Berry SR, Wollins DS, et al. American society of clinical oncology statement: toward individualized care for patients with advanced cancer. J Clin Oncol. 2011;29:755–760. pmid:21263086
  36. 36. Lipscomb J, Gotay CC, Snyder CF. Patient-reported outcomes in cancer: a review of recent research and policy initiatives. CA Cancer J Clin. 2007;57:278–300. pmid:17855485
  37. 37. Physician Data Query. Levels of evidence for human studies of integrative, alternative, and complementary therapies. Apr 15, 2016 [cited Apr 12, 2019]. In: PDQ Cancer Information Summaries. Bethesda (MD): National Cancer Institute. https://www.cancer.gov/publications/pdq/levels-evidence/cam#section/_21.
  38. 38. Altman DG, Bland JM. Treatment allocation by minimisation. BMJ. 2005;330:843. pmid:15817555
  39. 39. Aaronson NK, Ahmedzai S, Bergman B, Bullinger M, Cull A, Duez NJ, et al. The European Organization for Research and Treatment of Cancer QLQ-C30: a quality-of-life instrument for use in international clinical trials in oncology. J Natl Cancer Inst. 1993;85:365–376. pmid:8433390
  40. 40. Kobayashi K, Takeda F, Teramukai S, Gotoh I, Sakai H, Yoneda S, et al. A cross-validation of the European Organization for Research and Treatment of Cancer QLQ-C30 (EORTC QLQ-C30) for Japanese with lung cancer. Eur J Cancer. 1998;34:810–815. pmid:9797690
  41. 41. Fayers PM, Bjordal K, Groenvold M, Curran D, Bottomley A, on behalf of the EORTC Quality of Life Group. The EORTC QLQ-C30 Scoring Manual (3rd Edition). 2001 [cited Apr 12, 2019]. In: EORTC Quality of Life. Brussels (Belgium): European Organisation for Research and Treatment of Cancer. http://qol.eortc.org/manuals/.
  42. 42. Scott NW, McPherson GC, Ramsay CR, Campbell MK. The method of minimization for allocation to clinical trials. a review. Control Clin Trials. 2002;23:662–674. pmid:12505244
  43. 43. Senn SS, Senn S. Cross-over trials in clinical research. 2nd ed. West Sussex, UK: John Wiley & Sons; 2002.
  44. 44. Grob JJ, Amonkar MM, Martin-Algarra S, Demidov LV, Goodman V, Grotzinger K, et al. Patient perception of the benefit of a BRAF inhibitor in metastatic melanoma: quality-of-life analyses of the BREAK-3 study comparing dabrafenib with dacarbazine. Ann Oncol. 2014;25:1428–1436. pmid:24769640
  45. 45. Reijneveld JC, Taphoorn MJ, Coens C, Bromberg JE, Mason WP, Hoang-Xuan K, et al. Health-related quality of life in patients with high-risk low-grade glioma (EORTC 22033–26033): a randomised, open-label, phase 3 intergroup study. Lancet Oncol. 2016;17:1533–1542. pmid:27686943
  46. 46. Linge-Dahl LM, Heintz S, Ruch W, Radbruch L. Humor Assessment and Interventions in Palliative Care: A Systematic Review. Front Psychol. 2018;9:890. pmid:29973892
  47. 47. Gotay CC, Muraoka MY. Quality of life in long-term survivors of adult-onset cancers. J Natl Cancer Inst. 1998;90:656–667. pmid:9586662
  48. 48. McPhail S, Haines T. Response shift, recall bias and their effect on measuring change in health-related quality of life amongst older hospital patients. Health Qual Life Outcomes. 2010;8:65. pmid:20618978
  49. 49. Ellis L. Gender differences in smiling: An evolutionary neuroandrogenic theory. Physiol Behav. 2006;88:303–308. pmid:16753190