Skip to main content
Advertisement
Browse Subject Areas
?

Click through the PLOS taxonomy to find articles in your field.

For more information about PLOS Subject Areas, click here.

  • Loading metrics

Seeing attractive faces challenges inhibitory control, especially when mindful

  • Zsófia Logemann-Molnár,

    Roles Conceptualization, Formal analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Project administration, Visualization, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliations Doctoral School of Psychology, ELTE, Eötvös Loránd University, Budapest, Hungary, Institute of Research on Adult Education and Knowledge Management, ELTE Eötvös Loránd University, Budapest, Hungary, Institute of Psychology, ELTE, Eötvös Loránd University, Budapest, Hungary

  • Anna Veres-Székely,

    Roles Conceptualization, Funding acquisition, Methodology, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation MTA-ELTE Lendület Adaptation Research Group, Institute of Psychology, ELTE Eötvös Loránd University, Budapest, Hungary

  • Zsolt Demetrovics,

    Roles Conceptualization, Funding acquisition, Methodology, Resources, Supervision, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliations Institute of Psychology, ELTE, Eötvös Loránd University, Budapest, Hungary, Centre of Excellence in Responsible Gaming, University of Gibraltar, Gibraltar, Gibraltar

  • H. N. Alexander Logemann

    Roles Conceptualization, Formal analysis, Funding acquisition, Methodology, Project administration, Resources, Software, Visualization, Writing – review & editing

    alexander.logemann@ppk.elte.hu

    Affiliation Institute of Psychology, ELTE, Eötvös Loránd University, Budapest, Hungary

Abstract

Previous studies have suggested positive effects of mindfulness on inhibitory control (stopping behaviour). However, scarce previous studies suggest the relationship may depend on context. We provide first evidence that inhibitory control is challenged when perceiving attractive faces, especially when being mindful. Specifically, we investigated the relationship between mindfulness and inhibitory control and the moderating role of a social reward context (being exposed to attractive opposite sex faces). Participants (n = 50) between 18–43 years old (M = 25, SD = 5.4) filled out questionnaires assessing standard demographic variables and dispositional mindfulness. Subsequently, they performed a Go/No-go task with a neutral condition and attractive faces condition. Results showed that inhibitory control was challenged in the attractive condition relative to the neutral condition, p = 0.019. Dispositional mindfulness was negatively correlated with inhibitory performance, but only in the attractive faces condition (r = -0.32, p = 0.024). Results did not support a moderating role of gender. Finally, though post-hoc, higher mindfulness was associated with reduced perceived attractiveness of presented faces (r = -0.33, p = 0.019). However, the relationship between mindfulness and reduced inhibitory control could not be explained by mindfulness associated reduced attractiveness. Taken together, results show that mindfulness challenges inhibitory control when perceiving attractive faces. This implies that mindfulness interventions aimed at enhancing inhibitory control, may not render the desired effect in a context of being exposed to attractive faces. Though certainly plausible, it remains an open question whether results generalize to other reward contexts as well.

1 Introduction

In short, mindfulness may be defined as the ability to employ awareness by attending internal as well as external events in an open, discerning and attentive manner [1]. Importantly, recent reviews strongly suggest that mindfulness-training is associated with improved inhibitory control, both in children [2] and adults [3]. Certainly, as inhibitory control is crucial for everyday functioning, these results are of importance in relation to optimizing general performance, but also in relation to the treatment of conditions in which inhibitory control is impaired. It should be noted however, that individuals do not operate in isolation but in social rewarding environments, and in varying emotional states. Interestingly, to the best of our knowledge, the effect of being exposed to attractive faces (a social rewarding context) as well as individuals’ emotional states in relation to the relationship between dispositional mindfulness and inhibitory control has not yet been thoroughly explored. This is important, as the relationship between mindfulness and inhibitory control may vary as a function of such moderators. In the current report, the main aim was to evaluate the relationship between dispositional mindfulness and inhibitory control in the contexts that differed in terms of reward. As a secondary explorative aim, we addressed the question whether emotional distress would moderate the relationship between mindfulness and inhibitory control in the different contexts.

The mechanism of mindfulness is still not fully understood, but progress is made. Firstly, it should be mentioned that there seems to be considerable overlap regarding the brain regions associated with induced mindfulness and dispositional (trait) mindfulness [4, 5], and mindfulness training has been shown to enhance dispositional mindfulness [6, 7] Importantly, reduced default mode network (DMN) activity and connectivity in associated regions has been reported both for dispositional mindfulness [4] and induced mindfulness [5]. Simplified, the default mode network is essential for adaptive functioning and is associated with self-generated thought [8]. It includes a number of interconnected regions: a core, medial temporal, and dorsomedial subsystem [5, 8]. According to an elaborate integrative overview by Andrews-Hanna et al. [8], the core subsystem is involved in self-referential processes. This system includes the anterior medial prefrontal and posterior cingulated cortex. The medial temporal subsystem is deemed important in episodic memory processes as well as mental simulation. It includes the hippocampus, parahippocampal cortex, the posterior inferior parietal lobule and ventral medial prefrontal cortex. Lastly, the dorsomedial subsystem mediates mentalizing, as well as conceptual processes. It should be noted that there is some overlap with respect to processes (e.g. social and memory processes) subserved by the three subsystems [8].

There is a dynamic interplay between the DMN and the so called “Salience Network” (SN), and mindfulness has been shown to differentially affect the DMN and SN [4, 9]. The SN consist of several regions including the anterior cingulate cortex and anterior insula [10]. The SN is involved in the detection of and attentional reorienting to relevant/salient external stimuli, and activation of this network has been shown to be associated with suppressed DMN activity [11].

On the cognitive/behavioural level, previous studies have shown that mindfulness based interventions have positive effects on executive functions, both in terms of performance [2, 3, 1214], as well as in the brain activity (electrophysiological) responses that drive the performance benefits [12]. One important component of executive functions is inhibitory control which can be defined as the ability to suppress or withhold a prepotent response, and is commonly objectively measured with go/no-go tasks [15] or a stop signal task [16, 17]. Specific positive effects on inhibitory control as a result of mindfulness have been suggested, though most studies have employed measures that make it difficult to disentangle inhibitory related processes from other processes that contribute to inhibitory performance [3].

In light of the above, it might be seen as counterintuitive that previous studies have shown a negative association between mindfulness and synchronicity in the right Inferior Frontal Gyrus (rIFG) [18], a region implicated in inhibitory control [19]. However, the relationship between mindfulness and inhibitory control may depend on when inhibition is required. For instance, in the context of an attentional blink paradigm, it has been shown that mindfulness was associated with improved disengagement of attention from previously presented salient stimuli [20]. This is congruent with another study which showed, in a series of experiments, that carryover effects from prior task-sets were reduced in participants that received brief mindfulness training [21]. These findings are of relevance to inhibitory control paradigms in which an inhibitory requirement generally follows a different task-set, namely a requirement to respond as fast and accurately as possible to a go stimulus (e.g. in SST [22] and go/no-go tasks [15]). Due to the mindfulness-associated attenuated carry-over effects from the response requirement of the primary task, inhibitory control may thus be facilitated, at least in neutral contexts.

The aforementioned results regarding the relationship between mindfulness and inhibitory control pertain to neutral contexts. However, as mentioned, individuals do not solely operate in neutral contexts, but are often engaged in social contexts. Firstly, it should be noted that social contexts that include attractive individuals are essentially reward-related contexts. To elaborate, perception of attractive individuals has been shown to trigger reward-related brain-circuitry [2325] and can be associated with increased approach behaviour. Specifically, results of various previous studies suggest that depending on subject characteristics individuals may respond more impulsively to stimuli that have reward related value [26, 27]. For instance, higher body mass index has been associated with poor inhibitory control, but only in a reward related condition [27]. Again, this underscores the importance of taking reward context and in a similar vein, social context, into account in evaluating effects on inhibition. Secondly, noting the very definition of mindfulness, but also noting the association with reduced DMN and inversely with enhanced activity of components of the Salience Network [4, 9], it may be that mindfulness is associated with increased susceptibility and associated reduced inhibition to stimuli that have reward value.

On the behavioural and electrophysiological level, studies provide some preliminary support for this notion. For instance, studies using an oddball paradigms suggest that increased mindfulness is associated with relatively enhanced attentional capture of salient stimuli [2830], including social stimuli such as human faces [30]. Taken together, this may suggest that when mindfulness is high, salient (e.g. reward-related) stimuli may capture attention more readily, which may complicate inhibitory control when (but not after, due to attenuated carry-over effects) being exposed to the salient, reward-related stimulus. Results of a previous study seem congruent with this notion [31]. Specifically, in that study, smokers were randomly assigned to either a brief mindfulness intervention or control group and performed a go/no-go task with target pictures that were related to smoking. Results indicated that mindfulness training was associated with a reduced stop (no-go stimulus) related P300, which may be interpreted as reflecting reduced inhibitory activity [15, 31, 32].

Based on the above, the following hypotheses were formulated. Firstly, it was hypothesized that there would be a positive relationship between dispositional mindfulness and inhibitory control in the neutral condition. The exact opposite was expected with respect to the attractive faces condition.

2 Methods

2.1 Participants

Individuals could participate if they were between 18–50 years old, did not use drug is the seven days prior to participation and did not have a neurological and/or psychological disorder (by self-admission). Participants’ data was excluded in case of erroneous data (defined in the materials section). After exclusion, the sample consisted of 50 participants (24 men, 26 women) between 18–43 years old (M = 25, SD = 5.4). Power was estimated using G*Power [33]. Specifically, a sample of 50 participants is sufficient to detect moderate correlations with a critical r of |.28|, assuming 80% power, and alpha set at 0.05. All participants were fully informed prior to participation and provided their informed consent prior to participation. The project was approved by the research ethics committee and conducted in accordance with the latest version of the declaration of Helsinki.

2.2 Materials

2.2.1 Mindful attention awareness scale (MAAS) [34].

The MAAS self-report questionnaire consisting of 15 items and is thought to measure dispositional mindfulness. A higher score represents a higher level of dispositional mindfulness. It has been reported that the scale has high reliability with Cronbach’s alpha exceeding .80 [34, 35].

2.2.2 The go/no-go (GNG) task.

The go/no-go task was modeled after version 4 as reported in Wessel et al. [15], and is thought to measure inhibitory control. There were two conditions, the neutral condition and attractive faces condition. Conditions started with the task instruction, followed by a 2000 ms fixation dot. A given trial started with the presentation of either a go (400x400 pixels) or no-go stimulus (500x500 pixels, equal to go-stimulus but surrounded by white border) presented for 150 ms. Subsequently, the fixation dot was presented for 1350 ms. Participants were required to respond to the go stimulus by pressing the spacebar, and to withhold a response to no-go stimuli. In the neutral condition, the target stimulus (go or no-go stimulus) consisted of one of four possible color-filled squares. In the attractive faces condition, the target stimulus consisted of one of four possible attractive opposite sex faces. Each condition consisted of 80 trials of which 20% were no-go trials. Trials were randomized and condition order was counterbalanced over participants. The task started with a practice block, that consisted of 12 trials with grey square target stimuli. Inhibitory performance was reflected in the proportion of inhibitions to no-go stimuli.

2.3 Procedure

The experiment was implemented online, using Psytoolkit [36, 37]. All participants were fully informed and could only participate after providing informed consent. After participants filled out the MAAS and DAS-21, they performed the GNG task. After completing the GNG task, participants were requested to rate the attractiveness of the opposite sex faces from a scale from 1 (unattractive) to 5 (attractive). Subsequently, the experiment was completed.

2.4 Statistical analyses

Calculation of the outcome variables was performed using R [38], and inferential statistical analyses were performed with SPSS [39]. Participants with missing and/or erroneous data were excluded from analyses. Specifically, participants with 20% or more omissions in either condition of the GNG task were excluded. In addition, response times faster or longer than 150 ms and 1500 ms respectively, were discarded. To test our hypotheses, the following tests were apriori determined, with alpha set at 0.05. For each condition (neutral and faces), the two-tailed Pearson’s test was applied to test for correlation between MAAS score and the proportion of inhibitions to no-go stimuli (as an index of inhibitory performance). To test whether gender moderated the potential aforementioned relationships, we employed ANCOVAs to test for interaction of gender with MAAS score with respect to proportion of inhibitions.

3 Results

3.1 Main analyses

Performance data from the GNG task is shown in Table 1.

On average, both women and men rated the opposite sex pictures higher than moderately attractive (respectively, M = 3.35, SD = .75; M = 3.94, SD = .78). There were no extreme values (values exceeding 3 times the interquartile distance) with respect to the proportion of inhibitions. Mean response time did not differ across conditions, F(1,49) = 1.18, p = 0.283, partial η2 = 0.023. As indicated in Table 1, inhibitory performance was lower in the faces condition as compared to the neutral condition. This main effect of condition was significant, F(1,49) = 5.89, p = 0.019. As depicted in Fig 1, no relationship between MAAS score and proportion of inhibitions was evident in the neutral condition. There was no interaction between gender and MAAS score (partial η2 = 0.018).

thumbnail
Fig 1. MAAS score and proportion inhibitions in the neutral condition.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0273913.g001

With respect to the faces condition, as shown in Fig 2, there was a significant negative correlation between MAAS score and the proportion of inhibitions (r = -0.32, p = 0.024). Importantly, the relationship was not affected by gender (partial η2 = 0.013).

thumbnail
Fig 2. MAAS score and proportion inhibitions in the faces condition.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0273913.g002

3.2 Post-hoc exploratory analyses

We also explored (post-hoc) the relationship between mindfulness and inhibitory control in the subsample that rated the faces higher than moderately attractive, and in the subsample that rated the faces not higher than moderately attractive, see Figs 3 and 4 respectively. The results of this additional analysis showed that in the group that perceived the faces as attractive there was a negative correlation between mindfulness and inhibitory control, r(22) = -0.493, p = 0.020. In the group that was exposed to face-stimuli that were perceived as no higher than moderately attractive, the relationship did not reach significance, r(18) = -0.288, p = 0.246. Though these results suggest a reduced effect size in the latter group, the absence of the statistically significant effect may be primarily due to relatively low statistical power of this post-hoc exploratory analysis.

thumbnail
Fig 3. MAAS score and proportion inhibitions for faces viewed as higher than moderately attractive.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0273913.g003

thumbnail
Fig 4. MAAS score and proportion inhibitions for faces viewed as no higher than moderately attractive.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0273913.g004

Lastly, to assess whether mindfulness would affect subjective attractiveness and to assess whether attractiveness may mediate/explain the relationship between mindfulness and reduced inhibitory control in the faces condition, we performed a Pearson’s correlation for the MAAS score and the average attractiveness score, and subsequently for the attractiveness score and proportion of inhibitions in the faces condition. There was a significant negative correlation between MAAS score and self-report score of attractiveness r(50) = -0.33, p = 0.019, but there was no significant association between self-report score of attractiveness and the proportion of inhibitions in the faces condition r(50) = -0.062, p = 0.667. Hence, no test of mediation was performed.

4 Discussion

To the best of our knowledge, it has not yet been thoroughly investigated whether dispositional mindfulness is associated with inhibitory performance and what the role of common moderators is with respect to this relationship. Specifically, we assessed the relationship between dispositional mindfulness and inhibitory performance in a neutral context and common social context in which individuals are exposed to attractive opposite sex faces. In addition, we assessed whether gender moderated the relationships between MAAS score and inhibitory performance in these contexts. Congruent with our hypothesis, our results indicate that higher dispositional mindfulness is associated with reduced inhibitory performance, specifically in a context of attractive opposite sex faces. However, we could not confirm an association between dispositional mindfulness and inhibitory performance in a neutral context. We did not find evidence for a moderating role of gender.

Our main results indicate that individuals with higher dispositional mindfulness show reduced inhibitory performance in a context of attractive opposite sex individuals regardless of gender. Indeed, one may question whether the relevant index (proportion of inhibitions to no-go trials) as assessed with the GNG task (at least partly) really reflects inhibitory control. It should be emphasized though, that the index as assessed with our current employed GNG task using a 1500 ms intertrial interval and 20% no-go trials is thought to reflect inhibitory control [15]. Of course, theoretically this index can also be affected by other processes, most notably attentional bias. In other words, if attentional bias and associated response tendencies (i.e. to reward-related stimuli) is enhanced, this logically complicates subsequent inhibition. However, and importantly, we did not find that the relationship between MAAS and reduced inhibitory performance could be explained by an inverse relationship between MAAS and response speed. More specifically, in the subsequent explorative analysis, we did not find a significant correlation between MAAS and response time.

Extrapolating our results, one may suggest that mindfulness training may result in poorer inhibitory control in a reward context such as being exposed to attractive opposite faces. However, nuance should be applied. Though there is a clear association between induced mindfulness and dispositional mindfulness [47], the effects of mindfulness training may differ from dispositional mindfulness. Specifically, one may argue that mindfulness training entails more than just enhancing mindfulness, and its supplementary effects may be partly attributable to non-specific effects (effects not directly related to increases in the level of mindfulness). In that vein, it should be noted that we did not find evidence of a relationship between dispositional mindfulness and inhibitory performance in a neutral context, whereas effects of mindfulness training regarding inhibitory performance have been reported.

Interestingly, though a post-hoc finding, mindfulness was associated with a significant reduction of reported attractiveness of the presented faces. Importantly, the relationship between mindfulness and reduced inhibitory control in the faces condition could not be explained by attractiveness rating as there was no association between rated attractiveness and the proportion of inhibitions. Though several studies have assessed the relationship between mindfulness and responses to emotional/angry faces[4042], to the best of our knowledge, no studies have assessed the relationship between mindfulness and attractiveness. One might speculate that with higher mindfulness one might be more restricted in reporting relatively high attractiveness of faces. To disentangle such effects from brain physiological effects, it would be interesting and necessary to combine a similar paradigm with brain activity indices of reward-processing.

One (at least perceived) limitation may be that the current study was employed online in a less-controlled environment as opposed to lab-conditions. On the other hand, it can be argued that the current format increased the ecological validity of the study. Still, concerns may be raised with respect to task-adherence and understanding and relatedly, the overall validity of obtained results. Certainly, a high-inhibit rate could simply be due to a failure to respond at all. To address such potential issue, we excluded those participants with 20% or more omissions to go-stimuli. It should also be emphasized that despite persistent myths regarding issues of validity and reliability, current online cognitive psychological experiments can yield valid and reliable data comparable to that assessed in controlled lab environments [43], which also applies to the online platform used for the current study [44].

A relatively limited number of trials and type of stimuli per condition were implemented. This was done for feasibility and avoid attrition as a result of task duration. To further evaluate the robustness of the effects, future studies could incorporate more trials and a larger set of stimuli per condition. This would also allow for the assessment of random effects for the different types of implemented stimuli. We should also note that stimuli in the different conditions were not specifically matched on stimulus complexity. Hence, one might argue this is a limitation as conditions may differ not only in terms of social context, but also in terms of stimulus complexity. However, though stimulus complexity differences might have affected the data, it does not explain the observed direction of effects. To elaborate, as mentioned, previous studies have shown that mindfulness is positively correlated with executive processes including inhibitory control in neutral contexts. Now, when task complexity is higher, as with increased stimulus complexity, response times and false alarms increase making ceiling effects less likely [45]. Hence, with increased task difficulty there is more room for detecting the previously reported positive association between mindfulness and inhibitory performance. However, in our paradigm, we evidenced an inverse relationship between mindfulness and inhibitory performance in the faces condition and this cannot be explained by stimulus complexity. In addition, if stimulus complexity would be sizeable in the faces condition relative to the neutral condition, this would be reflected in an overall response time difference between the conditions [45]. However, there was no observable main effect of condition regarding response time. Taken together, it is not plausible that stimulus complexity difference across conditions accounted for the observed effect. In a related vein, it might be argued that the difference between conditions in terms of the observed relationship between MAAS score and inhibitions is merely due to face content relative to a neutral content instead of attractiveness. However, results of our post-hoc analysis is incongruent with such notion. Specifically, the negative correlation between MAAS score and proportion of inhibitions was restricted to the subsample that rated the faces on average as higher than moderately attractive.

Lastly, as mentioned, the MAAS is a commonly used instrument to assess dispositional mindfulness with excellent reliability and validity [34, 35]. With respect to the construct, some nuance should be applied though. Specifically, the measure of dispositional mindfulness as assessed with the MAAS is a unidimensional, one-factor construct representing the degree to which one is aware of and attentive to present-moment experiences [34, 46]. Now, depending on how mindfulness is defined, it can be argued that mindfulness may be regarded as a multidimensional construct which includes other factors, such as acceptance, and non-reactivity [46]. In the current paper, we focused on the attention and awareness component of dispositional mindfulness, and cannot rule out other contributing elements of mindfulness when defined as a multidimensional construct. For instance, it could be argued that, at least in our paradigm, enhanced non-reactivity may translate to reduced responsiveness to inhibitory cues. It would be interesting for future studies to further explore such contributing factors of mindfulness.”

Taken together, results imply that enhancing mindfulness may challenge inhibitory control under conditions in which individuals perceive attractive faces. Though it is plausible and tempting to suggest these results apply to other reward related contexts, this is still an open question. Thus, appropriate nuance should be applied in generalizing results to other reward contexts.

References

  1. 1. Carlson SLS and LE. Mindfulness-Based Psychotherapy. Art Sci Mindfulness Integr Mindfulness Into Psychol Help Prof. 2009; 45–60.
  2. 2. Vekety B, Logemann HNA, Takacs ZK. The effect of mindfulness-based interventions on inattentive and hyperactive–impulsive behavior in childhood: A meta-analysis. Int J Behav Dev. 2020.
  3. 3. Gallant SN. Mindfulness meditation practice and executive functioning: Breaking down the benefit. Conscious Cogn. 2016;40: 116–130. pmid:26784917
  4. 4. Bilevicius E, Smith SD, Kornelsen J. Resting-State Network Functional Connectivity Patterns Associated with the Mindful Attention Awareness Scale. Brain Connect. 2017;8: 40–48. pmid:29130326
  5. 5. Feruglio S, Matiz A, Pagnoni G, Fabbro F, Crescentini C. The Impact of Mindfulness Meditation on the Wandering Mind: a Systematic Review. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2021;131: 313–330. pmid:34560133
  6. 6. D’Antoni F, Feruglio S, Matiz A, Cantone D, Crescentini C. Mindfulness Meditation Leads To Increased Dispositional Mindfulness And Interoceptive Awareness Linked To A Reduced Dissociative Tendency. J Trauma Dissociation. 2022;23: 8–23. pmid:34076566
  7. 7. de Vibe M, Solhaug I, Rosenvinge JH, Tyssen R, Hanley A, Garland E. Six-year positive effects of a mindfulness-based intervention on mindfulness, coping and well-being in medical and psychology students; Results from a randomized controlled trial. PLoS One. 2018;13: e0196053. Available: pmid:29689081
  8. 8. Andrews-Hanna JR, Smallwood J, Spreng RN. The default network and self-generated thought: component processes, dynamic control, and clinical relevance. Ann N Y Acad Sci. 2014;1316: 29–52. pmid:24502540
  9. 9. Doll A, Hölzel BK, Boucard CC, Wohlschläger AM, Sorg C. Mindfulness is associated with intrinsic functional connectivity between default mode and salience networks. Front Hum Neurosci. 2015;9. pmid:26379526
  10. 10. Yeo BTT, Krienen FM, Sepulcre J, Sabuncu MR, Lashkari D, Hollinshead M, et al. The organization of the human cerebral cortex estimated by intrinsic functional connectivity. J Neurophysiol. 2011/06/08. 2011;106: 1125–1165. pmid:21653723
  11. 11. Seeley WW, Menon V, Schatzberg AF, Keller J, Glover GH, Kenna H, et al. Dissociable intrinsic connectivity networks for salience processing and executive control. J Neurosci. 2007;27: 2349–2356. pmid:17329432
  12. 12. Cahn BR, Polich J. Meditation states and traits: EEG, ERP, and neuroimaging studies. Psychol Bull. 2006;132: 180–211. pmid:16536641
  13. 13. Keune PM, Bostanov V, Kotchoubey B, Hautzinger M. Mindfulness versus rumination and behavioral inhibition: A perspective from research on frontal brain asymmetry. Pers Individ Dif. 2012;53: 323–328.
  14. 14. Sahdra BK, MacLean KA, Ferrer E, Shaver PR, Rosenberg EL, Jacobs TL, et al. Enhanced response inhibition during intensive meditation training predicts improvements in self-reported adaptive socioemotional functioning. Emotion. 2011;11: 299–312. pmid:21500899
  15. 15. Wessel JR. Prepotent motor activity and inhibitory control demands in different variants of the go/no-go paradigm. Psychophysiology. 2018. pmid:28390090
  16. 16. de Jong R, Coles MG, Logan GD, Gratton G. In search of the point of no return: The control of response processes. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform. 1990;16: 164–182. pmid:2137517
  17. 17. Verbruggen F, Chambers CD, Logan GD. Fictitious inhibitory differences: how skewness and slowing distort the estimation of stopping latencies. Psychol Sci. 2013;24: 352–362. pmid:23399493
  18. 18. Kong F, Wang X, Song Y, Liu J. Brain regions involved in dispositional mindfulness during resting state and their relation with well-being. Soc Neurosci. 2016;11: 331–343. pmid:26360907
  19. 19. Aron AR, Fletcher PC, Bullmore ET, Sahakian BJ, Robbins TW. Stop-signal inhibition disrupted by damage to right inferior frontal gyrus in humans. Nat Neurosci. 2003;6: 115–116. pmid:12536210
  20. 20. Makowski D, Sperduti M, Lavallée S, Nicolas S, Piolino P. Dispositional mindfulness attenuates the emotional attentional blink. Conscious Cogn. 2019;67: 16–25. pmid:30471471
  21. 21. Kuo CY, Yeh YY. Reset a task set after five minutes of mindfulness practice. Conscious Cogn. 2015;35: 98–109. pmid:25982056
  22. 22. Logan GD, Cowan WB, Davis KA. On the ability to inhibit simple and choice reaction time responses: a model and a method. J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform. 1984;10: 276–291. Available: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/6232345 pmid:6232345
  23. 23. Aharon I, Etcoff N, Ariely D, Chabris CF, O’Connor E, Breiter HC. Beautiful faces have variable reward value: fMRI and behavioral evidence. Neuron. 2001;32: 537–551. pmid:11709163
  24. 24. Wilson M, Daly M. Do pretty women inspire men to discount the future? Proc R Soc London Ser B Biol Sci. 2004;271: 177–179. pmid:15252976
  25. 25. Liang X, Zebrowitz LA, Zhang Y. Neural activation in the “reward circuit” shows a nonlinear response to facial attractiveness. Soc Neurosci. 2010. pmid:20221946
  26. 26. Robinson MJF, Fischer AM, Ahuja A, Lesser EN, Maniates H. Roles of “Wanting” and “Liking” in Motivating Behavior: Gambling, Food, and Drug Addictions. Curr Top Behav Neurosci. 2016;27: 105–136. pmid:26407959
  27. 27. Houben K, Nederkoorn C, Jansen A. Eating on impulse: the relation between overweight and food-specific inhibitory control. Obesity (Silver Spring). 2014;22: E6–8. Available: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/24910860 pmid:24910860
  28. 28. Sanger KL, Thierry G, Dorjee D. Effects of school-based mindfulness training on emotion processing and well-being in adolescents: evidence from event-related potentials. Dev Sci. 2018/01/22. 2018;21: e12646–e12646. pmid:29356254
  29. 29. Isbel BD, Lagopoulos J, Hermens DF, Summers MJ. Mental training affects electrophysiological markers of attention resource allocation in healthy older adults. Neurosci Lett. 2019;698: 186–191. pmid:30659914
  30. 30. Delgado-Pastor LC, Perakakis P, Subramanya P, Telles S, Vila J. Mindfulness (Vipassana) meditation: Effects on P3b event-related potential and heart rate variability. Int J Psychophysiol. 2013;90: 207–214. pmid:23892096
  31. 31. Andreu CI, Cosmelli D, Slagter HA, Franken IHA. Effects of a brief mindfulness-meditation intervention on neural measures of response inhibition in cigarette smokers. PLoS One. 2018;13: 1–16. pmid:29370256
  32. 32. Wessel JR, Aron AR. It’s not too late: The onset of the frontocentral P3 indexes successful response inhibition in the stop-signal paradigm. Psychophysiology. 2014. pmid:25348645
  33. 33. Faul F, Erdfelder E, Lang AG, Buchner A. G*Power 3: A flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behav Res Methods. 2007;39: 175–191. pmid:17695343
  34. 34. Brown KW, Ryan RM. The benefits of being present: Mindfulness and its role in psychological well-being. J Pers Soc Psychol. 2003;84: 822–848. pmid:12703651
  35. 35. Van Dam NT, Sheppard SC, Forsyth JP, Earleywine M. Self-compassion is a better predictor than mindfulness of symptom severity and quality of life in mixed anxiety and depression. J Anxiety Disord. 2011;25: 123–130. pmid:20832990
  36. 36. Stoet G. PsyToolkit: A software package for programming psychological experiments using Linux. Behav Res Methods. 2010. pmid:21139177
  37. 37. Stoet G. PsyToolkit: A Novel Web-Based Method for Running Online Questionnaires and Reaction-Time Experiments. Teach Psychol. 2017;44: 24–31.
  38. 38. R Development Core Team. R: A language and environment for statistical computing. Vienna, Austria. 2017. doi:R Foundation for Statistical Computing, Vienna, Austria. ISBN 3-900051-07-0, URL http://www.R-project.org.
  39. 39. IBM Corp. IBM SPSS Statistics for Windows, Version 26.0. 2019. 2019.
  40. 40. Bauer CCC, Caballero C, Scherer E, West MR, Mrazek MD, Phillips DT, et al. Mindfulness training reduces stress and amygdala reactivity to fearful faces in middle-school children. Behavioral Neuroscience. Bauer, Clemens C. C.: Department of Brain and Cognitive Sciences and McGovern Institute for Brain Research, Massachusetts Institute of Technology, Cambridge, MA, US, 02139, cccbauer@mit.edu: American Psychological Association; 2019. pp. 569–585. https://doi.org/10.1037/bne0000337 pmid:31448928
  41. 41. Lee AKW, Gansler DA, Zhang N, Jerram MW, King JA, Fulwiler C. Relationship of mindful awareness to neural processing of angry faces and impact of mindfulness training: A pilot investigation. Psychiatry Res Neuroimaging. 2017/04/08. 2017;264: 22–28. pmid:28412558
  42. 42. Way BM, Creswell JD, Eisenberger NI, Lieberman MD. Dispositional mindfulness and depressive symptomatology: correlations with limbic and self-referential neural activity during rest. Emotion. 2010;10: 12–24. pmid:20141298
  43. 43. Hilbig BE. Reaction time effects in lab- versus Web-based research: Experimental evidence. Behav Res Methods. 2016;48: 1718–1724. pmid:26542972
  44. 44. Kim J, Gabriel U, Gygax P. Testing the effectiveness of the Internet-based instrument PsyToolkit: A comparison between web-based (PsyToolkit) and lab-based (E-Prime 3.0) measurements of response choice and response time in a complex psycholinguistic task. PLoS One. 2019;14: e0221802. pmid:31483826
  45. 45. Gajewski PD, Falkenstein M. Effects of task complexity on ERP components in Go/Nogo tasks. Int J Psychophysiol. 2013. pmid:22906814
  46. 46. Baer RA, Smith GT, Hopkins J, Krietemeyer J, Toney L. Using self-report assessment methods to explore facets of mindfulness. Assessment. 2006;13: 27–45. pmid:16443717