Skip to main content
Advertisement
  • Loading metrics

Understanding gendered trait preferences: Implications for client-responsive breeding programs

  • Cynthia McDougall ,

    Roles Conceptualization, Formal analysis, Funding acquisition, Investigation, Methodology, Project administration, Resources, Supervision, Validation, Visualization, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing

    cynthia.mcdougall@sei.org

    ‡ These authors are joint senior authors on this work.

    Affiliation Gender, Environment and Development Cluster, Stockholm Environment Institute (SEI), Bangkok, Thailand

  • Juliet Kariuki ,

    Contributed equally to this work with: Juliet Kariuki, Birhanu M. Lenjiso, Pricilla Marimo, Mamta Mehar, Seamus Murphy, Béla Teeken, Michael J. Akester, John A. H. Benzie, Alessandra Galiè, Peter Kulakow, Wagdy Mekkawy, Lilian Nkengla-Asi, Julie M. K. Ojango, Robooni Tumuhimbise, Brigitte Uwimana

    Roles Conceptualization, Data curation, Investigation, Methodology, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Social and Institutional Change in Agricultural Development, University of Hohenheim, Stuttgart, Germany

  • Birhanu M. Lenjiso ,

    Contributed equally to this work with: Juliet Kariuki, Birhanu M. Lenjiso, Pricilla Marimo, Mamta Mehar, Seamus Murphy, Béla Teeken, Michael J. Akester, John A. H. Benzie, Alessandra Galiè, Peter Kulakow, Wagdy Mekkawy, Lilian Nkengla-Asi, Julie M. K. Ojango, Robooni Tumuhimbise, Brigitte Uwimana

    Roles Conceptualization, Data curation, Investigation, Methodology, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation East African Policy Research Institute (EAPRI), San Jose, CA, United States of America

  • Pricilla Marimo ,

    Contributed equally to this work with: Juliet Kariuki, Birhanu M. Lenjiso, Pricilla Marimo, Mamta Mehar, Seamus Murphy, Béla Teeken, Michael J. Akester, John A. H. Benzie, Alessandra Galiè, Peter Kulakow, Wagdy Mekkawy, Lilian Nkengla-Asi, Julie M. K. Ojango, Robooni Tumuhimbise, Brigitte Uwimana

    Roles Conceptualization, Data curation, Investigation, Methodology, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Alliance of Bioversity International and the International Center for Tropical Agriculture (CIAT), c/o National Agricultural Research Laboratories (Kawanda), Kampala, Uganda

  • Mamta Mehar ,

    Contributed equally to this work with: Juliet Kariuki, Birhanu M. Lenjiso, Pricilla Marimo, Mamta Mehar, Seamus Murphy, Béla Teeken, Michael J. Akester, John A. H. Benzie, Alessandra Galiè, Peter Kulakow, Wagdy Mekkawy, Lilian Nkengla-Asi, Julie M. K. Ojango, Robooni Tumuhimbise, Brigitte Uwimana

    Roles Conceptualization, Data curation, Investigation, Methodology, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Athena Infonomics, Delhi, India

  • Seamus Murphy ,

    Contributed equally to this work with: Juliet Kariuki, Birhanu M. Lenjiso, Pricilla Marimo, Mamta Mehar, Seamus Murphy, Béla Teeken, Michael J. Akester, John A. H. Benzie, Alessandra Galiè, Peter Kulakow, Wagdy Mekkawy, Lilian Nkengla-Asi, Julie M. K. Ojango, Robooni Tumuhimbise, Brigitte Uwimana

    Roles Conceptualization, Data curation, Investigation, Methodology, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Resource Analyst, Cabin Resource Management, Vernon, BC, Canada

  • Béla Teeken ,

    Contributed equally to this work with: Juliet Kariuki, Birhanu M. Lenjiso, Pricilla Marimo, Mamta Mehar, Seamus Murphy, Béla Teeken, Michael J. Akester, John A. H. Benzie, Alessandra Galiè, Peter Kulakow, Wagdy Mekkawy, Lilian Nkengla-Asi, Julie M. K. Ojango, Robooni Tumuhimbise, Brigitte Uwimana

    Roles Conceptualization, Data curation, Investigation, Methodology, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation International Institute of Tropical Agriculture (IITA), Ibadan, Oyo State, Nigeria

  • Michael J. Akester ,

    Contributed equally to this work with: Juliet Kariuki, Birhanu M. Lenjiso, Pricilla Marimo, Mamta Mehar, Seamus Murphy, Béla Teeken, Michael J. Akester, John A. H. Benzie, Alessandra Galiè, Peter Kulakow, Wagdy Mekkawy, Lilian Nkengla-Asi, Julie M. K. Ojango, Robooni Tumuhimbise, Brigitte Uwimana

    Roles Conceptualization, Investigation, Methodology, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Myanmar Country Office, WorldFish, Yangon, Myanmar

  • John A. H. Benzie ,

    Contributed equally to this work with: Juliet Kariuki, Birhanu M. Lenjiso, Pricilla Marimo, Mamta Mehar, Seamus Murphy, Béla Teeken, Michael J. Akester, John A. H. Benzie, Alessandra Galiè, Peter Kulakow, Wagdy Mekkawy, Lilian Nkengla-Asi, Julie M. K. Ojango, Robooni Tumuhimbise, Brigitte Uwimana

    Roles Conceptualization, Data curation, Investigation, Methodology, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Sustainable Aquaculture Program, WorldFish, Penang, Malaysia

  • Alessandra Galiè ,

    Contributed equally to this work with: Juliet Kariuki, Birhanu M. Lenjiso, Pricilla Marimo, Mamta Mehar, Seamus Murphy, Béla Teeken, Michael J. Akester, John A. H. Benzie, Alessandra Galiè, Peter Kulakow, Wagdy Mekkawy, Lilian Nkengla-Asi, Julie M. K. Ojango, Robooni Tumuhimbise, Brigitte Uwimana

    Roles Conceptualization, Investigation, Methodology, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation International Livestock Research Institute (ILRI), Nairobi, Kenya

  • Peter Kulakow ,

    Contributed equally to this work with: Juliet Kariuki, Birhanu M. Lenjiso, Pricilla Marimo, Mamta Mehar, Seamus Murphy, Béla Teeken, Michael J. Akester, John A. H. Benzie, Alessandra Galiè, Peter Kulakow, Wagdy Mekkawy, Lilian Nkengla-Asi, Julie M. K. Ojango, Robooni Tumuhimbise, Brigitte Uwimana

    Roles Conceptualization, Data curation, Investigation, Methodology, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation International Institute of Tropical Agriculture (IITA), Ibadan, Oyo State, Nigeria

  • Wagdy Mekkawy ,

    Contributed equally to this work with: Juliet Kariuki, Birhanu M. Lenjiso, Pricilla Marimo, Mamta Mehar, Seamus Murphy, Béla Teeken, Michael J. Akester, John A. H. Benzie, Alessandra Galiè, Peter Kulakow, Wagdy Mekkawy, Lilian Nkengla-Asi, Julie M. K. Ojango, Robooni Tumuhimbise, Brigitte Uwimana

    Roles Conceptualization, Data curation, Investigation, Methodology, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation CSIRO Livestock & Aquaculture, CSIRO Agriculture and Food, Hobart, Australia

  • Lilian Nkengla-Asi ,

    Contributed equally to this work with: Juliet Kariuki, Birhanu M. Lenjiso, Pricilla Marimo, Mamta Mehar, Seamus Murphy, Béla Teeken, Michael J. Akester, John A. H. Benzie, Alessandra Galiè, Peter Kulakow, Wagdy Mekkawy, Lilian Nkengla-Asi, Julie M. K. Ojango, Robooni Tumuhimbise, Brigitte Uwimana

    Roles Investigation, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation Oxfam America, Washington, DC, United States of America

  • Julie M. K. Ojango ,

    Contributed equally to this work with: Juliet Kariuki, Birhanu M. Lenjiso, Pricilla Marimo, Mamta Mehar, Seamus Murphy, Béla Teeken, Michael J. Akester, John A. H. Benzie, Alessandra Galiè, Peter Kulakow, Wagdy Mekkawy, Lilian Nkengla-Asi, Julie M. K. Ojango, Robooni Tumuhimbise, Brigitte Uwimana

    Roles Investigation, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation International Livestock Research Institute (ILRI), Nairobi, Kenya

  • Robooni Tumuhimbise ,

    Contributed equally to this work with: Juliet Kariuki, Birhanu M. Lenjiso, Pricilla Marimo, Mamta Mehar, Seamus Murphy, Béla Teeken, Michael J. Akester, John A. H. Benzie, Alessandra Galiè, Peter Kulakow, Wagdy Mekkawy, Lilian Nkengla-Asi, Julie M. K. Ojango, Robooni Tumuhimbise, Brigitte Uwimana

    Roles Conceptualization, Investigation, Methodology, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation National Agricultural Research Organisation-Rwebitaba ZARDI, Fort Portal, Uganda

  • Brigitte Uwimana ,

    Contributed equally to this work with: Juliet Kariuki, Birhanu M. Lenjiso, Pricilla Marimo, Mamta Mehar, Seamus Murphy, Béla Teeken, Michael J. Akester, John A. H. Benzie, Alessandra Galiè, Peter Kulakow, Wagdy Mekkawy, Lilian Nkengla-Asi, Julie M. K. Ojango, Robooni Tumuhimbise, Brigitte Uwimana

    Roles Conceptualization, Investigation, Methodology, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing

    Affiliation International Institute of Tropical Agriculture (IITA), Kampala, Uganda

  •  [ ... ],
  • Alastair Orr

    Roles Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Supervision, Validation, Visualization, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing

    ‡ These authors are joint senior authors on this work.

    Affiliation International Crops Research Institute for the Semi-Arid Tropics (ICRISAT), Nairobi, Kenya

  • [ view all ]
  • [ view less ]

Abstract

Client-responsiveness is a foundation for effectiveness of public sector breeding programs in agriculture, aquaculture and livestock. However, there remains a considerable lack of clarity about what this means, specifically in terms of how programs can be gender-responsive. This study contributes to addressing that need. It does so through sharing higher-level insights emerging from the combined experiences of eight gendered trait preference cases from across nine countries in Asia and Africa. The cases spanned crops, fish and livestock. This study inquires into the nature of gendered trait preference information that can be generated, if there are systematic gendered preference differences and how to understand these, and implications for breeding programs seeking to be more gender-responsive. Key findings include that while not all data are immediately usable by programs, the information that is generated through mixed method, intersectional gender preference assessments usefully deepens and widens programs’ knowledge. The study evidences differences in trait preferences between women and men. It also reveals that these differences are more complex than previously thought. In doing so, it challenges binary or homogenous models of preferences, suggesting instead that preferences are likely to be overlapping and nuanced. The study applies a novel ‘Three models of gendered trait preferences’ framework and sub-framework and finds these useful in that they challenge misconceptions and enable a needed analytical nuance to inform gender-responsive breeding programs. Finally, the study highlights implications and offers a call to action for gender-responsive breeding, proposing ways forward for public breeding programs, teams and funding agencies. These include investments in interdisciplinary capabilities and considerations for navigating trade-offs while orienting to sustainable development goals.

Author summary

The success of public sector breeding innovation relies on client-responsiveness. Yet while women make up half of clients, they have been under-recognized and underserved to date. There is growing agreement that public breeding programs need to become more gender-responsive to address this. However, experience with and understanding of this approach remains limited. Information about gendered preferences is scarce. More fundamentally, there is need for greater clarity regarding how to understand and approach gendered trait preferences. Through this study, we contribute to addressing this. We look across and generate insights from the experiences of eight gendered trait preference studies from nine countries in Asia and Africa. These spanned crops, fish and livestock. While all cases found preference differences between women and men, together these add up to more than specific trait recommendations. They suggest that earlier framings of gender and preferences were limited. In response, we propose a more nuanced model. Moreover, we surface practical and strategic implications for breeding programs. We highlight that success requires more than strengthening frameworks and methodologies. More fundamentally, it requires building interdisciplinary capacities and using these perspectives and gendered data to navigate trade-offs while orienting to broader development goals, including women’s empowerment.

Introduction

The recent surge in public investment for agricultural research for development (AR4D) reflects renewed optimism that crop, aquaculture and livestock breeding innovations can contribute to food security, reduced poverty and resilience for low-income women and men [1]. Yet since the Green Revolution, many products from public-sector breeding programs, particularly in Sub-Saharan Africa, have not been widely adopted. [2]. This has raised questions about how these programs can become more responsive to the needs and preferences of their clients.

Clients of public breeding programs (directly) encompass fish, crop, and livestock farmers and supply chain actors as well as (indirectly) downstream market actors and consumers. These clients differ, however, not only in terms of value chain role–or even socio-economic status–but also in terms of gender. Breeding programs that have been gender-blind have proved costly failures [36]. Conversely, there are indications that breeding programs that explicitly address preferences of women (and men) may be more effective in meeting client needs and overcoming the gender gap in adoption [7,8]. This mirrors the broader gender design and data gap trend that undermines innovation through to policy globally, with perverse implications not only for (the female half of) the world’s population but also for development overall [9].

These insights have led to calls for public breeding programs to become more gender-responsive, i.e., to understand and equitably consider the preferences of both women and men [7]. A key rationale for gender-responsive breeding is thus instrumental: it may increase the impact of programs by raising levels of adoption. In this pathway, more gender-responsive breeding contributes to productivity and/or increasing incomes, and thus to Sustainable Development Goals relating to hunger (SDG 1), poverty (SDG 2), climate (SDG 13) and more. Gender-responsive breeding potentially contributes to expanding women’s ‘choice’ in innovation, and thus to a practical form of empowerment (SDG 5) [8]. Yet despite growing agreement that public breeding programs need to become more gender-responsive, experience with and understanding of this approach remains limited. For many programs, this is uncharted territory.

While there are multiple aspects to gender-responsive breeding [10], logically a foundation for making progress along this pathway to gender-responsive breeding is effective knowledge and understanding of women’s and men’s preferences. A major challenge, however, is that information about gendered preferences is limited. A search of English-language databases, for example, found that in the 30-year period between 1985–2015 just 39 published studies provided information on gendered trait preferences [11]. This gap represents more than limited gender-disaggregated data. In the context of growing recognition of gender and social dimensions in food systems as complex [12], it indicates a broader lack of cognizance—and thus need—in the sector regarding how to understand and approach gendered trait preferences.

This article seeks to address this need. It does so by synthesizing insights and lessons from experience from across eight gendered trait preference case studies. These cases cover four types of commodities: food crops (banana, cassava, sorghum), a fodder crop, livestock and fish. They span nine countries: six in Africa ((Egypt, Nigeria, Kenya, Mali, Uganda and Tanzania) and three in Asia (Myanmar, India and Bangladesh). Specifically, the article looks across case experiences, summary findings and expert perspectives in order to generate higher-level insights informing public breeding programs’ journey towards greater gender-responsiveness. This investigation is crystalized through the paper’s three guiding questions:

  1. What is the nature of information that can be generated by public breeding programs about gendered preferences? What are the challenges to that?
  2. Does this information reveal systematic differences in preferences between men and women, how and why?
  3. What do these findings imply for breeding programs seeking to be more gender-responsive?

To enable analysis regarding gendered preference differences (question 2), the article shares a novel 3-model analytical framework for gendered trait preferences. This includes a sub-framework unpacking four types of difference (granularity, ranking, intensity and encapsulation).

The paper is structured as follows. The analytical framework presented in the next section sets the stage by outlining the typology for understanding differences in gendered preferences. The next section presents the methodology of this paper. This is followed by results across the eight cases, which includes assessment of preferences using the analytical frameworks’ three models and sub-framework. The Discussion then assesses the insights in relation to the three guiding questions, including implications for gender-responsive breeding programs that are interested in becoming more gender-responsive. These embody four practical implications relating to data and interdisciplinarity, followed by two strategic implications relating to navigating trade-offs in decision-making once the gendered preference information is gathered. The Conclusion wraps up with a call to action for breeding programs, interdisciplinary teams, and funding agencies in the field of public sector breeding.

Analytical framework

This section outlines a novel analytical framework that we developed and applied in this study for understanding differences and commonalities in gender trait preferences (study question 2). We emphasise that this is an emerging framework, not a refined or widely tested ‘model’. Rather, it stems first from this study’s own need for a useful conceptual and analytical tool when none were available. More broadly, it represents an effort to address the need identified above for greater cognizance in the sector regarding how to understand and approach gendered trait preferences. Specifically, it embodies a preliminary framework and tool for sensemaking and analysis in this area. As such, in this study, we also assess this conceptualisation and its potential utility for client-responsive breeding programs (within study question 3).

Three models of gendered trait preferences: Overview

The framework, hereafter the ‘Three models of gendered trait preferences framework’, distinguishes three broad ways of understanding patterns of gendered trait preferences (Fig 1). At the higher level, the framework represents three ‘mental models’ or ‘ideal types’ (in the sociological sense of mental images representing core characteristics): homogeneous preferences; heterogeneous (dichotomous, binary) preferences; and overlapping preferences. As heuristic tools, such models are necessary assumptions to help conceptualise and simplify complex realities. In presenting these, we outline them in relation to the broader backdrop of evolving AR4D paradigms, in which public-sector breeding operates. We note, however, that the associations sketched out here between prevailing AR4D trends and the evolution of the mental models or assumptions in the public breeding sector are loosely defined based on author experience and need greater analysis, which is beyond the scope of this paper.

thumbnail
Fig 1. Three mental models of gendered trait preferences.

The three models are illustrated using Venn diagrams in which W = Women’s preferences; M = Men’s preferences. The notation elaborates: P = the full set of preferences; Pw and Pm = the subsets of women’s and men’s preferences, respectively; Pw1, w2, w3 = Women’s individual preferences, Pm1, m2, m3. = Men’s individual preferences.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pstr.0000025.g001

Green Revolution-era breeding programs in the 1960s did not specifically address the needs of women [13]. This aligned with the Green Revolution paradigm of monoculture [14]. In this framing, homogeneity ruled. In Asia, for example, the rural population was perceived in development as “farmers” living in unitary “households” and cultivating “irrigated rice”, which became the target environment for breeding programs [14]. Perspectives of trait preferences as homogenous—as represented by model 1 in Fig 1—mirrored the perceived homogeneity of environments. This is shown as a Venn diagram where the set of women’s preferences (W) is identical to men’s (M).

By the 1970s and 1980s there was an evolving trend in post-Green Revolution breeding towards a different framing. Breeding programs in this era pivoted towards the challenge of complex, diverse, risk-prone environments [15]. This recognition of differences mirrored the “women in development” frame of the 1970s, in which women emerged as a separate entity in farming systems [16]. Agricultural literature signalled women’s autonomy and their status as independent actors (for example, [17]). This emphasis on differentiating women and men as distinct actors is reflected in a binary model, in which trait preferences are assumed to be separate sets of preferences unique to women and to men. This heterogeneous view is represented by model 2 in Fig 1. In the Venn diagram, W ≠ M.

In more recent decades, broader development discourse has shifted from a framing of women as isolated or atomic actors to a more relational and interconnected view of gender (the ‘Gender and Development’ approach) [18,19]. At the same time, public breeding programs are under pressure to increase effectiveness by becoming more client-responsive [8]. In line with these trends, a third view emerges. This is represented by model 3 in Fig 1. This is a ‘blended’ model, in which women’s and men’s trait preferences are assumed neither to be fully identical (model 1) nor entirely separate (model 2), but potentially overlapping. In the Venn diagram, W and M intersect.

Unpacking overlapping preferences: A sub-framework of distinct types of difference

Model 3 is arguably an important advance in that it reflects increased breeding program recognition of human (client) diversity, while allowing for possibilities of convergence in preferences. However, we propose that this ‘blended model’ on its own is unlikely to capture the needed nuance and complexity of gendered differences in trait preferences. Specifically, this model on its own implies that the ‘overlapping subset’ represents identical preferences (in other words it is model 1’s homogeneity, transposed to sit within model 3). In contrast, we hypothesize the overlap may encompass important types of difference. As such, we flesh out this model further using an additional sub-framework oriented around four distinct types of difference (Fig 2). While these do not exhaust the types of difference in gendered preferences, as illustrated in Fig 2, they may provide a useful starting point:

  1. Encapsulation, where one set of gendered trait preferences is a subset of trait preferences for the opposite gender (model 3.1); For example, men’s preferences (M) may be a subset of women’s preferences (W).
  2. Granularity, where either women or men (or both on separate traits) can provide richer, more detailed information about a specific trait (model 3.2); for example, women may give more attributes (a+b+c) for a specific preference (Pw1) than men.
  3. Ranking, where either women or men give a higher or lower order of importance or rank to the same trait (model 3.3), for example, where Pw1 = r = 1 and where Pm1 = r = 2.
  4. Intensity, where either women or men attach greater importance to a specific trait, as measured by a higher score or the frequency with which they mentioned this trait (model 3.4); for example, where Pw1 = s = 10 and where Pm1 = s = 5.
thumbnail
Fig 2. Four types of difference in gendered trait preferences.

The models are illustrated using Venn diagrams in which W = Women’s preferences; M = Men’s preferences. The notation elaborates: P = the full set of preferences; Pw and Pm = the subsets of women’s and men’s preferences, respectively; Pw1, w2, w3 = women’s individual preferences, Pm1, m2, m3 = men’s individual preferences. For the relevant models, we note in subscript: r = rank given to preference; s = frequency score given to preference; 1… n = the order of ranking or frequency; and, Pa, Pb = attributes attached to a preference.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pstr.0000025.g002

Materials and methods

This study is based on collaborative expert reflection and analysis across eight case studies, including reflections on challenges and value of information generated. This was generated via a series of learning events and subsequent analysis (spanning 2018–2021) under the umbrella of the CGIAR Gender & Breeding Postdoctoral Fellow (PDF) Capacity Development Initiative (explained below).

Study methodology

The study’s methodology was oriented to leveraging higher-level, cross-case learning and insights (not in-depth comparison of specific preferences or other case- or commodity specific findings). As such, this study used the following four-stage, interdisciplinary methodology to critically reflect, assess, and generate lessons from across the cases and experiences.

Stage 1: Finding common ground across cases.

A two-day in-person workshop (May 2018) for all case teams to crystalise the shared research problem and associated research questions. It also involved examining questions, methods and emerging findings from each case and co-generating insights through assessing commonalities and differences. Key points from each case were synthesized and available to all team members as the foundation for joint analysis. Written or virtual contributions were provided by PDFs unable to be present.

ii) Stage 2: Conceptual model (analytical framework) development and testing.

Based on Stage 1 insights and literature review, a subset of the study team drafted a conceptual model (Figs 1 and 2), for potential use as the study’s analytical framework. The team shared this for validation and solicited feedback from larger peer group of researchers from the CGIAR Gender Platform Community of practice on gender and breeding as well as with the PDF members of the study team. This was at the CGIAR Gender Platform ‘Seeds of Change’ Annual Scientific Conference (April 2019).

iii) 3: Deeper dive into breeding program perspective.

The study team undertook a deep dive from the breeding program perspective through in-person and virtual assessments with the geneticists and breeders involved in the studies (May–July 2019, roughly one year after field data gathering). This was done to crosscheck against the original interest of the programs in their own cases (to generate information to inform breeding programs). Two participating breeders gave their views in a live panel discussion, responding to particularly ‘knotty’ problems relating to priority setting and interdisciplinary breeding, as identified by study participants. Further, five participating breeders were interviewed individually as key informants on Skype using a checklist. Breeders’ observations were manually analyzed and categorized into positives and negatives.

iv) Stage 4: Consolidating.

Throughout 2020–2021, the case-based insights from Stage 1 (including additional written case material and associated literature reviews), were assessed in relation to the Stage 2 analytical framework and integrated with Stage 3 insights to generate responses to the identified shared questions. Using virtual and written dialogue, the study team synthesized the insights into this paper.

In terms of scope and limitations, we note multiple issues of potential interest relating to the cases that are important, but beyond the scope. These include detailed commodity preferences, relation to and variation across contexts, in-depth explanations of gendered preferences in each case, and nuances of integration into programs. Similarly, while we present the summary of case’ methodologies, in-depth analysis of methodologies or the relationship of these and results are beyond scope. Finally, we note that the breeding programs associated with these cases are at different stages of gender-responsiveness. Some are already field-testing new products with women and men. Others have used gendered preferences to identify usable traits, while still others are at earlier stages of considering preference information. These, and differences in commodities and contexts, mean that we do not aim to directly compare study results in terms of traits. Moreover, since breeding is a slow process, evaluation of impact of the cases on their programs would be premature.

The cases

Each case represents a research project focused on identifying gendered trait preferences to inform public breeding programs (Table 1). The cases span six African countries (Egypt, Nigeria, Kenya, Mali, Uganda and Tanzania) and three Asian countries (India, Myanmar and Bangladesh). Three involve crops, three involve fish, and two focus on livestock, including fodder. The cases were purposively selected: they represent work from the Post-Doctoral Fellow (PDF) positions on Gender and Breeding established by the CGIAR in 2016. The findings from each of the individual cases were used to inform their respective CGIAR center breeding investments, with the potential to scale insights to national public breeding programs in low-income country contexts.

thumbnail
Table 1. Overview of the eight case studies identifying gendered trait preferences, spanning nine countries.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pstr.0000025.t001

As per Tables 1 and 2, the cases shared several common features. Although the specific research problem varied by case (commodity, context, breeding program stage), all studies focused on identifying gendered trait preferences. Each of the teams was multidisciplinary, involving at least one breeder (or in one case, hatchery, nursery and grow-out staff as no breeder was available), a social scientist (specializing in gender), and a PDF (sociologists, anthropologists, or economists, all with some gender expertise). As resources allowed, cases gathered primary or secondary data on contextual information—including agro-ecology and social and gender dynamics—to enable interpretation. The PDFs carried out reviews of literature on gendered preferences for their commodity. In terms of primary data, studies varied methodologically. Three (1,2,8) used mixed methods (quantitative and qualitative). The quantitative involved surveys, and in one case (8), a stated choice experiment. The remaining five relied on qualitative methods, either focus group discussions (FGDs)(3,5,6,7) and/or interviews (4). Sampling strategies for villages and respondents varied by case, according to aims and methodologies (randomized and/or purposive at different levels). At the level of respondent, all cases sampled to engage women and men. All cases combined this with intersectional sampling for representation of different socio-economic groups, as case-relevant—generally around value chain role (farmer, processors and/or consumers), age, and in one case (1), purposively selecting for low-income households aligned to national wealth index categorization. Teams used separate spaces and single gender discussions to avoid influence across genders. As pilots (learning-oriented) studies, these were relatively small in scale and, with exception of Case 1 and 8’s surveys, none aimed to be representative to the district/governorate level. In all cases, analysis used a gender lens (disaggregating preferences and including explanatory gendered information); they ranged in how explicit they were with intersectional analysis (such as by gender and age or region).

thumbnail
Table 2. Team composition, methodologies and further information on the eight cases.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pstr.0000025.t002

Results

Information generated

The information generated showed several consistencies across cases, but also some variation in terms of novelty (identification of gendered trait preferences that were previously unknown), scope (species or value chain focus), type of information (simple versus ranked), and immediate utility. This is presented in summary in Table 3 (based on Stage 1 workshop discussions, Stage 3’s key informant interviews, and Stage 4’s consolidation of information using case outputs, validated by case teams).

thumbnail
Table 3. Types and nature of information generated by the eight case studies.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pstr.0000025.t003

Novelty.

Based on PDF literature reviews and breeders’ expert knowledge, two cases (3,6) were identified as first to investigate gendered trait preferences for that commodity and location (Table 3). For the remaining six, the information available was limited. For example, the PDF literature review on banana in Sub-saharan Africa found that of 44 trait preference studies, only four gender-disaggregated [26]. Likewise, the fish PDF review surfaced significant data gaps [24]. In these cases, information was novel in terms of filling gaps. For example, while there were gender-disaggregated studies on fish size-related preferences, Case 1 this was the first to investigate preferences in terms of tilapia morphology. A further key aspect of novelty was new information that enabled understanding of preferences, including nuanced meanings, and information on related decision-making or roles (see below).

Scope.

Just over half the cases generated data on actors from a single value chain node: cases 3, 4, 6, 7, 8 elicited preference insights from rural farmers as producer-consumers (or in the case of fodder, ‘producers’), who may use as well as sell at market (Table 3). The other three (1,2,5) spanned multiple value chain nodes: one expanded from producers-consumers to a more explicit focus on processors (2); a second, included expanded further to also include some assessment of traders’ preference (5); and finally, one changed chain contexts and nodes altogether, by assessing low-income urban consumers (who were not producers)(1).

Type of information generated.

All cases generated information not only on identified preferences, but also the relative importance of the identified preferences (usually 10 traits or more). This was measured by qualitative or quantitative ranking (by the respondents themselves) or by assessment of frequency of mention in qualitative data (measured by researcher). In addition to preference information, all cases generated qualitative contextual information about the gender dynamics, including division of labour in relation to the commodity (summarized briefly in Table 4).

thumbnail
Table 4. Gender division of labour regarding the commodity for each case.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pstr.0000025.t004

Immediate utility for breeding program.

At the time of interviews with breeders (roughly one year after data collection), three of the cases (2,4,8) had advanced to reporting prospective usable traits. In the cassava case, women who made cassava products required varieties that stayed white/bright after processing. The program had previously believed that the colour of processed products was determined only by processing methods; the case surfaced that this may also be genetically-controlled. In sorghum, the preference for hard grains can be met by selecting for plants with corneous endosperm (endosperm is the largest part of the sorghum grain). Finally, in the case of sheep and goats, the productivity traits of growth rate and milk yield were identified.

Additionally, the rohu fish case confirmed priority traits already being bred for (larger size and faster growth rates) as preferences to continue. The other identified characteristics of potential interest for the program (flesh content, boniness), require a further round of data gathering to identify specifically heritable traits that could be addressed by the breeding program. Additionally, three of the cases (1,2,3) generated information that informed understanding of the value chains, such as regarding the form of fish consumption (1) or the adoption of improved varieties of cassava or fodder grass (2,3).

Gender analysis of preferences

Table 5 maps findings of the cases against the three mental models of gendered trait preferences: identical, totally discrete, overlapping (Fig 1). In none of the eight cases were the preferences of women and men completely identical, nor were they fully binary (completely separate). Only three cases (3,7,8) found some trait preferences mentioned by women but not by men, and vice versa. In the case of fodder grass (3), women (only) preferred leaves without sharp edges that made them easier to harvest; in the case of sheep and goats (7), only women’s preferences included health traits, while only men’s preferences for goats included ‘adaptability’; in the case of rohu fish (8), only women’s preferences included odour and flesh content, while only men mentioned price. However, even in these cases, the majority of trait preferences identified were overlapping (mentioned by both women and men). In all eight cases, therefore, there were overlapping preferences: in other words, although trait preferences were not identical, most preferences were mentioned by both men and women.

thumbnail
Table 5. Findings of the case studies against the three models of gendered trait preferences: identical, totally discrete, overlapping.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pstr.0000025.t005

Table 6 looks within overlapping preferences. It assesses the cases against the four types of difference within overlapping preferences: ranking, granularity, intensity, encapsulation (models 3.1–3.4 in Fig 2). The data indicates the presence of each of the four types of difference. These types occur to different degrees; the results suggest that most cases have more than one type of difference.

thumbnail
Table 6. The eight case studies mapped against the four types of differences in overlapping gendered trait preferences.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pstr.0000025.t006

The most common type of difference was ranking. The six cases that ranked traits found that while women and men might identify the same set of preferences, within these they ranked priorities differently. Generally, women gave higher ranking to preferences related to food processing, preparation and quality. These included ease of peeling (banana), ease of threshing (sorghum), harder grains to reduce post-harvest losses when de-hulling (sorghum), leaf traits enabling use in cooking (banana), flesh content (referring to firm meat quantity of flesh, rohu, tilapia), odour and appearance (tilapia, rohu). By contrast, men gave higher ranking to preferences related to production and sale, such as milk yield (cattle), grain yield (sorghum), and price (rohu). Where women emphasised production traits like milk (goats) this was in cases where they were responsible for sales. Granularity as a form of difference was second most common, found in 4 cases. Cases 2, 3 and 6 found that women’s trait preferences revealed or more nuanced knowledge of products or phenotype-related information. Conversely, in one case (5) men provided more detailed information (traits for preparing banana-based beverages). Intensity was similarly common: four cases (1,2,5,8) found that women mentioned a preference more frequently than men, and vice versa. Similarly, encapsulation was discovered in two cases. Here, case 3 and 8 found that men’s preferences were a subset of the total preferences expressed by women, or vice versa. For example, in the Rohu-Bangladesh/India case (8), in both countries women identified 10 ‘liked’ traits, which included 3 not mentioned by men.

Value of the information from breeders’ perspective

Breeders were asked in key informant interviews about cases’ benefit to their programs and limitations. Although necessarily subjective, their views are expert perspectives based on direct knowledge through participation in the study and as authorities in their fields, informed as well by the PDF literature reviews.

In terms of critical assessment of the value of the cases for the breeding programs, breeders identified eight positive values and five limitations (Table 7). On the former, breeders saw the main value of the cases as improving the quality (‘positives’ values 1–4), range (value 5), and availability (values 6–8) of information on preferences. In terms of limitations, discussion revolved around three main issues: specificity of information generated (precision); novelty (some did not identify previously unknown preferences); and, usability (characteristics and preferences identified may not be actionable by breeding programs, although possibly actionable by other programs such as farm management).

thumbnail
Table 7. Eight aspects of positive value, and five limitations, identified by breeders involved in the case studies.

https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pstr.0000025.t007

Discussion

In this section, we surface higher-level insights, structured around the study’s three questions.

What is the nature of the information that can be generated by public breeding programs about gendered preferences? What are the challenges to that?

Novelty, quality, and utility.

The results indicate that even established public breeding programs have the potential—through interdisciplinary gender trait preference studies such as these—to generate novel information. Whether gap filling or fuller information (needed for interpreting preferences), this is significant in that it represents the potential for programs to enrich their evidence-based prioritization. Broader literature suggests that the role of such studies in gender data gap-filling may be particularly important for noncereal commodities [11].

The results also suggest that breeding programs can use this type of interdisciplinary, gender trait preference studies to add to the quality of their information, in terms of accuracy enabling effective client-targeting and responsiveness. This may be perceived differently by different disciplines. Breeder’s reflections (Table 7) indicated quality in relation to depth and nuance generated by qualitative methods. Social scientists additionally emphasized breadth, in terms of socio-economic information for a new target group or outcome (e.g., case 1’s data linking low-income consumers, nutrition to preferences). Additionally, the cases signal the feasibility of enhancing quality in the sense of sufficient sex-disaggregated and gender-balanced data, which has been a persistent weakness in innovation systems [9]. While further work is needed, the cases’ headway in the much called for—but underdeveloped—area of intersectionality flags feasibility of this dimension as well. Together these reflect a sharpening and refining of programs’ understanding of client groups’ interests. As such, the study reinforces [7,27] in signalling that targeted, mixed methods studies can enable a more refined grasp of client segments, elucidating priorities that reflect the multiple dimensions of clients’ lived experiences.

In terms of immediate utility from the breeding program perspective, results indicate that interdisciplinary gender trait preference studies can generate information about new, potentially actionable traits (e.g., 4 of 8 cases in this study)—but it should not be assumed that they will always or immediately do so. Notably, when they did, new preferences appeared synergistic with current breeding priorities: i.e., none contradicted ‘must have’ breeding priorities (pest and disease resistance, growth). Rather they flagged traits that may increase adoption (e.g., quality traits such as colour for processed cassava or ease-of-use traits such as non-scratchy fodder leaves). Similarly, by expanding to generate data on clients in roles other than ‘producer’, the value-chain lens illustrated by the study (especially cases 1 and 2) may contribute to addressing the challenge evidenced by Thiele et al. [28]: that insufficient priority on consumer’s preferred traits contributes to limited uptake of modern varieties.

Challenges: Saying versus meaning, precision and expectations.

In relation to limitations, study teams identified a key challenge to generating useable and reliable information: accurate interpretation. This aligns with the problem of distinguishing between what is said, heard, and meant [29]. The study found that preferences, in particular around quality-related traits, may be expressed in terms that seem (to the interviewer) ambiguous, or one preference may conceal another. For example, women in Kenya expressed a preference for fodder plants with ‘wide’ leaves. Once clarified, the meaning was that they disliked Napier grass which has narrow leaves and sharp edges that cuts them when harvesting. The preference for ‘sticky’ eba, a doughy food from cassava, meant not that it stuck to the hands, but rather that it could be easily formed into lumps that held together well when eating without sticking to hands. Seemingly vague (to the interviewer) preferences such as these, or ‘tasty’ and ‘small’ bananas, are not directly actionable in breeding programs [30]. More detail is needed on the specifics associated with a preference. As such, quality preferences like these require closer investigation, i.e., the ‘thick description’ favoured by social anthropologists [31] and rely on qualitative skills to avoid intended meanings being ‘lost in translation’. Teams also reflected that social scientists’ awareness of the breeders need for precision, and what that meant (through breeder’s’ eyes), was critical. The foundation for getting the degree of nuance and clarity right—or wrong—thus relies not only on disciplinary skills, but also on the degree and quality of collaboration between disciplines, similar to other complex research challenges [32].

A related challenge emerged around goals and expectations. Overall, the cases set out to “identify trait preferences”. Yet to have practical value for breeding, a preference had to be linked to a useable trait—i.e., specific, quantifiable, hereditable characteristics that were technically feasible for breeding. Some preferences identified, however, were relevant to clients, but not actionable by breeding programs (for example, “price”). Moreover, for breeders, establishing the preference is only the first step in a longer process of justifying trait inclusion, which may include weighing economic values and cost-effectiveness. Even if multiple preferences are identified, only a limited number of traits can be practically included (given that the influence of one trait is reduced as more are added). In relation to this, a disciplinary difference emerged: the objective of social scientists focused on identifying client preferences; the end goal of breeders was to identify usable and economic, high-priority traits. This difference was not fully appreciated at the design stage of the cases. Sharper and earlier joint (interdisciplinary) understanding of objectives, the nature of informational requirements, and of hereditable and economic filters, would likely have helped generate greater usable information in a shorter timeframe, as well as temper expectations about how easily first order information about gendered preferences can translate to usable traits.

Does this information reveal systematic differences in preferences between men and women, how and why?

Elucidating commonalities and differences through the analytical framework.

The presence of identified gender differences across all cases, despite varying commodities, contexts and methodologies, indicates that the homogeneous view of preferences (model 1, Fig 1) is inadequate. This echoes the presence of gendered trait preference differences in the (limited) literature (e.g., [24,33]). Yet, results did not support a binary mental model either (model 2, Fig 1): only three cases (3,7,8) found traits not mentioned by the opposite gender (Table 5). Rather, the results indicate some gender differences—and considerable overlap—in preferences (i.e., overlaps were present in all cases). This suggests that the model of overlapping trait preferences is best fit here (model 3, Fig 1). This aligns (and refines) Tufan et al.’s [7,p.139] review that signalled “the resulting picture is not always one of clearly divergent preferences”. With only eight cases, it is too early to predict if this pattern would be found more broadly; given the diversity of contexts and commodities, teams and methodologies, it would be a valuable working hypothesis for future testing.

To unpack the overlap further, we turn to insights from the sub-framework addressing four types of difference in gendered preferences. How useful was this lens? We highlight four points:

  1. The sub-framework revealed that all four types of difference were present. Within a given case, there was usually more than one type of difference. This confirms that differences in gendered preferences are more complex than previously thought. In other words, a ‘difference’ is not simply the presence or absence of a preference. Difference may take other forms: as per the framework, it may also represent the relative importance attached to the preference (ranking, intensity), the specificity of the preference (granularity), or if one gender’s preferences are a subset of the other gender’s (encapsulation). While there may be other or more variations on types of difference, the sub-framework’s four types enabled greater clarity and precision in thinking about and understanding gendered preferences.
  2. The sub-framework elucidated that some types of difference were more common than others. The most common types of difference related to importance ascribed to shared preferences (ranking and intensity); in contrast, only two cases found differences where women’s preferences were not shared by men. While this needs further testing, the sub-framework here suggests that gendered preference differences exist—yet are less likely to be absolute and more likely to relate to differences of importance. This lens thus offers a useful corrective to overly binary assumptions about gender preferences.
  3. It appears that identifying the different types of difference (through the sub-framework) may serve varying functions in informing breeding priorities. Identifying differences in ranking and intensity can support programs in addressing preferences that are most important to both women and men (which can help inform assessment of trade-offs). Looking for encapsulation can reveal, for example, that men’s preferences may be a subset of women’s preferences. In this case, if a breeding program focused only on shared preferences, and did not assess encapsulation, it would overlook other preferences (those that were exclusive to women) and potentially miss important traits. Identifying differences in granularity is useful particularly for identifying specialist knowledge to inform new traits or refine existing ones in order to tailor to client preference. The sub-framework on its own, however, does not enable analysis of how differences would translate into risks or benefits if preferences are met or not met (and associated trade-offs)—more needs to be done in this area to inform prioritization.
  4. The development of the framework and sub-framework proved valuable in enabling cross-discipline dialogue. This suggests its use and adaptation may serve a programmatic function. In mapping gendered preferences, the framework(s) can facilitate dialogue between social scientists and breeders, supporting interdisciplinarity. This may be particularly useful in visualizing commonalities and differences in a way that supports evidence-based and transparent navigation of trade-offs in the prioritization process (see below, Strategic Implications).

Explaining gendered preference differences.

Gender differences in trait preferences are not biological in nature, but rather socially-constructed [34]. As Ashby [34,p.14] explains “The golden rule for making sense out of gender-differentiated trait preferences is to look for the explanation of how these preferences reflect underlying gender differences in assets, markets, information, and risk, and the ways institutions and policies condition these.” Here we unpack the socially-constructed (and thus dynamic) nature of preferences through examining a factor that emerges across cases: gender division of labour. This refers to the socially-constructed patterns of paid and unpaid work by women and men in households and along value chains (correlating to Ashby’s [34] components of institutions and markets). While the whole picture is more complex, it is an appropriate entry point here in that gender division of labour has been flagged as shaping trait preferences (e.g., [8,11,33]) and because it reflects and shapes power relations, agency, and resources, which in turn shape seed and technology adoption and innovation experiences [8,35].

The pattern of preferences in the results suggest that women and men gave higher priority to traits that are important for the work in which they currently engage, and which is gendered and context-dependent—in other words, it reflects the gender division of labour (Table 4). For example, women (who sell milk) gave a higher priority to milk yield than men (who sell goats) and gave a higher priority to meat in case 7. Conversely, it was men–who predominate in banana transportation–that identified traits associated with robustness during transport to market in case 5 (see also [30]). Women identified preferences for firm meat (making fish easier to cook) and hard sorghum grains (which produce more flour when pounded)—and were also primarily responsible for food preparation (cases 6,4). This association complements Christinck et al. [36] and Galiè et al. [37]’s findings that differing gender work explain why women focused more on production and use-related traits, while men focused more on production and marketing-related traits.

The above illustrates that, in many ways, the cases reflect gendered preferences that align with ‘traditional’ gender division of labour in low-income countries. In these contexts, as a result of constraining norms, interacting with factors such as gendered access to education, control over resources, and more, women are largely responsible for reproductive roles (cooking, feeding, care) and the productive roles socially-associated with these (harvesting, processing), and are more bound to homestead spaces (e.g.,[19,38,39]). Conversely, men–who embody socially-ascribed decision-maker and ‘breadwinner’ roles—tend to be associated with commercial production and higher-return work, public spaces, and emerging technologies (e.g.,[19,38,39]).

However, gender division of labour is neither universal nor immutable—it changes both by context and over time [40,41]. Preferences are thus not only gendered, intersectional, and context-specific—but also changing and influenced by evolving gender and socio-economic contexts. The fact that preferences will evolve with labour, and with progress in women’s empowerment and gender equality, signals that “it can be misleading to assume that trait preferences necessarily follow a ‘traditional’ division of labour” [11,p.259; e.g.,42]. Breeding programs will thus need to be pro-active in understanding changing preferences [11]. Moreover, noting these complex relationships opens the door for programs to consider influence in both directions, i.e., the question of if breeding programs may influence women’s empowerment through trait choices (see Strategic implications).

What do these findings imply for breeding programs seeking to be more gender responsive?

Practical implications: Data gathering, clients, frameworks and teams.

On a practical level, the study signals four implications for gender-responsive breeding. Given the study substantiating that preferences differ by gender, the first practical implication is basic yet important: in order to be client-responsive, breeding programs need to overcome previous sampling imbalances in AR4D by gathering information from (sufficient samples of) both women and men. While this may seem obvious, it is worth underscoring given the dearth of information available on gendered preferences and the tendencies for oversampling of men in data for innovation [8,9]. Extending this, and aligning with Orr et al. [43], systematizing the integration of socio-economic characteristics such as age, wealth or other along with gender into data collection and analysis will sharpen understanding of client segments [7].

Second, the study experiences suggest that breeding programs would benefit from expanding from a focus on producers’ to other value chain actors’ preferences, in particular, processors and consumers. The study complements the literature (e.g.,[44,45]) in evincing women’s significant post-harvest roles in agricultural and fisheries value chains. As such, widening the lens to post-harvest actors creates a pathway for greater gender-inclusivity. As per the study, this expansion may draw breeding program attention to women as (previously underrecognized) clients and surface potentially important traits.

Third, the study underscores that effective interpretation of (gendered) preferences requires fit-for-purpose data and innovative frameworks. Qualitative, explanatory data—as valued by breeders—are needed for their interpretive and nuanced insights and are an important complement to the quantitative data more commonly used in technical AR4D. This aligns with the private sector-inspired approach to customer segmentation and gender-integrated client profiles [27,43], as well as bringing into the breeding sphere the wider AR4D calls for qualitative methods (e.g.,[46]) and analysis of gender dynamics (e.g., from adoption research [35]). Conceptualization and analysis with enough depth and refinement to interpret gendered preferences implies the need for frameworks, such as the ones used in this study. As demonstrated, considering differences through such frameworks can elucidate critical information about types of differences, including importance (through ranking and/or frequency assessment), granularity and encapsulation, which in turn supports interpretation and use.

Finally, the fourth practical implication from the case experiences is that being more effectively client-responsive and gender-responsive requires building interdisciplinary commitment and ‘muscles’ between breeding teams and social science, including gender, teams. Thoughtful, routinized exchanges, mutual understanding and collaboration emerged as necessary investments. As an implication, this aligns with the call for more balanced respect between disciplines in science [47] and the notion that interdisciplinarity is essential to addressing complex problems in sustainable development [32]. The case experiences suggest, however, that this may take time, capacity development, and purposeful effort and strategies (see also [32]. We note that while this study focused on social science and breeding interdisciplinarity, teams may benefit from nutrition, food science, or other disciplinary expertise. For example, nutritional needs and potential trait responses to them, may not be known to clients and thus not identified, but could be brought into consideration by nutritionists within a multidisciplinary team.

Strategic implications: From preference data to navigating priority setting for gender-responsive breeding in the context of development objectives.

Designs and data presenting a sharp understanding of distinct plus overlapping preferences (model 3), whose preferences are whose, the extent and nature of difference (e.g., importance versus encapsulation), and why, create an evidence-based foundation for the next steps in gender-responsiveness: transparent, equitable and ethical decisions about which preferences to prioritize and thus whose needs and preferences will be met (and whose will not be) (see [7,43]). While details of gender-responsiveness in breeding cycles are beyond scope, study insights raise two important gendered data-to-decision making implications.

First, the study illuminates that a range of prioritization (and synergy or trade-off) scenarios—and thus considerations for gender-responsiveness—may emerge. When gendered preferences overlap or are complementary, breeding programs may be able to devise win-win solutions where an improved variety or species can meet the preferences of both women and men (for example, cassava that is both easier to peel and gives higher yields). Yet in some cases, differences in gendered preferences are such that win-win solutions are not viable in the same product. Using as an example the sheep case (7): men preferred rapid growth to enable quick sale of lambs, whereas women preferred slower growth so that lambs stayed longer with mothers, enabling more milk for women to sell. In instances when win-wins are not viable in the same product, as economically feasible and fit for clients, programs might develop multiple (different) targeted products to meet specific role and gender preferences. If that is not viable, programs may need to choose between priorities of different groups. In doing so, programs will orient to their larger development goals—yet these may also have trade-offs and complex feedback loops. In the sheep example, it is not only rapid income (sale of lambs) versus slower income (sale of milk); this scenario represents choices between nutritional security for children (via milk used for the household) and enhanced household wellbeing (via income), which also depends on who controls the income (as women’s spending is reported to lead to greater household benefits than men’s [48]). Given the need to navigate such complex scenarios, one strategy for gender-responsiveness is for programs to build mechanisms into decision-making (trade-off) processes that keep women as a priority client group at the fore. While it may not entail always generating women-targeted products, such mechanisms could serve to offset historical male-bias in technical innovation processes [9]. Additionally, as a foundation, priority-setting processes in these complex navigations will need to be explicit about why one client group’s preferences are selected over another. To enable transparency, data on who is being bred for—and who is not—could usefully be analyzed over time as part of monitoring and evaluation by teams, (potential) clients and funding agencies.

Second, the study signals important strategic choices breeding programs face in trait prioritization decision-making in relation to gender-related outcomes. In the context of gender-unequal food systems [49], programs are often embedded within larger AR4D institutions and funded by agencies with explicit gender-related goals, such as benefiting or empowering women or addressing persistent gender gaps and barriers. Common approaches to operationalizing these in breeding have involved aiming to meet needs in women’s current circumstances, and thus working on traits for marginal agricultural conditions. For example, prioritizing a variety of sorghum that tolerates low soil fertility, as women often farm in marginal lands. As this aims to respond to and benefit women, it is a step forward from gender-blind prioritization. While valuable, this option has a downside: as it ‘accommodates’ (works around) gender inequalities, it also reinforces (and does not challenge) norms and dynamics that position women in marginal aspects of production (and men in more lucrative ‘male domains’)[19,39]. Strategies for empowerment through breeding would include ensuring women have equitable voice in identifying priorities in the first place (such as through the cases), and complementary strategies to enable meaningful choice amongst breeding products (see [8]). This may have spin-off effects: studies explicitly engaging both genders as valuable ‘clients’ role model a recognition of equality that is potentially (constructively) disruptive in the gender-unequal contexts in which breeding programs operate. Economic empowerment outcomes through breeding may require prioritizing women’s preferences associated with their paid value chain roles (and wider program strategies to ensure women are not displaced). To contribute more deeply and sustainably to addressing persistent gender inequalities, breeding programs might take an even more ‘constructively disruptive’ approach [8], such as prioritizing innovations designed to support women shifting into higher return roles (instead of supporting in marginal roles). For example, a variety of cassava suitable for machine peeling (rather than current hand-processing) might support women in transitioning to machine processing, in combination with complementary AR4D strategies to address underlying structural factors (such as engaging men in addressing norms limiting women’s use of machines and preventing male-takeover). All approaches above are gender-responsive, because they consider women’s needs or preferences and are transparent about whose preferences are (or are not) being met. In deciding which approach(es) to follow, as well as client input and aligning to gender goals, AR4D’s ethical principles—the most important of which is “do no harm”—can help breeding programs navigate these choices [8,50,51].

Conclusion

Public breeding programs have historically struggled with the challenge that Criado Perez [9] underscores: innovation processes’ lack of gender-responsiveness leads to development that (unintentionally) privileges men over women. Less powerful actors (including women, especially from marginalized groups) are less recognized, and therefore less served as clients. In breeding for low-income contexts, this has consequences not only for equality, but also for adoption, and thus for nutrition, poverty reduction and resilience. Yet there has been a lack of clarity about how programs can address this, including a need for greater cognizance about how to understand and approach gendered trait preferences.

The study confirms that there are gendered differences in trait preferences. It also reveals that that these are complex: more likely overlapping than entirely discreet, and with meaningful differences within overlaps, in particular, regarding importance. The ‘Three models of gendered trait preferences framework’ and sub-framework tested in this study emerge as having potential value here in that they challenge misconceptions and enable the needed analytical nuance.

The study highlights that intersectional, gender preference assessments generate information about novel traits and can validate existing preferences. Importantly, they deepen and widen programs’ knowledge, building the foundation for gender- and client-responsiveness. Qualitative information refines understanding of preferences. Gendered value chain framing brings preferences of previously under-recognized clients (e.g., processors, consumers) into view.

Such information contributes to programs’ goals of (increased) adoption and thus potentially to development objectives such as poverty reduction, nutrition, or resilience. More broadly, what is the value of a gendered trait preference approach in relation to addressing inequalities in food systems? By ‘counting’ inputs of both men and women as valued clients in AR4D, it models equality and contributes to closing the gender data gaps that otherwise drive (gender-blind) innovation, services and policy. Moreover, as the foundation of a gender-responsive approach, it sets up to enable breeding to go beyond ’benefiting’ women within the status quo, to potentially strengthening ‘voice and choice’ as a form of women’s empowerment.

Given this value, we conclude with a call to action. The co-learning and lived experiences of the cases show that teams working together can advance towards the goal of gender-responsiveness. The challenge is now to move beyond experimentation. With this in mind, we propose the following:

Breeding programs are recommended to develop committed, multi-discipline teams of breeders, social (gender) scientists, and other disciplines as needed. These will need to build their interdisciplinary capabilities and vocabularies to ensure focus, actionable information, effective defining of clients, and rigorous multi-perspective insights.

These teams (and breeding programs) are suggested to iteratively test best practices for gender-responsiveness. In relation to preferences, from this study, these include: systematically collecting data from both (all) genders; expanding ‘clients’ to include processors’ and consumers’; explicitly recognizing women (and marginalized groups), as valued clients; drawing on qualitative, explanatory, gender dynamics, and context information; and, utilizing sufficiently nuanced frameworks to interpret, identify synergies or trade-offs. Teams and programs can pro-actively learn their way forward, with these as part of the transparent, equitable and ethical prioritization processes that embody gender-responsiveness, as well as engage collaborative with prospective clients and other stakeholders. At the heart of this is navigating decisions in relation to development aims, including empowerment.

Funding agencies can play a key role by recognizing the above as requiring long-term and beyond-technical investments. To support this, agencies are suggested to invest in extended timelines, and in the specific expertise, capacities and processes needed. Especially important here are the interdisciplinary structures and capabilities, as well as ‘learning lab’ type processes that can build on current momentum. These investments may both increase likelihood of fit with, and thus adoption by, (a fuller range of) clients and help embed public breeding programs in a trajectory of informed and equitable innovation for inclusive and sustainable food systems.

Acknowledgments

The authors sincerely thank the three anonymous journal reviewers who provided extremely valuable feedback to improve the manuscript. Next, we acknowledge that this work was undertaken as part of the CGIAR Gender & Breeding Postdoctoral Fellow Capacity Development Initiative, which was hosted by WorldFish. The Initiative involved the CGIAR Research Programs: Roots, Tubers and Bananas (RTB); Fish Agri-food Systems (FISH); Livestock; and, Grain Legumes and Dryland Cereals (GLDC). The authors express sincere thanks to the Programs, the contributors to the CGIAR Trust Fund that support the programs, and the CGIAR System Office for enabling this study—as well as to USAID for enabling open access of the publication. We particularly acknowledge the Senior Gender Advisor Dr Jacqueline Ashby for spearheading the development of this Initiative and for her early and important encouragement and support to all. The authors would also like to acknowledge the incredibly important contributions of partners at BoP Innovation, Stats4SustainableDevelopment (Stats4SD), and Penn State University for their excellent design, methodological and analytical coaching during the studies. We recognize Anna Patel for her great support, including on formatting and finalizing the references and tables and Phil Macoun for inputs on figures. We also thank Dr Baloua Nebie who worked with the Sorghum Breeding program at ICRISAT in Mali, for providing access to information and resources for the sorghum case for this publication. Finally, we express sincere thanks and recognition to partners and all participants in the individual breeding studies in the nine countries.

References

  1. 1. Alston JM, Pardey PG, Rao X. The payoff to investing in CGIAR research. Arlington (VA): SoAR Foundation; 2020 Oct.
  2. 2. Walker TS, Alwang J. Crop improvement, adoption and impact of improved varieties in food crops in sub-Saharan Africa. Wallingford (UK): Cabi; 2015.
  3. 3. Kydd J. Maize research in Malawi: Lessons from failure. J Int Dev. 1989;1(1):112–44.
  4. 4. Njuki J, Kaaria S, Chamunorwa A, Chiuri W. Linking smallholder farmers to markets, gender and intra-household dynamics: does the choice of commodity matter? The EJDR. 2011;23(3):426–43.
  5. 5. Fischer E, Qaim M. Gender, agricultural commercialization, and collective action in Kenya. Food Secur. 2012;4(3):441–53.
  6. 6. Njuki JM, Wyatt A, Baltenweck I, Yount K, Null C, Ramakrishnan U, et al. An exploratory study of dairying intensification, women’s decisionmaking, and time use and implications for child nutrition in Kenya. EJDR. 2016;28(4):722–40.
  7. 7. Tufan HA, Grando S, Meola C. State of the knowledge for gender in breeding: case studies for practitioners. Lima (Peru): CGIAR Gender and Breeding Initiative; 2018. Working Paper No. 3.
  8. 8. Polar V, Mohan RR, McDougall C, Teeken B, Mulema AA, Marimo P, et al. Examining choice to advance gender equality in breeding research. In: Pyburn R, van Eerdewijk A, editors. Advancing gender equality through agricultural and environmental research: past, present, and future. Washington (DC): International Food Policy Research Institute (IFPRI); 2021. p.77–111.
  9. 9. Criado Perez C. Invisible women: Exposing data bias in a world designed for men. New York: Abrams Press; 2019.
  10. 10. Ashby JA, Polar V, Thiele G. Critical decisions for ensuring plant or animal breeding is gender-responsive. Lima (Peru): CGIAR Gender and Breeding Initiative; 2018. Brief No. 1.
  11. 11. Weltzien E, Rattunde F, Christinck A, Isaacs K, Ashby J. Gender and farmer preferences for varietal traits: evidence and issues for crop improvement. Plant Breed Rev. 2019;43:243–78.
  12. 12. Brouwer ID, McDermott J, Ruben R. Food systems everywhere: improving relevance in practice. Glob Food Sec. 2020;26,100398.
  13. 13. Lipton M with Longhurst R. New seeds and poor people. London: Unwin Hyman; 1989.
  14. 14. Anderson RS, Levy E, Morrison BM. Rice science and development politics: research strategies and IRRI’s technologies confront Asian diversity (1950–1980). Oxford (UK): Clarendon Press; 1991.
  15. 15. Chambers R. Challenging the professions: frontiers for rural development. London: Intermediate Technology Publications; 1993.
  16. 16. Quisumbing AR, Meinzen-Dick R, Raney TL, Croppenstedt A, Behrman JA, Peterman A. Closing the knowledge gap on gender in agriculture. In: Quisumbing AR, Meinzen-Dick RS, Raney TR, Croppenstedt A., Behrman JA, Peterman A, editors. Gender in agriculture. Dordrecht (The Netherlands): Springer; 2014, p. 3–27.
  17. 17. Dey J. Women in African rice farming systems. In: Women in Rice Farming. Proceedings of a conference on women in rice farming systems; 1983 Sept 26–30; Hants (UK): Gower Publishing; 1985. p. 419–444.
  18. 18. Okali C. Searching for new pathways towards achieving gender equity: beyond Boserup and ‘Women’s Role in Economic Development’. Rome: FAO, Agricultural Development Economics Division; 2011. ESA Working Paper 11–09.
  19. 19. McDougall C, Badstue L, Mulema A, Fischer G, Najjar D, Pyburn R, et al. Toward structural change: gender transformative approaches. In: Pyburn R, van Eerdewijk A, editors. In Advancing gender equality through agricultural and environmental research: past, present, and future. Washington (DC): International Food Policy Research Institute (IFPRI); 2021. p. 365–401.
  20. 20. Teeken B, Garner E, Agbona A, Balogun I, Olaosebikan O, Bello A, et al. Beyond “women’s traits”: exploring how gender, social difference, and household characteristics influence trait preferences. Front Sustain Food Syst. 2021;490.
  21. 21. Teeken B, Agbona A, Bello A, Olaosebikan O, Alamu E, Adesokan M, et al. Understanding cassava varietal preferences through pairwise ranking of gari-eba and fufu prepared by local farmer–processors. Intl J Food Sci Technol. 2021;56(3):1258. pmid:33776234
  22. 22. Diallo C, Isaacs K, Gracen V, Touré A, Weltzien ER, Danquah EY, et al. Learning from farmers to improve sorghum breeding objectives and adoption in Mali. J Crop Improv. 2018;32(6):829–46.
  23. 23. Rattunde F, Sidibé M, Diallo B, van den Broek E, Somé H, vom Brocke K, et al. Involving women farmers in variety evaluations of a “men’s crop”: consequences for the sorghum breeding strategy and farmer empowerment in Mali. In: Tufan HA, Grando S, Meola C, editors. State of the knowledge for gender in breeding: case studies for practitioners. Lima (Peru): CGIAR Gender and Breeding Initiative; 2018. Working Paper No. 3. p. 95–107.
  24. 24. Mehar M, Mekkawy W, McDougall C, Benzie JAH. Fish trait preferences: a review of existing knowledge and implications for breeding programmes. Rev Aquac. 2020;12(3):1273–96.
  25. 25. Orr A, McDougall C, Mehar M, Mekkawy W, Benzie JAH. Gender-differentiated preferences regarding rohu carp (Labeo rohita) in Bangladesh: A case study. Penang (Malaysia): WorldFish; 2021. Program Report 2021–29.
  26. 26. Marimo P, Caron C, van den Bergh I, Crichton R, Weltzien E, Ortiz R, et al. Gender and trait preferences for banana cultivation and use in Sub-Saharan Africa: a literature review. Econ Bot. 2020;74(2):226–41.
  27. 27. Ugen M, Karanja D, Birachi E, Katabalwa C, Ouma J, Mutuku R. Pre-cooked beans for improving food and nutrition security and income generation in Kenya and Uganda. Nairobi: IDRC; 2017 June. Final Technical Report.
  28. 28. Thiele G, Dufour D, Vernier P, Mwanga ROM, Parker ML, Schulte Geldermann E, et al. A review of varietal change in roots, tubers and bananas: consumer preferences and other drivers of adoption and implications for breeding. Intl J Food Sci Technol. 2021;56(3):1076–92.
  29. 29. BoP Innovation Center, CGIAR Research Program on Fish (FISH). Tool navigator: using market-based research methods for user-responsive innovation. [Internet]. Penang (Malaysia): FISH; 2018. Available from: https://www.slideshare.net/worldfishcenter/worldfish-tool-navigator-for-market-based-research-2018
  30. 30. Marimo P, Karamura D, Tumuhimbise R, Shimwela MM, Bergh I, Batte M, et al. Post-harvest use of banana in Uganda and Tanzania: product characteristics and cultivar preferences of male and female farmers. Lima (Peru): CGIAR Research Program on Roots, Tubers and Bananas; 2019. Working Paper No. 3.
  31. 31. Geertz C. The interpretation of cultures. Basic books; 1973.
  32. 32. Brown RR, Deletic A, Wong THF. Interdisciplinarity: how to catalyse collaboration. Nature news. 2015;525(7569):315. pmid:26381970
  33. 33. Marshall K, Mtimet N, Wanyoike F, Ndiwa N, Ghebremariam H, Mugunieri L, et al. Traditional livestock breeding practices of men and women Somali pastoralists: trait preferences and selection of breeding animals. J Anim Breed Genet. 2016;133(6):534–47. pmid:27292043
  34. 34. Ashby J., Relevance adoption, and impact: why gender matters in breeding. In: Tufan HA, Grando S, Meola C, editors. State of the Knowledge for Gender in Breeding: Case Studies for Practitioners. Lima (Peru): CGIAR Gender and Breeding Initiative; 2018. Working Paper No. 3. P.11-21.
  35. 35. Acevedo M, Pixley K, Zinyengere N, Meng S, Tufan H, Cichy K, et al. A scoping review of adoption of climate-resilient crops by small-scale producers in low-and middle-income countries. Nature plants. 2020 Oct;6(10):1231–41. pmid:33051616
  36. 36. Christinck A, Weltzien E, Rattunde F, Ashby J. Gender differentiation of farmer preferences for varietal traits in crop improvement: evidence and issues. Cali (Columbia): CGIAR Gender and Agriculture Research Network; 2017. Working Paper No. 2.
  37. 37. Galiè A, Jiggins J, Struik PC, Grando S, Ceccarelli S. Women’s empowerment through seed improvement and seed governance: evidence from participatory barley breeding in pre-war Syria. NJAS. 2017; 81(1), 1–8.
  38. 38. Mulema AA, Jogo W, Damtew E, Mekonnen K, Thorne P. Women farmers’ participation in the agricultural research process: implications for agricultural sustainability in Ethiopia. Int J Agric Sustain. 2019 Mar 4;17(2):127–45.
  39. 39. Aregu L, Choudhury A, Rajaratnam S, Locke C, McDougall M. Gender norms and agricultural innovation: insights from six villages in Bangladesh. J Sustain Dev. 2018; 11,4:270–287.
  40. 40. Sachs CE. Gender, agriculture and agrarian transformations. In: Sachs CE, editor. Gender, agriculture and agrarian transformations. Oxford: Routledge; 2019. p. 3–7.
  41. 41. Sachs CE, Jensen L, Castellanos P, Sexsmith K, editors. Routledge handbook of gender and agriculture. Oxford: Routledge; 2021.
  42. 42. Olaosebikan O, Kulakow P, Tufan HA, Madu T, Egesi CN, Teeken B. A case study of cassava trait preferences of men and women farmers in Nigeria: implications for gender-responsive cassava variety development. In: Tufan HA, Grando S, Meola C, editors. State of the knowledge for gender in breeding: case studies for practitioners. Lima (Peru): CGIAR Gender and Breeding Initiative; 2018. Working Paper No. 3. p. 35–43.
  43. 43. Orr A, Cox CM, Ru Y, Ashby J. Gender and social targeting in plant breeding. Lima (Peru): CGIAR Gender and Breeding Initiative; 2018. Working Paper 1.
  44. 44. Totobesola M, Delve R, Nkundimana JD, Cini L, Gianfelici F, Mvumi B, et al. A holistic approach to food loss reduction in Africa: food loss analysis, integrated capacity development and policy implications. Food Secur. 2022 Jun 6.
  45. 45. Kruijssen F, McDougall CL, van Asseldonk IJ. Gender and aquaculture value chains: a review of key issues and implications for research. Aquaculture. 2018 Aug 1;493:328–337.
  46. 46. Akimowicz M, Vyn RJ, Cummings H, Landman K. An introduction to mixed methods research in agricultural economics: the example of farm investment in Ontario’s Greenbelt, Canada. J Rural Stud. 2018;61:162–74
  47. 47. Urbanska K, Huet S, Guimond S. Does increased interdisciplinary contact among hard and social scientists help or hinder interdisciplinary research? PLOS One. 2019; 4(9), e0221907. pmid:31483810
  48. 48. Bank World. Gender equality and development. Washington: World Bank. 2011. World Development Report 2012.
  49. 49. Botreau H, Cohen MJ. Gender inequality and food insecurity: a dozen years after the food price crisis, rural women still bear the brunt of poverty and hunger. Advances in Food Security and Sustainability. 2020;5:53–117.
  50. 50. Ashby JA, Polar V. The implications of gender relations for modern approaches to crop improvement and plant breeding. In: Sachs CE, editor. Gender, agriculture and agrarian transformations. Oxford (UK): Routledge; 2019.
  51. 51. Orr A, Polar V, Ashby JA. User Guide to the G+ Customer Profile Tool (G+ CP). Lima (Peru): International Potato Center; 2021. CGIAR Research Program on Roots, Tubers and Bananas, User Guide 2021–1.