Skip to content
BY 4.0 license Open Access Published by De Gruyter Open Access December 19, 2019

Autobiographical memory increases pupil dilation

  • Mohamad El Haj EMAIL logo , Steve M. J. Janssen , Karim Gallouj and Quentin Lenoble

Abstract

Background

Pupil activity has been widely considered as a “summed index” of physiological activities during cognitive processing.

Methodology

We investigated pupil dilation during retrieval of autobiographical memory and compared pupil diameter with a control condition in which participants had to count aloud. We also measured pupil diameters retrieval of free (i.e., first memory that comes to mind), positive, and negative memories (memories associated, respectively, with the words “happy” and “sad”).

Results

Analyses demonstrated larger pupil diameters during the free, positive, and negative autobiographical memory retrieval than during the control task. Analyses also demonstrated no significant differences in pupil diameters across the three autobiographical memory conditions.

Conclusion

These outcomes demonstrate that, compared with counting, autobiographical retrieval results in a larger pupil size. However, the emotional valence of memories yields non-significant effect on pupil diameters. Our findings demonstrate how autobiographical memory retrieval yields pupil dilation.

Autobiographical memory increases pupil dilation

This paper investigates whether retrieval of autobiographical memory (i.e., memory regarding personal information) increases pupil dilation. Given this objective, we briefly the describe physiological bases of pupil dilation. We then describe research demonstrating how memory can modulate pupil dilation. As we will highlight, there is a large body of research demonstrating how memory can shape pupil dilation, but there is a lack of studies on pupil dilation during the retrieval of autobiographical memory.

To begin with the physiological basis of pupil dilation, the pupil is the opening area of the iris that allows light to enter the eye and reach the retina. The pupil is controlled by two sets of smooth muscles in the iris, namely the sphincter muscles and dilator muscles [1, 2]. Whereas the sphincter muscles decrease the diameter of pupil, the dilator muscles increase it. These muscles serve to optimize vision by modulating the amount of light that reaches the retina; whereas the pupil dilates in darker conditions, it constricts in brighter conditions. Pupil diameter typically varies from 1.5 to nine mm; pupil reacts to stimulation in about 200 ms and, in standard light conditions, pupil diameter is about three mm [3].

Pupil dilation is mediated by simultaneous activation of the sympathetic system and inhibition of the parasympathetic system, more precisely, cortical inhibition of the parasympathetic oculomotor nucleus [4]. Pupil dilation has been shown to be mediated by activity of the locus coeruleus–noradrenaline system [5], which plays an important role in cognitive processes [6]. More precisely, pupil dilation is mediated by activation of neurons in the locus coeruleus which supply noradrenaline to the eyes and brain; in the eye, the noradrenaline regulates pupil dilation, and, in the brain, it regulates attention [7, 8]. Pupil dilation is not solely mediated by neurophysiological processes but also by cognitive and emotional processes.

In a seminal work, Hess and Polt [9] reported that pupil was larger in response to positive images and smaller in response to negative images. Subsequent research has demonstrated that pupil typically dilates when participants are in conditions of increased emotion [10, 11] or attention [12]. This dilation has been attributed to the cognitive load of the task [13]. The effect of cognitive load on pupil dilation has been also observed on memory tests. Kahneman and Beatty [13] reported increased pupil dilation in response to the increased difficulty of a working memory task. Similar findings were reported by subsequent studies using the span tasks, on which participants are typically invited to repeat a string of numbers. The studies reported that pupil dilation increases with each digit retained in digit span tasks until the length of the digits exceeds the capacity of working memory, at which pupil diameter begins to plateau or even diminish [14, 15, 16, 17, 18].

Pupil dilation has been also studied in recognition memory [19, 20]. In recognition memory, participants typically make old/ new judgments on previously studied and new information. Using these procedures, Gardner, Philp [21] reported increased pupil dilation when participants processed old information during recognition memory tasks. They suggested that pupil dilation mirrors mental effort related to encoding and retrieval of information from memory rather than the general level of mental effort, as proposed by the cognitive load theory of Kahneman and Beatty [13]. In a similar vein, Vo, Jacobs [22] attributed pupil dilation in recognition memory tasks to the cognitive demands of recognizing old information compared with rejecting new information. According to Vo, Jacobs [22], recognition of old information requires retrieval of qualitative contextual information about the encoding episode (e.g., when and where the information was encoded), whereas correct rejection does not require this effortful retrieval. Also, Otero, Weekes [23] suggested that pupil dilation for old information depends on the strength of memory traces upon which recognition judgment is made. These suggestions can explain why, in recognition memory, pupil dilates more for information judged as old versus information judged as new [20, 24, 25, 26].

The above-mentioned research suggests that, on recognition memory tasks, pupil dilates more when participants process old stimuli compared with new stimuli. Inspired by this research, studies consider pupil dilation as an indicator of encoding and retrieval of long-term memory as pupil dilation predicts the strength of subsequent memory [27] and discriminating between familiar and recollected information [28]. The latter discrimination was evaluated by Kafkas and Montaldi [28] who reported a linear effect in which pupil dilation increased linearly from new to familiar and recollected information, with recollection producing the highest levels of pupil dilation and novelty the lowest, with familiarity falling somewhere in between.

Pupil dilation, as observed during recognition memory tasks, can be modulated by expectations; an assumption proposed by Mill, O’Connor [29] who reported increased pupil dilation during recognition of unexpected information compared to expected information. The authors suggested that dilation responses during memory recognition are mediated by expectations; more precisely, they suggested that expected information yields an acontextual sense of recollection whereas unexpected information yields a recollection of contextual information, and consequently, pupil dilation. This suggestion can be supported by the dual processing model according to which recollection-based decisions, compared to familiarity-based memory decisions, yields retrieval of contextual information about the encoding episode [30].

Together, there is a substantial body of research on the effects of memory on pupil dilation. This research reported the increased pupil dilation in response to increased difficulty of memory processing [13, 14, 15, 16, 17, 18]. This research also reported that, on recognition memory tasks, pupil dilates more when participants process old stimuli compared to new stimuli [20, 24, 25, 26].

The Present Study

Although the previous research is useful in understanding the effects of memory processing on pupil dilation, there is, to the very best of our knowledge, a lack of research on pupil dilation during autobiographical memory retrieval. This issue is important because the study of autobiographical memory is concerned with how people remember personal events. Autobiographical memory allows the recall of events that are relevant to one’s identity and sense of self [31, 32].

To this aim, we compared pupil dilation during autobiographical memory retrieval and during a control task in which participants had to count aloud. We also investigated pupil dilation following the emotional valence of memories for two reasons. First, autobiographical memory has been intimately associated with emotion [31]. Second, pupil dilation has been found to be sensitive to emotion [9, 10, 11]. Therefore, pupil dilation can be influenced by the emotional tone of memory. As for hypotheses, we expected a larger pupil diameter during autobiographical memory retrieval than during the control task. We also expected the large pupil diameter during retrieval of emotional memories than during the retrieval of free memories.

Method

Participants

The study included 36 graduate/undergraduate students from the University of Nantes (19 females, M age = 24.21 years, SD = 6.14, M education = 14.61 years, SD = 4.74). Participants were native French speakers. Among the original sample (N = 44), pupil data of two participants were corrupted, three participants were excluded owing to signal loss during recording, and three participants were excluded owing to previous psychiatric or neurological disorders. This final sample size was determined a priori using G*Power [33]. The calculation was conducted for repeated measures (four within-subjects measurements) ANOVA tests, based on 95% power, an estimated probability of making Type I error as .05, and a medium effect size of 0.25 [34]. In the final sample size, no significant differences were observed regarding gender [X2 (1, N = 36) = .87, p = .50].

Informed consent: Informed consent has been obtained from all individuals included in this study.

Ethical approval: The research related to human use has been complied with all the relevant national regulations, institutional policies and in accordance the tenets of the Helsinki Declaration, and has been approved by the authors’ institutional review board or equivalent committee.

Procedures and Materials

Generally speaking, procedures consisted of four conditions (i.e., free autobiographical recall, positive autobiographical recall, and negative autobiographical recall, as well as counting as a control condition). During these conditions, the participants wore eye-tracking glasses and faced a white wall (see Figure 1). Participants were tested individually and were informed that the experiment was concerned with memory. However, in order not to influence their performance, the participants were not provided with further details about autobiographical memory or pupil dilation.

Figure 1 During the four conditions (i.e., free autobiographical recall, positive autobiographical recall, and negative autobiographical recall, as well as counting as a control condition), participants wore eye-tracking glasses and faced a white wall
Figure 1

During the four conditions (i.e., free autobiographical recall, positive autobiographical recall, and negative autobiographical recall, as well as counting as a control condition), participants wore eye-tracking glasses and faced a white wall

In the autobiographical conditions, participants were invited to verbally generate three autobiographical events. Prior to each autobiographical event, participants were instructed to remember in detail an event related to the cue. They were also instructed that the event had to be personally experienced in the past and that the description had to be precise and specific (e.g., when and where the event occurred, what they were doing during it, who was present, what their feelings were). The participants were invited to retrieve one free memory, one positive memory, and one negative memory. For free memory, they were instructed to verbally describe the first memory that comes to mind. For the positive memory, they were instructed to verbally describe a memory associated with the word “happy”. For the negative memory, they were instructed to verbally describe a memory associated with the word “sad”. One minute was allocated to generate each autobiographical memory, and the duration was made clear beforehand so that participants could structure their memories accordingly.

On the control condition, participants were invited to count aloud to count from 1 “in their own time” until the experimenter said “Stop”. The latter signal was provided after one minute of counting. This control condition was chosen, because, like autobiographical memory, it relies on verbal behaviour, therefore, any potential differences in pupil dilation between the two conditions would not be the result of verbal behaviour.

Autobiographical memory retrieval and counting occurred while pupil dilation was recorded. Recording was stopped directly after memory retrieval and counting. Participants wore eye-tracking glasses. These glasses (Pupil Lab) are a remote pupil-tracking system that uses infrared illumination with a gaze position accuracy of < 0.1° and a 200 Hz sampling rate. Recording was processed with the Pupil Capture software. Prior to each condition (i.e., free, positive, and negative memories, as well as for counting) calibration was made by inviting participants to fixate on a black cross (a 5 x 5 cm cross, printed on an A4 white paper fixated at the wall center) and the cross was defined as a calibration reference, needless to say, that the cross was withdrawn after calibration. The experiment occurred in a quiet room at the psychology department of the University of Nantes. Blinds were closed and the lightness of the room (60-watt fluorescent lamp) was the same in the two conditions to ensure that differences in pupil dilation were not caused by differences in retinal illumination. Participants were seated in front of a white wall and the distance between the subjects and wall was approximately 30 to 50 cm. Participants were invited not to look outside the wall but were free to explore all parts of it. The wall displayed no visual stimuli (e.g., drawings, windows). We defined pupil dilation as the average dilation during each trial; this interval was chosen to encompass the full retrieval of memories, as well as the full counting in the control condition.

Results

We compared pupil dilation across counting and the three autobiographical trials (i.e., the “free”, “positive”, and “negative” memories) with repeated measures ANOVA, followed up by t-tests pair-wise comparisons. For t-tests, we provided effect sizes by using Cohen’s d [34]: 0.20 = small, 0.50 = medium, 0.80 = large. For all tests, the level of significance was set as p ≤ 0.05.

The pupil diameter data is provided in Figure 2. Analysis showed significant differences between pupil diameters across the four trials (i.e., counting, “free”, “positive”, and “negative” memories), F(3, 105) = 6.14, p = .001, η2 = .15. This effect was solely caused by the difference between the counting and the three autobiographical retrieval conditions. Wilcoxon tests showed larger pupil diameter during free autobiographical retrieval than during counting [t(35) = 2.76, p = .009, Cohen’s d = .56], larger pupil diameter during positive autobiographical retrieval than during counting [t(35) = 4.31, p < .001, Cohen’s d = .66], and larger pupil diameter during negative autobiographical retrieval than during counting [t(35) = 3.52, p = .001, Cohen’s d = .59]. However, no significant differences in pupil diameter were observed between free and positive autobiographical retrieval [t(35) = .76, p > .10, Cohen’s d = .17], between free and negative autobiographical retrieval [t(35) = .89, p > .10, Cohen’s d = .18], and between positive and negative autobiographical retrieval ([t(35) = .35, p > .10, Cohen’s d = .09].

Figure 2 Means of pupil diameters during counting and during retrieval of “free” (i.e., first memory that comes to mind), and “positive” and “negative” memories (memories associated, respectively, with the words “happy” and “sad”). Error bars are 95% within-subjects confidence intervals.
Figure 2

Means of pupil diameters during counting and during retrieval of “free” (i.e., first memory that comes to mind), and “positive” and “negative” memories (memories associated, respectively, with the words “happy” and “sad”). Error bars are 95% within-subjects confidence intervals.

Discussion

We compared pupil diameter during the retrieval of three autobiographical memories and a control task, in which participants had to count. Analyses demonstrated larger pupil diameters during free, positive, and negative autobiographical memory retrieval than during the control task. Analyses also demonstrated no significant differences in pupil diameter across the three autobiographical memory conditions (i.e., free, positive, and negative). These outcomes demonstrate that, compared to counting, autobiographical memory retrieval results in larger pupil size. However, the emotional valence of the recalled memories yields no significant effect on pupil diameter.

The main finding is the larger pupil diameter during autobiographical memory retrieval. This study is the first one to demonstrate that the retrieval of personal information yields dilation of the pupils. More specifically, it demonstrates that pupil dilation, as observed in the literature on the memory of general information (e.g., memory for words), can also be extended to self-related information (i.e., autobiographical memory).

Effects of Memory on Pupil Dilation

To begin with the contribution of our study to the literature on the effects of memory on pupil dilation (and as mentioned in the introduction), there is a large body of research reporting increased pupil dilation in response to increased difficulty of memory processing [13, 14, 15, 16, 17, 18]. This research also reported that, on recognition memory tasks, pupil dilates more when participants process old stimuli compared to new stimuli [20, 24, 25, 26]. This body of literature is, however, mainly concerned by recognition memory and working memory.

Research on pupil dilation and working memory has been mainly interested in how updating in working memory yields pupillary responses. In this research, updating has been defined as the ability to integrate incoming information with information that is currently held in working memory [35]. Using span tasks, research has shown increased pupil dilation with the increased number of to-be-tracked information [17, 18, 36]. Similar findings are reported by research using the n-back task. This working memory task requires participants to indicate whether the currently presented information is the same as information presented n trials back [35]. Using this task, studies have demonstrated that pupil dilation increases with increasing n [37, 38, 39]. This line of research has attributed the pupil dilation to the cognitive load of the tasks.

Interestingly, Hogervorst, Brouwer, and van Erp (2014) have shown that pupil dilation can be sufficient to distinguish high cognitive load from the low cognitive load. When compared to other physiological measures, such as electroencephalography, respiration, skin conductance, or cardiac rhythms, pupil dilation discriminates high and low cognitive load with 80% accuracy [40]. These findings demonstrate that pupil dilation is a reliable index of cognitive load associated with memory.

Pupil dilation, as observed in our study, may be attributed to the cognitive load associated with autobiographical memory retrieval. This attribution is supported by the above-mentioned research, suggesting how pupil dilation mirrors the cognitive load of working memory tasks [40]. Our attribution is also supported by the Self Memory System [31], according to which autobiographical memory retrieval requires a controlled reconstruction. This controlled reconstruction is required to retrieve not only the targeted information but also the context in which this information was encoded [30, 41, 42, 43]. In our view, pupil dilation during autobiographical memory, compared to counting, may be attributed to the general cognitive load of memory retrieval and, more specifically, to the cognitive effort required to construct the context in which the information was previously encoded [22, 29]. Alternatively, verbalizations were perhaps more complex in the autobiographical memory conditions than in the counting condition and this complexity might, therefore, rather than the autobiographical memory retrieval, drove the pupil dilation.

To summarize, there is a large body of literature on pupil dilation during memory a recognition and working memory tasks. Our findings contribute to this literature by demonstrating how the retrieval of self-related information (i.e., autobiographical memory) yields pupil activation as well. This activation may be attributed to the cognitive effort as required to reconstruct the memories and, more specifically, to retrieve the contextual information about the encoding episode.

Pupil Dilation as Physiological Measure

Our findings demonstrate how pupil dilation can be used as a physiological evaluation of this retrieval. Physiological correlates of autobiographical memory have been mainly evaluated with regard to brain activity. In this research, autobiographical memory has been associated with a “core network” of brain areas, including the hippocampus, medial and ventrolateral prefrontal cortex, posterior cingulate, and temporoparietal junction [44, 45].

Unlike the large body of research on brain activity during autobiographical memory retrieval, little research has attempted to evaluate cardiovascular or electrodermal activities during this retrieval. The latter research has demonstrated significant variations in cardiovascular and electrodermal activity during autobiographical memory retrieval [46]. In a similar vein, Robertson, Swickert [47] reported variations in blood pressure during autobiographical memory retrieval. Considering aging, Labouvie-Vief, Lumley [48] reported a lower heart rate in older adults than in younger participants during autobiographical memory retrieval.

Another physiological evaluation of autobiographical memory is facial expressions. Research has demonstrated variations of facial expressions during autobiographical memory retrieval [49, 50, 51]. This research has suggested that emotional facial expressions may reflect the physiological states that were experienced in the encoded events [52].

Autobiographical memory has not been only associated with the brain, cardiovascular, electrodermal, and facial expressions activities, but also with eye movements [53]. Eye movements during autobiographical memory retrieval were evaluated in a study in which participants had to retrieve autobiographical memories and, as a control condition, count aloud [54]. Results demonstrated a lower number of fixations but a higher number, larger amplitude, and longer duration of saccades in the autobiographical condition than in the control condition. Eye movement activity was attributed to the attempt of the visual system to create and manipulate mental representations of the memories [54]. A similar suggestion was made in a study comparing eye movements during retrieval of neutral memories and emotional memories [55]. Another study reported that autobiographical memory retrieval and future episodic thinking were accompanied by similar durations of fixations and saccades, as well as similar amplitudes of saccades [56].

Together, physiological correlates of autobiographical memory have been evaluated with regard to facial expressions and brain, cardiovascular, electrodermal, and eye movement activity. Our study contributes to this literature by demonstrating that autobiographical memory can also be evaluated with pupil dilation.

Effects of Emotion on Pupil Dilation

Although we have found that pupil diameter was larger during autobiographical memory retrieval than during the control task, our findings demonstrated no significant differences between pupil dilations across the retrieval of freely recalled, positive, and negative memories. This finding seems to contradict a body of research demonstrating how pupil dilation is dependent on emotion. Research has demonstrated that pupils tend to dilate when participants view positive or negative images relative to neutral images [10]. This dilation has been replicated in many studies in response to emotional images [57, 58, 59], emotional video-clips [60], as well as facial expressions [61, 62].

To resolve the apparent contradiction between our findings and the previous literature on the pupil responsivity to emotion, it would be of interest to highlight differences between our study and the studies in the literature. Our study is concerned with autobiographical memory, a memory system that has been intimately associated with emotion [31]. Except psychopathological conditions [63], autobiographical memory retrieval is emotionally loaded, the free retrieval, therefore, might activate emotional material in our participants. In other words, when asked to describe the memory that comes to mind first, as was the case in the free autobiographical condition, participants might have retrieved emotional memories. This interpretation can be supported by a previous study that reported that the majority of autobiographical memories cued by neutral words were emotional [51]. Because autobiographical memory typically triggers emotional material [31, 63], the lack of significant differences of pupil dilation between the free autobiographical memory retrieval compared with the positive and negative retrieval can be attributed to the assumption that all the three conditions might triggeremotional material.

In our view, similar pupil dilation for freely recalled, positive and negative memories may be attributed to the fact that our study, and that of Schaefer and Philippot [46] and Marci, Glick [64], included subjects without affective disorders. These subjects may succeed to regulate the emotional load of memories. It would be of interest therefore to replicate our procedures with a clinical population. For instance, because patients with depression have demonstrated systematic bias that favours negative memories [65, 66], it would be of interest to assess pupil dilation for negative memories in these patients.

Venues for Future Research

As previously mentioned, one venue for future research would be to assess pupil dilation during autobiographical memory retrieval in pathological populations. The assessment of pupil dilation would be especially valuable in amnesia. For instance, patients with Alzheimer’s disease have severe difficulties to retrieve autobiographical memories [67, 68, 69] and the same thing can be said for patients with Korsakoff’s syndrome [70, 71, 72, 73, 74]. It would be of interest to investigate whether pupil dilation would be observed despite autobiographical memory retrieval in these patients.

Also, both Alzheimer’s disease and Korsakoff’s syndrome are characterized by false memories [75, 76, 77, 78, 79, 80, 81, 82, 83, 84, 85, 86]. It would be of interest to evaluate whether these memories would activate pupil dilations because research has demonstrated pupil dilation for false memories in normal populations. For instance, Montefinese, Ambrosini [87] reported higher pupil dilation for false alarms (i.e., items erroneously judged as old) than misses (i.e., items erroneously judged as new).

We would also like to emphasize that pupil dilation, as observed in our study, reflects the general physiological characteristics of this dilation. As mentioned in the introduction, pupil diameter typically varies from 1.5 to nine mm, and, in standard light conditions, pupil diameter is about 3 mm [3]. Also, the difference differences on pupil dilation between the autobiographical and control conditions mirrors that observed in research on pupil dilation and cognition; pupil typically dilates around 0.5 mm to cognitive stimulation [2].

Conclusions

Pupil activity has been considered as a “summed index” of brain activity during cognitive processing. Cognitively relevant pupil activity typically occurs following inhibition of the parasympathetic nervous system as controlled by the locus coeruleus– norepinephrine system, which plays a key role in the regulation of cognition. By demonstrating how autobiographical memory can influence pupil activity, our study shows how pupillometry can be used as a measure of physiological responses to retrieval of self-related information.

Acknowledgments

The authors would like to thank Donia Ahmed (in the Figure 1) who assisted in testing.

  1. Conflict of interest: Authors state no conflict of interest

  2. Funding

    MEH was supported by the Intstitut Universitaire de France and the EU Interreg 2 Seas Programme 2014-2020 (co-funded by the European Regional Development Fund). The funder has no other role in the study design, data collection and analysis or decision to publish.

References

[1] Kawasaki A. Physiology, assessment, and disorders of the pupil. Curr Opin Ophthalmol. 1999;10(6):394-400.10.1097/00055735-199912000-00005Search in Google Scholar PubMed

[2] Sirois S, Brisson J. Pupillometry. Wiley interdisciplinary reviews Cognitive science. 2014;5(6):679-92.10.1002/wcs.1323Search in Google Scholar PubMed

[3] Wyatt HJ. The form of the human pupil. Vision Res. 1995;35(14):2021-36.10.1016/0042-6989(94)00268-QSearch in Google Scholar PubMed

[4] Steinhauer SR, Siegle GJ, Condray R, Pless M. Sympathetic and parasympathetic innervation of pupillary dilation during sustained processing. Int J Psychophysiol. 2004;52(1):77-86.10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2003.12.005Search in Google Scholar PubMed

[5] Varazzani C, San-Galli A, Gilardeau S, Bouret S. Noradrenaline and dopamine neurons in the reward/effort trade-off: a direct electrophysiological comparison in behaving monkeys. J Neurosci. 2015;35(20):7866-77.10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0454-15.2015Search in Google Scholar PubMed PubMed Central

[6] Bouret S, Sara SJ. Network reset: a simplified overarching theory of locus coeruleus noradrenaline function. Trends Neurosci. 2005;28(11):574-82.10.1016/j.tins.2005.09.002Search in Google Scholar PubMed

[7] Laeng B, Sirois S, Gredeback G. Pupillometry: A Window to the Preconscious? Perspect Psychol Sci. 2012;7(1):18-27.10.1177/1745691611427305Search in Google Scholar PubMed

[8] Eldar E, Cohen JD, Niv Y. The effects of neural gain on attention and learning. Nat Neurosci. 2013;16(8):1146-53.10.1038/nn.3428Search in Google Scholar PubMed PubMed Central

[9] Hess EH, Polt JM. Pupil Size in Relation to Mental Activity during Simple Problem-Solving. Science. 1964;143(3611):1190-2.10.1126/science.143.3611.1190Search in Google Scholar PubMed

[10] Bradley MM, Lang PJ. Memory, emotion, and pupil diameter: Repetition of natural scenes. Psychophysiology. 2015;52(9):1186-93.10.1111/psyp.12442Search in Google Scholar PubMed

[11] Bradley MM, Miccoli L, Escrig MA, Lang PJ. The pupil as a measure of emotional arousal and autonomic activation. Psychophysiology. 2008;45(4):602-7.10.1111/j.1469-8986.2008.00654.xSearch in Google Scholar PubMed PubMed Central

[12] Unsworth N, Robison MK. Pupillary correlates of lapses of sustained attention. Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci. 2016;16(4):601-15.10.3758/s13415-016-0417-4Search in Google Scholar PubMed

[13] Kahneman D, Beatty J. Pupil diameter and load on memory. Science. 1966;154(3756):1583-5.10.1126/science.154.3756.1583Search in Google Scholar PubMed

[14] Peavler WS. Pupil size, information overload, and performance differences. Psychophysiology. 1974;11(5):559-66.10.1111/j.1469-8986.1974.tb01114.xSearch in Google Scholar PubMed

[15] Granholm E, Asarnow RF, Sarkin AJ, Dykes KL. Pupillary responses index cognitive resource limitations. Psychophysiology. 1996;33(4):457-61.10.1111/j.1469-8986.1996.tb01071.xSearch in Google Scholar PubMed

[16] Cabestrero R, Crespo A, Quirós P. Pupillary Dilation as an Index of Task Demands. Percept Mot Skills. 2009;109(3):664-78.10.2466/pms.109.3.664-678Search in Google Scholar PubMed

[17] Wahn B, Ferris DP, Hairston WD, Konig P. Pupil Sizes Scale with Attentional Load and Task Experience in a Multiple Object Tracking Task. PLoS One. 2016;11(12):e0168087.10.1371/journal.pone.0168087Search in Google Scholar PubMed PubMed Central

[18] Alnaes D, Sneve MH, Espeseth T, Endestad T, van de Pavert SH, Laeng B. Pupil size signals mental effort deployed during multiple object tracking and predicts brain activity in the dorsal attention network and the locus coeruleus. Journal of vision. 2014;14(4).10.1167/14.4.1Search in Google Scholar PubMed

[19] Goldinger SD, Papesh MH. Pupil Dilation Reflects the Creation and Retrieval of Memories. Curr Dir Psychol Sci. 2012;21(2):90-5.10.1177/0963721412436811Search in Google Scholar PubMed PubMed Central

[20] Heaver B, Hutton SB. Keeping an eye on the truth? Pupil size changes associated with recognition memory. Memory. 2011;19(4):398-405.10.1080/09658211.2011.575788Search in Google Scholar PubMed

[21] Gardner RM, Philp P, Radacy S. Pupillary changes during recall in children. J Exp Child Psychol. 1978;25(1):168-72.10.1016/0022-0965(78)90046-2Search in Google Scholar PubMed

[22] Vo ML, Jacobs AM, Kuchinke L, Hofmann M, Conrad M, Schacht A, et al. The coupling of emotion and cognition in the eye: introducing the pupil old/new effect. Psychophysiology. 2008;45(1):130-40.Search in Google Scholar PubMed

[23] Otero SC, Weekes BS, Hutton SB. Pupil size changes during recognition memory. Psychophysiology. 2011;48(10):1346-53.10.1111/j.1469-8986.2011.01217.xSearch in Google Scholar PubMed

[24] Naber M, Frassle S, Rutishauser U, Einhauser W. Pupil size signals novelty and predicts later retrieval success for declarative memories of natural scenes. Journal of vision. 2013;13(2):11.10.1167/13.2.11Search in Google Scholar PubMed

[25] Papesh MH, Goldinger SD, Hout MC. Memory strength and specificity revealed by pupillometry. Int J Psychophysiol. 2012;83(1):56-64.10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2011.10.002Search in Google Scholar PubMed PubMed Central

[26] Montefinese M, Ambrosini E, Fairfield B, Mammarella N. Semantic memory: a feature-based analysis and new norms for Italian. Behav Res Methods. 2013;45(2):440-61.10.3758/s13428-012-0263-4Search in Google Scholar PubMed

[27] Kafkas A, Montaldi D. Recognition Memory Strength is Predicted by Pupillary Responses at Encoding While Fixation Patterns Distinguish Recollection from Familiarity. Q J Exp Psychol. 2011;64(10):1971-89.10.1080/17470218.2011.588335Search in Google Scholar PubMed

[28] Kafkas A, Montaldi D. Familiarity and recollection produce distinct eye movement, pupil and medial temporal lobe responses when memory strength is matched. Neuropsychologia. 2012;50(13):308093.10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2012.08.001Search in Google Scholar PubMed

[29] Mill RD, O’Connor AR, Dobbins IG. Pupil dilation during recognition memory: Isolating unexpected recognition from judgment uncertainty. Cognition. 2016;154:81-94.10.1016/j.cognition.2016.05.018Search in Google Scholar PubMed

[30] Yonelinas AP. The nature of recollection and familiarity: A review of 30 years of research. Journal of memory and language. 2002;46(3):441-517.10.1006/jmla.2002.2864Search in Google Scholar

[31] Conway MA. Memory and the self. Journal of Memory and Language. 2005;53(4):594-628.10.1016/j.jml.2005.08.005Search in Google Scholar

[32] Rubin DC. A basic-systems approach to autobiographical memory. Curr Dir Psychol Sci. 2005;14(2):79-83.10.1111/j.0963-7214.2005.00339.xSearch in Google Scholar

[33] Faul F, Erdfelder E, Lang AG, Buchner A. G*Power 3: a flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behav Res Methods. 2007;39(2):175-91.10.3758/BF03193146Search in Google Scholar PubMed

[34] Cohen J. Statistical Power Analysis. Curr Dir Psychol Sci. 1992;1(3):98-101.10.1111/1467-8721.ep10768783Search in Google Scholar

[35] Miyake A, Friedman NP, Emerson MJ, Witzki AH, Howerter A, Wager TD. The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex “frontal lobe” tasks: A latent variable analysis. Cogn Psychol. 2000;41(1):49-100.10.1006/cogp.1999.0734Search in Google Scholar PubMed

[36] Wright TJ, Boot WR, Morgan CS. Pupillary response predicts multiple object tracking load, error rate, and conscientiousness, but not inattentional blindness. Acta Psychol (Amst). 2013;144(1):6-11.10.1016/j.actpsy.2013.04.018Search in Google Scholar PubMed

[37] Belayachi S, Majerus S, Gendolla G, Salmon E, Peters F, Van der Linden M. Are the carrot and the stick the two sides of same coin? A neural examination of approach/avoidance motivation during cognitive performance. Behav Brain Res. 2015;293:217-26.10.1016/j.bbr.2015.07.042Search in Google Scholar PubMed

[38] Brouwer AM, Hogervorst MA, Holewijn M, van Erp JB. Evidence for effects of task difficulty but not learning on neurophysiological variables associated with effort. Int J Psychophysiol. 2014;93(2):242-52.10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2014.05.004Search in Google Scholar PubMed

[39] Pehlivanoglu D, Jain S, Ariel R, Verhaeghen P. The ties to unbind: age-related differences in feature (un)binding in working memory for emotional faces. Front Psychol. 2014;5:253.10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00253Search in Google Scholar PubMed PubMed Central

[40] Hogervorst MA, Brouwer AM, van Erp JB. Combining and comparing EEG, peripheral physiology and eye-related measures for the assessment of mental workload. Front Neurosci. 2014;8:322.10.3389/fnins.2014.00322Search in Google Scholar PubMed PubMed Central

[41] Johnson MK, Hashtroudi S, Lindsay DS. Source monitoring. Psychol Bull. 1993;114(1):3-28.10.1037/0033-2909.114.1.3Search in Google Scholar PubMed

[42] El Haj M, Antoine P. Context Memory in Alzheimer’s Disease: The “Who, Where, and When”. Arch Clin Neuropsychol. 2018;33(2):158-67.10.1093/arclin/acx062Search in Google Scholar PubMed

[43] El Haj M, Allain P. Role of context recall in destination memory decline in normal aging. Geriatr Psychol Neuropsychiatr Vieil. 2014;12(4):432-9.10.1684/pnv.2014.0499Search in Google Scholar PubMed

[44] Svoboda E, McKinnon MC, Levine B. The functional neuroanatomy of autobiographical memory: a meta-analysis. Neuropsychologia. 2006;44(12):2189-208.10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2006.05.023Search in Google Scholar PubMed PubMed Central

[45] St Jacques PL, Carpenter AC, Szpunar KK, Schacter DL. Remembering and imagining alternative versions of the personal past. Neuropsychologia. 2017.10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2017.06.015Search in Google Scholar PubMed PubMed Central

[46] Schaefer A, Philippot P. Selective effects of emotion on the phenomenal characteristics of autobiographical memories. Memory. 2005;13(2):148-60.10.1080/09658210344000648Search in Google Scholar PubMed

[47] Robertson SM, Swickert RJ, Connelly K, Galizio A. Physiological reactivity during autobiographical narratives in older adults: the roles of depression and anxiety. Aging & mental health. 2015;19(8):689-97.10.1080/13607863.2014.962010Search in Google Scholar PubMed

[48] Labouvie-Vief G, Lumley MA, Jain E, Heinze H. Age and gender differences in cardiac reactivity and subjective emotion responses to emotional autobiographical memories. Emotion. 2003;3(2):115-26.10.1037/1528-3542.3.2.115Search in Google Scholar PubMed

[49] El Haj M, Antoine P, Nandrino JL. Facial expressions triggered by imagining the future. J Integr Neurosci. 2017;16(4):483-92.10.3233/JIN-170030Search in Google Scholar PubMed

[50] Gandolphe MC, Nandrino JL, Delelis G, Ducro C, Lavallee A, Saloppe X, et al. Positive facial expressions during retrieval of self-defining memories. J Integr Neurosci. 2017.10.3233/JIN-170073Search in Google Scholar PubMed

[51] El Haj M, Antoine P, Nandrino JL. More emotional facial expressions during episodic than during semantic autobiographical retrieval. Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci. 2016;16(2):374-81.10.3758/s13415-015-0397-9Search in Google Scholar PubMed

[52] El Haj M, Daoudi M, Gallouj K, Moustafa AA, Nandrino J-L. When your face describes your memories: facial expressions during retrieval of autobiographical memories. Rev Neurosci2018.10.1515/revneuro-2018-0001Search in Google Scholar PubMed

[53] Lenoble Q, Janssen SMJ, El Haj M. Don’t stare, unless you don’t want to remember: Maintaining fixation compromises autobiographical memory retrieval. Memory. 2018:1-8.10.1080/09658211.2018.1501068Search in Google Scholar PubMed

[54] El Haj M, Delerue C, Omigie D, Antoine P, Nandrino JL, Boucart M. Autobiographical recall triggers visual exploration. Journal of Eye Movement Research. 2014;7(5):1-7.10.16910/jemr.7.5.1Search in Google Scholar

[55] El Haj M, Nandrino JL, Antoine P, Boucart M, Lenoble Q. Eye movement during retrieval of emotional autobiographical memories. Acta Psychol (Amst). 2017;174:54-8.10.1016/j.actpsy.2017.02.002Search in Google Scholar PubMed

[56] El Haj M, Lenoble Q. Eying the future: Eye movement in past and future thinking. Cortex. 2017.10.1016/j.cortex.2017.05.024Search in Google Scholar PubMed

[57] Babiker A, Faye I, Prehn K, Malik A. Machine Learning to Differentiate Between Positive and Negative Emotions Using Pupil Diameter. Front Psychol. 2015;6(1921).10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01921Search in Google Scholar PubMed PubMed Central

[58] Geangu E, Hauf P, Bhardwaj R, Bentz W. Infant pupil diameter changes in response to others’ positive and negative emotions. PLoS One. 2011;6(11):e27132.10.1371/journal.pone.0027132Search in Google Scholar PubMed PubMed Central

[59] Snowden RJ, O’Farrell KR, Burley D, Erichsen JT, Newton NV, Gray NS. The pupil’s response to affective pictures: Role of image duration, habituation, and viewing mode. Psychophysiology. 2016;53(8):1217-23.10.1111/psyp.12668Search in Google Scholar PubMed PubMed Central

[60] Rieger G, Savin-Williams RC. The Eyes Have It: Sex and Sexual Orientation Differences in Pupil Dilation Patterns. PLoS One. 2012;7(8):e40256.10.1371/journal.pone.0040256Search in Google Scholar PubMed PubMed Central

[61] Burkhouse KL, Owens M, Feurer C, Sosoo E, Kudinova A, Gibb BE. Increased neural and pupillary reactivity to emotional faces in adolescents with current and remitted major depressive disorder. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci. 2017;12(5):783-92.10.1093/scan/nsw184Search in Google Scholar PubMed PubMed Central

[62] Prehn K, Schlagenhauf F, Schulze L, Berger C, Vohs K, Fleischer M, et al. Neural correlates of risk taking in violent criminal offenders characterized by emotional hypo- and hyper-reactivity. Soc Neurosci. 2013;8(2):136-47.10.1080/17470919.2012.686923Search in Google Scholar PubMed

[63] Williams JM, Barnhofer T, Crane C, Herman D, Raes F, Watkins E, et al. Autobiographical memory specificity and emotional disorder. Psychol Bull. 2007;133(1):122-48.10.1037/0033-2909.133.1.122Search in Google Scholar PubMed

[64] Marci CD, Glick DM, Loh R, Dougherty DD. Autonomic and prefrontal cortex responses to autobiographical recall of emotions. Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci. 2007;7(3):243-50.10.3758/CABN.7.3.243Search in Google Scholar PubMed

[65] Gotlib IH, Joormann J. Cognition and depression: current status and future directions. Annu Rev Clin Psychol. 2010;6:285-312.10.1146/annurev.clinpsy.121208.131305Search in Google Scholar PubMed

[66] Dalgleish T, Werner-Seidler A. Disruptions in autobiographical memory processing in depression and the emergence of memory therapeutics. Trends in cognitive sciences. 2014;18(11):596-604.10.1016/j.tics.2014.06.010Search in Google Scholar PubMed

[67] El Haj M, Antoine P, Nandrino JL, Kapogiannis D. Autobiographical memory decline in Alzheimer’s disease, a theoretical and clinical overview. Ageing research reviews. 2015;23(Pt B):183-92.10.1016/j.arr.2015.07.001Search in Google Scholar PubMed

[68] Addis DR, Tippett LJ. Memory of myself: autobiographical memory and identity in Alzheimer’s disease. Memory. 2004;12(1):56-74.10.1080/09658210244000423Search in Google Scholar PubMed

[69] El Haj M, Antoine P. Describe yourself to improve your autobiographical memory: A study in Alzheimer’s disease. Cortex. 2017;88:165-72.10.1016/j.cortex.2017.01.004Search in Google Scholar PubMed

[70] Kopelman MD. Remote and autobiographical memory, temporal context memory and frontal atrophy in Korsakoff and Alzheimer patients. Neuropsychologia. 1989;27(4):437-60.10.1016/0028-3932(89)90050-XSearch in Google Scholar PubMed

[71] Kopelman MD, Kapur N. The loss of episodic memories in retrograde amnesia: single-case and group studies. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci. 2001;356(1413):1409-21.10.1098/rstb.2001.0942Search in Google Scholar PubMed PubMed Central

[72] Rensen YCM, Kessels RPC, Migo EM, Wester AJ, Eling P, Kopelman MD. Personal semantic and episodic autobiographical memories in Korsakoff syndrome: A comparison of interview methods. J Clin Exp Neuropsychol. 2017;39(6):534-46.10.1080/13803395.2016.1248811Search in Google Scholar PubMed

[73] El Haj M, Nandrino JL. Seeing life through rose-colored spectacles: autobiographical memory as experienced in Korsakoff’s syndrome. Conscious Cogn. accepted.10.1016/j.concog.2018.02.007Search in Google Scholar PubMed

[74] El Haj M, Nandrino JL. Phenomenological characteristics of autobiographical memory in Korsakoff’s syndrome. Conscious Cogn. 2017;55(Supplement C):188-96.10.1016/j.concog.2017.08.011Search in Google Scholar PubMed

[75] Noel M, Laroi F, Gallouj K, El Haj M. Confabulations and consciousness in Alzheimer’s disease. J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci. accepted for publication.Search in Google Scholar

[76] El Haj M, Larøi F. Provoked and spontaneous confabulations in Alzheimer’s disease: An examination of their prevalence and relation with general cognitive and executive functioning. Psychiatry Clin Neurosci. 2017;71(1):61-9.10.1111/pcn.12468Search in Google Scholar PubMed

[77] Coltheart M. Confabulation and conversation. Cortex. 2016.10.1016/j.cortex.2016.08.002Search in Google Scholar PubMed

[78] Dalla Barba G, Brazzarola M, Marangoni S, Barbera C, Zannoni I. A longitudinal study of confabulation. Cortex. 2016.10.1016/j.cortex.2016.05.009Search in Google Scholar PubMed

[79] Shakeel MK, Docherty NM, Rich PR, Zaragoza MS, Chrobak QM, McCleery A. Analyzing Confabulations in Schizophrenia and Healthy Participants. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2016;22(9):911-9.10.1017/S1355617716000801Search in Google Scholar PubMed

[80] Dalla Barba G, La Corte V. A neurophenomenological model for the role of the hippocampus in temporal consciousness. Evidence from confabulation. Front Behav Neurosci. 2015;9:218.Search in Google Scholar

[81] Attali E, Dalla Barba G. Confabulation in healthy aging is related to poor encoding and retrieval of over-learned information. Neuropsychol Dev Cogn B Aging Neuropsychol Cogn. 2013;20(3):339-55.10.1080/13825585.2012.711462Search in Google Scholar PubMed

[82] Nahum L, Bouzerda-Wahlen A, Guggisberg A, Ptak R, Schnider A. Forms of confabulation: dissociations and associations. Neuropsychologia. 2012;50(10):2524-34.10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2012.06.026Search in Google Scholar PubMed

[83] La Corte V, George N, Pradat-Diehl P, Barba GD. Distorted temporal consciousness and preserved knowing consciousness in confabulation: a case study. Behav Neurol. 2011;24(4):307-15.10.1155/2011/958359Search in Google Scholar PubMed

[84] Dalla Barba G, Boisse MF. Temporal consciousness and confabulation: is the medial temporal lobe “temporal”? Cogn Neuropsychiatry. 2010;15(1):95-117.10.1080/13546800902758017Search in Google Scholar PubMed

[85] Gilboa A. Strategic retrieval, confabulations, and delusions: theory and data. Cogn Neuropsychiatry. 2010;15(1):145-80.10.1080/13546800903056965Search in Google Scholar PubMed

[86] La Corte V, Serra M, Attali E, Boisse MF, Dalla Barba G. Confabulation in Alzheimer’s disease and amnesia: a qualitative account and a new taxonomy. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2010;16(6):967-74.10.1017/S1355617710001001Search in Google Scholar PubMed

[87] Montefinese M, Ambrosini E, Fairfield B, Mammarella N. The “subjective” pupil old/new effect: Is the truth plain to see? Int J Psychophysiol. 2013;89(1):48-56.10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2013.05.001Search in Google Scholar PubMed

Received: 2019-07-22
Accepted: 2019-09-17
Published Online: 2019-12-19

© 2019 Mohamad EL Haj et al. published by De Gruyter

This work is licensed under the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 Public License.

Downloaded on 22.5.2024 from https://www.degruyter.com/document/doi/10.1515/tnsci-2019-0044/html
Scroll to top button