Skip to main content

REVIEW article

Front. Psychiatry, 28 July 2015
Sec. Child and Adolescent Psychiatry

A Systematic Review of the Evidence for Impaired Cognitive Theory of Mind in Maltreated Children

  • 1Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, Hôpital Pitié-Salpêtrière, Paris, France
  • 2Groupe de Recherches sur l’Analyse Multimodale de la Fonction Cérébrale, INSERM U1105, CHU and Université Picardie Jules Verne, Amiens, France
  • 3Paris-Sud Innovation Group in Adolescent Mental Health, INSERM U669, Maison de Solenn, Paris, France
  • 4UMR 7222, Institute for Intelligent Systems and Robotics, Université Pierre et Marie Curie, Paris, France

Compared to the large number of studies exploring difficulties in emotion recognition in maltreated children, few (N = 12) have explored the cognitive aspect of theory of mind (ToM), i.e., the ability to understand others’ thoughts and intentions. A systematic review of these studies shows inconsistent results regarding cognitive ToM tasks. Youths with a history of maltreatment are more likely to fail at false-belief tasks (N = 2). However, results are less conclusive regarding other tasks (perspective-taking tasks, N = 4; and hostile attribution tasks, N = 7). Additionally, only one study controlled for potential psychopathology. Measures of psychopathology and other cognitive abilities, in addition to ToM, are required to establish a specific association between maltreatment and the cognitive dimension of ToM.

Introduction

In the past few decades, impairments in socio-emotional skills and empathy in children exposed to maltreatment have been recognized with the recent focus on individuals with callous-unemotional traits (1). The association has been claimed to exist even in the absence of psychopathology (2, 3). It has been suggested that a poor level of social understanding would partly explain the problematic peer relationships observed in maltreated children that lead to behavioral problems and peer rejection (3). The successful identification of the mechanisms underlying poor social abilities in maltreated children is essential to design specific interventions for this population. In this study, we explore the possible implications of one specific dimension of social cognition, the cognitive aspect of theory of mind (ToM), in the difficulties reported in maltreated children.

Theory of mind is defined as the awareness that others have a mind with various mental states (e.g., beliefs, desires, imagination, and emotion) that may differ from one’s own (4). It is one of the subcomponents of social cognition, i.e., the set of mental operations that underlie the ability to interact and communicate in accordance with social norms, values, and expectations (5). ToM is closely related to the development of empathy, the intuitive access to others’ subjective experiences (6). It denotes the capacity to understand others’ intentions and experience their feelings (6). Considering the importance of empathy in individuals’ ability to interpret socially relevant information, individual differences in ToM have important implications for social communication abilities (7, 8).

Theory of mind appears around age 4 in typically developing children and progresses from simple to more complex forms (5, 6, 9, 10). Infants’ early social behaviors rely on procedural empathy, an innate, non-verbal capacity to resonate with others’ emotional states (6). Procedural empathy encompasses three processes: (i) sensorimotor resonance and imitation, which are present in neonates (11); (ii) emotional mimicry (sympathy), when the subjectively experienced emotion is similar to the one observed in others (12); (iii) empathetic concern resulting from the attachment system, which develops between the infant and his/her primary caregivers (5). These precursors of ToM provide the bases for non-verbal communication between the child and his caregiver, which involve synchrony and reciprocity (13). Joint attention is present in children at 3 months and facilitates the ability to share a common point of reference, and then make inferences about others’ behaviors (5, 10). To predict and explain others’ behavior also implies the development of perspective-taking skills (e.g., the capacity to represent another person’s visual perspective) (12, 14, 15). In the second and third years, the ability to communicate with others firmly increases through the capacity to label emotions and concepts. Before the full development of ToM, language emergence paves the way for the development of a higher form of empathy – semantic empathy – that depends on verbal thought (16, 17). It involves different aptitudes: self-awareness (i.e., our own mental states are distinct from those of others) (18), the production of internal state words (19, 20), and the development of more general advances in symbolic maturity (21). More sophisticated interpersonal negotiation strategies continue to improve during adolescence and early adulthood. Biographical empathy, the capacity to bridge with others’ experiences, emerges later in life and corresponds to the interweaving of personal experience with feelings and words (6). Compared to procedural empathy, semantic and autobiographic forms of empathy may be more sensitive to childhood experience.

Empirical studies have noted that ToM development is strongly affected by non-heritable and environmental factors, such as parental practices (2226), parental conversational elaboration (22), or the presence of siblings (27, 28). For example, Pavarini et al. found that ToM ability emerges earlier in families where: (i) children are considered to be intentional agents in verbal exchanges (e.g., using the second personal pronoun); (ii) mental states language is used (e.g., pointing out the causes and consequences of intentions, desires, or beliefs); and (iii) children are exposed to a wide range of emotions. As maltreating families are frequently characterized by disorganized interaction and poor parenting (29, 30), it would be expected that ToM difficulties occur in children from these families.

A growing literature reports impairments in social abilities in maltreated children compared to non-maltreated children (3). However, the mechanisms underpinning such difficulties remain incompletely understood. Despite a large number of studies focusing on maltreated children’s difficulties recognizing others’ emotions, few studies have explored their capacity to infer others’ thoughts or intentions (3, 31). There is indeed growing evidence that the recognition of another’s emotions and thoughts, respectively, referred to as affective ToM (or “hot” ToM) and cognitive ToM (or “cold” ToM), are related but distinct domains (3235). Such capacities would depend in part on separate anatomical substrates (36, 37). Affective and cognitive ToM may be independently impaired in individuals with psychopathology (1, 6, 36, 38) or neurologic diseases (39). To distinguish cognitive and affective ToM, help to map specific domains of cognitive development altered in maltreated children. In doing so, it would provide possible mechanisms by which these children are at higher risk of developing severe behavioral disturbances and psychopathology (3). In particular, a better knowledge of these mediators is a major prerequisite for developing preventive interventions (30).

Our aim in this paper is to conduct a critical review examining the objective evidence for a specific impairment in cognitive ToM in maltreated children.

Materials and Methods

A systematic review was conducted, following the recommendations outlined in the PRISMA guide (40). The distinction between the behavioral tasks measuring cognitive and affective ToM, proposed by Henry et al. (33) and Brothers and Ring (32), were used to determine keywords. Cognitive ToM tasks are those requiring an understanding of others’ beliefs, thoughts, or intentions; affective ToM tasks typically ask participants to describe a protagonist’s emotion. The following terms were used in the literature search: social cognition or empathy or theory of mind or false-belief or social perception or social knowledge or attributional bias and child abuse or child maltreatment or early trauma or early adverse events or sexual abuse or physical abuse or emotional abuse or neglect. Three online databases – PsychInfo, PubMed, and Scopus – were used to identify relevant records on the basis of these search criteria. All articles published between 1980 and July 2014 were considered for inclusion in this review. All records resulting from the initial search were screened according to a priori inclusion criteria (Figure 1). These criteria were:

• Use of a task to measure cognitive ToM (i.e., attribution of intentions or beliefs). By contrast, studies where only emotional ToM was measured (i.e., attribution of emotion or feelings) were not selected. We used the distinction proposed by Henry et al. (33) and Luke and Banerjee (3) between ToM tasks measuring cognitive ToM, affective ToM, or both.

• Assessment of at least one of the following widely recognized forms of childhood maltreatment (41): physical abuse, sexual abuse, emotional abuse, emotional negligence, or physical negligence. Moreover, studies that explored the broader area of parenting style in relation to ToM as well as other forms of childhood adversity, such as parental loss or separation, bullying, or a wide range of early stressful life events, were considered to be outside the scope of this review.

• Studies conducted in children or in which a subgroup of children was analyzed. Studies were not selected if the results were only presented for childhood and adulthood together. Studies in adolescents only (older than 13 years old) were not selected.

• Presence of a comparison group in the study design. We did not retain studies in which maltreatment were explored as a covariate, without presenting data separately for individuals with and without a history of maltreatment.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. PRISMA diagram of screening process and study selection.

The relevant findings were extracted from the final set of papers by the first two authors. To clarify the presentation of results, we used the distinction proposed by Luke and Banerjee (3); i.e., results are separated into three main sections according to the tasks used: the false-belief task, the perspective-taking task, and measures of attributional bias. The two others tasks proposed by Luke and Banerjee (3) (i.e., emotion understanding and emotion recognition) were not explored here, as they refer to the affective, not the cognitive, aspect of ToM.

Results

In total, 12 studies were included in this review. Sample characteristics for each study are shown in Tables 13. It was not possible to perform any meta-analysis of the data generated by the current review due to the small number of studies and lack of task replication across studies. Therefore, a descriptive synthesis of the available data is provided according to the tasks involved.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Details of studies included in the review for false-belief tasks.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Details of studies included in the review for perspective-taking tasks.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Details of studies included in the review for hostile attribution tasks.

False-Belief Tasks

The classic “false-belief task” was initially a story acted out with dolls and props in the Sally and Anne Test (4). A first-order task that requires understanding of another person’s mental state is passed by 3–4 years of age for a typically developing child (9). Second-order false-belief tasks involve understanding what two people think sequentially, for example, “Pierre thinks what Paul thinks”; such tasks are usually passed by ~6 years of age. Control tasks include true belief trials, where the reality of a situation can be solved without any thought inference. Language development, age, and socioeconomic status (SES) have been empirically linked to children’s understanding of false-beliefs (33). Modality of task presentation differed between studies: one study used video to present the story while another used cartoons and verbal narrative.

Two studies compared false-beliefs tasks between maltreated and non-maltreated children. Findings from these two studies are consistent; children with a history of maltreatment showed poorer performance on false-belief tasks compared with their peers. Cicchetti et al. (42) showed the performance on two different unexpected-content tasks in a group of 4–8-year-olds, maltreated and non-maltreated children. They found that physically, sexually, or emotionally abused children had lower scores than non-maltreated peers. This would especially concern those youths for whom maltreatment had begun the earliest. Pears and Fisher (43) found that success rate at a discrepant belief tasks were lower among abused or neglected 3–5-year-olds compared to non-maltreated children. However, in this study, the principal outcome was a combined measure of four different tasks (i.e., a false-belief task, a perspective-taking task, a desire understanding task, and an appearance–reality task) (54). In these two studies, results persisted after controlling for chronological age, intelligence quotient (IQ) (WPPSI-R score), and SES.

Perspective-Taking Tasks

Perspective-taking tests require participants to adopt a third person’s perspective while making spatial judgments (i.e., visual perspective taking) or retelling a story from another’s point of view (i.e., conceptual perspective-taking) (12, 55).

Only one study explored visual perspective-taking in maltreated children (43). Pears and Fisher (43) found that children in foster care had difficulties performing ToM tasks, after controlling for age, intelligence, and executive function. As the main outcome was a combined measure (including a false-belief task, a two-level perspective-taking task, a desire understanding task and an understanding of appearance–reality task), it was difficult to discern which specific domains were impeded.

Two out of three studies showed poor conceptual perspective-taking skills in maltreated children who were asked to retell stories from a different point of view. Barahal et al. (44) explored this ability among a group of physically abused 6–8-year-olds. Abused children, compared to non-abused peers, more frequently continued to believe that others would describe stories similar to their own. Even after they knew that important story components had been removed in a boy–dog–tree task, 35% of the abused children maintained persistently egocentric views. This finding was no longer significant with IQ controlled. Burack et al. (46) found a higher level of egocentrism among abused or neglected 7–12-year-olds compared to controls using the Chandler’s Bystander Cartoons Test. This association was independent of self-esteem and externalizing and internalizing symptoms as measured by the Child Behavior Check List (CBCL). In contrast, Walker and Downey (45) sample of neglected 9-year-olds, compared to non-maltreated peers, showed impairments in perspective-taking tasks; however, performances of the two groups were comparable after controlling for gender, parental psychopathology, IQ (WISC-R), and age. The fact that the control group was partly recruited from local inpatient and outpatient facilities makes it difficult to interpret the data.

Social Attribution Tasks

Except for one study (50), social attributions have been assessed using a series of vignettes adopted from the work of Crick and Dodge (56) on social information processing in children. This task presents hypothetical situations depicting a child experiencing some type of negative outcome in a social situation where the intentions of the other person in the story are ambiguous. In these types of situations, children must rely on their internal mental representations to guide their interpretation of the other’s intentions.

Three out of seven studies found a hostile attribution bias in maltreated children when asked to attribute intent for negative acts in response to ambiguous scenarios. Dodge et al. (57) showed that physically abused children had more encoding errors, more hostile attributional biases, more accessing of aggressive response to peers, and more positive evaluation of the outcomes of aggression compared to non-abused children. As aggressive behaviors were measured through the CBCL teacher report in the same sample 4 years later, a mediation model was built in which difficulties in social attribution explained 33% of the effect of a history of abuse on the onset of aggressive behavior (48). The authors conceded that the lack of evaluation of IQ reduced the scope of the study. Ayoub et al. (50) explored the ability to retell complex stories upon different emotional contexts in abused and neglected 3–6-year-old children. They found that the maltreated children have more difficulty representing positive social interactions, although they were as good as non-maltreated peers at retelling stories about negative interactions. The authors suggested that maltreated children’s abilities to think with cognitive complexity is sustained but applied differentially to situations based on emotional valence. Keil and Price (53) found that 5–8-year-old children who had been neglected and physically abused were more likely to attribute hostile intents in response to videotaped vignettes about peer provocation compared to non-maltreated peers.

Three studies provided mixed evidence regarding hostile attributions in maltreated children. Price and Glad (49) found that 4–6-year-old boys who had been physically abused or who witnessed domestic violence were more likely to attribute hostile intentions to a variety of figures, including their parents, an unfamiliar teacher, their best friend, and unfamiliar peers. The effect increased with the frequency of physical abuse. However, this result was not found for girls. The authors proposed that physically abused girls who participated in this investigation experienced less frequent and severe forms of physical abuse than did the boys; an alternative hypothesis is that boys are more likely to be involved in social interactions that contribute to the development and maintenance of hostile attributions after abuse. Mediation analysis found these children’s hostile attributions of their mothers mediated the relationship between physical abuse and children’s hostile attributions of unfamiliar peers. Teisl and Cicchetti’s (51) sample of physically abused 6–12-year-olds gave no more hostile attributions to ambiguous vignettes than did non-maltreated children. Even if overall group differences were not significant, authors noted that physically abused children were more likely to incorrectly interpret prosocial and accidental scenarios as hostile and were more likely to have aggressive responses to provocation (after controlling for age, gender, and race/ethnicity). Sabourin Ward and Haskett (52) found no difference between physically abused 5–10-year-olds and non-maltreated children in their understanding of interpersonal situations. Using person-centered method, authors identified two distinct profiles among youths with little prosocial behavior. Youths with little prosocial and little maladjusted behavior, e.g., rarely rejected by peers (“Hanging in There” group), more frequently reported hostile attributions of intent in response to vignettes than those with little prosocial behavior and high social maladjustment (“Social Difficulties” group). Walker and Downey (45) sample of physically abused or neglected 7–14-year-olds did not face more difficulties to interpret interpersonal relationships compared to peers.

Discussion

Study Limitations

There were several limitations in the available data that preclude us from drawing firm conclusions with regard to the impact of maltreatment on cognitive ToM in children.

The first limitation concerns the measure of maltreatment. Indeed, the evaluation of child maltreatment has been regarded as an important issue in previous reviews in Ref. (30, 41, 58). Maltreatment is defined as any acts of commission or omission by a parent or other caregiver that result in harm, potential for harm, or threat of harm to a child (41). Four forms of maltreatment are widely recognized: physical abuse, sexual abuse, psychological (or emotional) abuse, and neglect. Despite the development of consensual definitions and standardized approaches to explore and quantify maltreatment, some types of abuse, especially neglect and emotional abuse, remain difficult to evaluate (30, 42). In our review, five studies used the Maltreatment Classification System proposed by Barnett (42, 4951, 53, 59). In four studies, the severity and frequency of the abuse and the identity of the abuser were explored, but this information was only reported in two studies (44, 53), likely because the sample sizes were too low to reach statistical significance in most of the analyses. It should also be noted that most studies included no mention of whether assessors were blind to children’s maltreatment status or not.

The second limitation is the lack of reporting on psychopathological symptoms in the samples studied. Only one study reported psychiatric comorbidities and controlled for psychopathologic score when examining the relationship between maltreatment and cognitive ToM (46). Altered social cognitions have been reported in children suffering from internalized and externalized psychiatric disorders (1, 60, 61). Considering the high prevalence of psychiatric comorbidities in maltreated children (30, 31, 34, 62), extreme caution is required in interpreting the results of studies without any measure of psychopathology. The question of whether cognitive ToM impairment occurs in maltreated children, even in the absence of psychopathology, has significant implications for therapeutic approaches. If this assumption is confirmed, interventions aiming at promoting social abilities in maltreated children by facilitating cognitive ToM abilities should not only focus on children with psychiatric symptoms. To answer this question, psychopathology, in addition to cognitive skills, should be assessed and reported in future research. Then, follow-up studies could help to determine to what extend deficits in cognitive ToM in maltreated children led to the development of overt cognitive, affective, or behavioral disturbance found in psychopathology.

The third limitation is the cross-sectional design of most of the studies reviewed. Indeed, the relationship emphasized in cross-sectional studies could be interpreted in the opposite direction, such as, children with poor cognitive ToM may be more frequently involved in intra-familial abuse (63). For example, in a large community-based sample, Shakoor et al. (63) showed that poor ToM predicted becoming a victim of bullying in early adolescence. In the same way, Sullivan and Knutson (64) noted that an increased prevalence of maltreatment in children with disabilities was associated with lower social cognition abilities. In this study, the risk of enduring all types of maltreatment was four times higher in children with mental retardation, and five times higher in children with speech and language impairments. Other longitudinal studies would be useful to explore the direction of the relationship between cognitive ToM and maltreatment.

The fourth limitation is the wide disparity in the composition of control groups (i.e., non-maltreated children) used in the studies. Maltreated children often experience multiple adversities, including poverty, parental psychopathology, or parental unemployment, which correlate with lower level social-cognitive development (30). For example, language, which is an essential factor for the emergence of a ToM (17), is strongly associated with the SES of the families (65). When examining the specific impact of maltreatment on social-cognitive abilities, the control group should be as similar as possible to the maltreated group on all factors associated with ToM. In three studies from the current review, the control group was composed of children whose families did not have prior contact with social service agencies (43, 49, 53). By contrast, Ayoub et al. (50), Cicchetti et al. (42), and Barahal et al. (44) compared maltreated youth with children whose families were receiving public assistance but who screened negatively for maltreatment. In the latter case, it may be more difficult to find a significant difference between the maltreated and control groups. Therefore, the positive findings reported by Pears and Fisher (43), Price and Glad (49), and Keil and Price (53) should be interpreted with care. However, the majority of the studies discussed here matched their maltreatment group and control groups on other factors known to affect social understanding, such as age and SES (46). Burack et al. (46) also controlled for children’s IQ. As an alternative, Cicchetti et al. (42) proposed a stratified sampling between three groups to independently explore the effect of maltreatment and SES on performance of a false-belief task.

Summary of the Main Results

The poorer performance in false-belief tasks in maltreated children was consistent between studies (42, 43). However, results were less consensual regarding perspective-taking tasks. Only one study found specific difficulties in perspective-taking tests in maltreated children (46). The difference showed by Barahal et al. (44) between maltreated and non-maltreated youths did not persist after controlling for IQ, and one study found no difference (45). Findings regarding hostile attribution bias in maltreated children were also mixed. Dodge et al. (48) and Keil and Price (53) found a significant difference in the level of hostile attribution between maltreated and non-maltreated children, whereas Price and Glad (49), Teisl and Cicchetti (51), and Sabourin Ward and Haskett (52) did not report such difference. Two studies were difficult to interpret because of the task used to examine attributional bias (50) and the severity of the control group (47).

Interpretation

The current findings suggest that there is limited available evidence of poorer cognitive ToM in child victims of maltreatment. In addition, we noted a large divergence in findings depending on how cognitive ToM was measured.

In comparison with non-maltreated children, youths with a history of maltreatment showed more difficulties with false-belief tasks. Some authors concluded that difficulties in cognitive ToM were independent of other deficits in cognitive functions; however, few studies controlled for the potential impact of other cognitive functions on their results. Controlling for cognitive deficits is important, as previous reports have noted that false-belief tasks involve other cognitive abilities (9) such as working memory, joint attention, or executive function (10). Therefore, an independent effect of maltreatment on the ability to understand other’s thoughts or intentions is consistent with the model of empathy proposed by Guile (6). In this model, the development of more elaborate forms of empathy continues throughout childhood and adolescence (i.e., “semantic” and “biographic” empathy) depending on the environmental context.

Discrepancies between studies exploring perspective-taking have been noted in our review. These discrepancies may be partly explained by the diversity in the tasks used and the age of the participants. First, differences in the level of inappropriate egocentric thinking between maltreated and non-maltreated youths was more marked in adolescents than in school-aged children according to Burack et al. (46). The lack of significant results in studies exploring conceptual perspective-taking tasks could be explained by the young age of the sample studied. Future studies should explore these results in distinct child and adolescent samples. Second, from our findings, it is still unclear whether spatial perspective-taking is impaired in maltreated children, considering the lack of specific measurement in the study of Pears and Fisher (43). This final assumption is supported by growing empirical studies exploring the relationship between visuo-spatial mechanisms and empathy in healthy children. Thirioux et al. (12) suggested that the cognitive component of empathy refers to a controlled process whereby individuals understand the mental states of others while adopting their psychological viewpoint. Therefore, attribution of the other’s experience to oneself requires, among other skills, visual perspective-change by the child to enable mental imagery in a mirror-like manner (i.e., heterocentered visuo-spatial) (12). Further studies should explore whether poor ability to develop visuo-spatial perspective may be one process by which empathy is disturbed in maltreated children.

Studies where social attribution bias in maltreated children was examined showed heterogeneous results. Our review supports a gender difference in the effect of maltreatment on hostile attribution bias that may explain such discrepancies. Price and Glad (49) found a difference in hostile attribution only in boys, but not in girls, with a history of maltreatment. Such difference is consistent with the works of Teisl and Cicchetti (51) who reported a higher level of hostile attribution in maltreated boys compared to girls. Moreover, the subgroup of maltreated children with the highest level of hostile attribution in the cluster analysis proposed by Sabourin Ward and Haskett (52) also had the greatest proportion of males compared to other subgroups. Although some studies reported outcomes for each gender separately, this was not the case for all papers. This circumstance may be a source of bias for three main reasons. (i) Findings indicate that some forms of abuse may be more common among women than men (41, 66), and perhaps only specific type of abuse have consequences for ToM (49). (ii) Abuse could have greater association with psychopathology and social impact according to gender (49). For example, compared to boys, disruptive disorders in girls with a history of physical abuse are less frequent but are associated with poorer outcome (e.g., more functional impairment and social consequences) (66). (iii) Development of ToM could differ between girls and boys, for example, on the age required to pass specific ToM tasks (33). In this context, future studies should take into account a possible gender difference in exploring the effect of maltreatment on hostile attribution.

Moreover, interpretation of studies concerning social attribution in maltreated children should also take into consideration that such tasks are strongly related to emotional understanding ability, i.e., the affective component of ToM. Indeed, recognition of affective states emerges before the child begins to understand their causes, and development of the latter may be more susceptible to the deleterious effects of maltreatment (67). Teisl and Cicchetti (51) showed that the association between hostile attribution and behavior problems in physically abused children was no longer significant when controlling for emotion regulation. Difficulties in emotion recognition and understanding observed in child victims of abuse or maltreatment may affect social information processing and contribute to social attribution bias, compared to children in non-maltreated context (3). For example, Pollak et al. (68) found that physically abused children showed a bias toward anger when reacting to ambiguous stimuli. Co-occurring deficits in emotion regulation and in ToM observed in maltreated children families may operate through poor child–caregiver interactions (31, 69). For example, Meins et al. (23) suggested that the development of a secure attachment facilitates the mentalizing ability (i.e., the process of making sense of one’s own and other’s mental states), which plays a role both in the capacity for understanding others’ intents and feelings but also in understanding and regulating one’s emotion. Therefore, disorganized or insecure attachment may impede the development of the mentalizing skill involved in both cognitive and affective ToM (7072).

Clinical Implications and Direction for Future Research

We have highlighted the importance of considering multiple domains of development in exploring cognitive ToM in maltreated children and of controlling for psychopathology and contributing cognitive abilities (e.g., working memory, joint attention, or executive function). Only then, could we precisely answer whether there are difficulties in cognitive ToM abilities in abused children, and, if so, how these difficulties are integrated to the more general social-cognitive impairments found in these children. Studies are needed to map the dynamic interactions between social-cognitive development and other cognitive abilities from early infancy to late adolescence. As illustrated with the model of empathy that distinguishes procedural, semantic, and autobiographic dimensions (6), the emergence of social cognition is affected by factors occurring at different sensitive periods throughout child development. Minor impairments in social-cognitive abilities may only become apparent in adolescence. It is therefore important that studies could be conducted in youth using standardized cognitive tasks across various age ranges.

Studies with large sample sizes and repeated measures of cognitive abilities (e.g., ToM and social attribution), psychopathology, and general functioning (including social performance) collected from infancy to late adolescence would be of great value. Based on these data, path analysis could be performed to test whether, and to what extent, cognitive difficulties mediate or moderate the risk for poor outcomes (i.e., psychopathology and social adaptation) in maltreated youth.

The resulting findings may, in turn, support the development and evaluation of treatment approaches for abused children that target social and emotional processing. First, studies in maltreated youth could help determine the feasibility and benefits of specific therapeutic interventions on social cognition (e.g., cognitive remediation). Second, future research should also focus on the environmental (e.g., foster care conditions) and therapeutic interventions that might moderate the long-term effects of maltreatment on social cognition (30, 41, 58). This is particularly important as poorly thought out social interventions (e.g., multiple changes in the child’s placement and early attempts at reunifying the child with his/her biological family) increase the risk of developing poor psychosocial outcomes (41).

Conclusion

Considering the limited available data, it was not possible to draw any firm conclusions about the association between childhood maltreatment and direct impairments in the cognitive component of ToM. Results were inconsistent with variations according to the tasks used; however, preliminary evidence supports difficulties in performance on false-beliefs tasks among child victims of maltreatment. Moreover, many studies did not control for psychopathology, making the results more difficult to interpret. Models linking cognitive ToM and emotion regulation via possible mediators or moderators (e.g., mentalization or attachment) need to be tested in future research, preferably using prospective designs.

Author Contributions

Substantial contributions to the conception and design of the work: XB, J-MG, and DC. Substantial contributions to the acquisition, analysis, or interpretation of data: XB and AC. Drafting the work or revising it critically for important intellectual content: XB, AC, J-MG, and DC. Final approval of the version to be published: J-MG, AC, and DC. Agreement to be accountable for all aspects of the work in ensuring that questions related to the accuracy or integrity of any part of the work are appropriately investigated and resolved: XB, AC, J-MG, and DC.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Acknowledgments

The authors are grateful to Miss Babilaëre for her assistance.

References

1. Sebastian CL, McCrory EJ, Cecil CA, Lockwood PL, De Brito SA, Fontaine NM, et al. Neural responses to affective and cognitive theory of mind in children with conduct problems and varying levels of callous-unemotional traits. Arch Gen Psychiatry (2012) 69(8):814–22. doi: 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2011.2070

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

2. Green AH. Psychopathology of abused children. J Am Acad Child Psychiatry (1978) 17(1):92–103. doi:10.1016/S0002-7138(09)62281-0

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

3. Luke N, Banerjee R. Differentiated associations between childhood maltreatment experiences and social understanding: a meta-analysis and systematic review. Dev Rev (2013) 33:1–28. doi:10.1016/j.dr.2012.10.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

4. Baron-Cohen S. Mindblindness: An Essay on Autism and Theory of Mind. (Vol. xxii). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press (1995). 171 p.

Google Scholar

5. Decety J, Lamm C. Human empathy through the lens of social neuroscience. ScientificWorldJournal (2006) 6:1146–63. doi:10.1100/tsw.2006.221

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

6. Guile JM. Probabilistic perception, empathy, and dynamic homeostasis: insights in autism spectrum disorders and conduct disorders. Front Public Health (2014) 2:4. doi:10.3389/fpubh.2014.00004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

7. Cutting AL, Dunn J. Conversations with siblings and with friends: links between relationship quality and social understanding. Br J Dev Psychol (2006) 24:73–87. doi:10.1348/026151005X70337

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

8. Watson AC, Nixon CL, Wilson A, Capage L. Social interaction skills and theory of mind in young children. Dev Psychol (1999) 35(2):386–91. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.35.2.386

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

9. Wellman HM, Cross D, Watson J. Meta-analysis of theory-of-mind development: the truth about false belief. Child Dev (2001) 72(3):655–84. doi:10.1111/1467-8624.00304

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

10. Korkmaz B. Theory of mind and neurodevelopmental disorders of childhood. Pediatr Res (2011) 69(5 Pt 2):101R–8R. doi:10.1203/PDR.0b013e318212c177

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

11. Meltzoff AN, Moore MK. Explaining facial imitation: a theoretical model. Early Dev Parent (1997) 6(3–4):179–92. doi:10.1002/(SICI)1099-0917(199709/12)6:3/4<179::AID-EDP157>3.3.CO;2-I

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

12. Thirioux B, Mercier MR, Blanke O, Berthoz A. The cognitive and neural time course of empathy and sympathy: an electrical neuroimaging study on self-other interaction. Neuroscience (2014) 267:286–306. doi:10.1016/j.neuroscience.2014.02.024

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

13. Leclere C, Viaux S, Avril M, Achard C, Chetouani M, Missonnier S, et al. Why synchrony matters during mother-child interactions: a systematic review. PLoS One (2014) 9(12):e113571. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0113571

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

14. Sodian B, Thoermer C, Metz U. Now i see it but you don’t: 14-month-olds can represent another person’s visual perspective. Dev Sci (2007) 10(2):199–204. doi:10.1111/j.1467-7687.2007.00580.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

15. Xavier J, Tilmont E, Bonnot O. Children’s synchrony and rhythmicity in imitation of peers: toward a developmental model of empathy. J Physiol Paris (2013) 107(4):291–7. doi:10.1016/j.jphysparis.2013.03.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

16. De Villiers J. Language and understanding minds: connections in mind. In: Baron-Cohen S, Tager-Flusberg H, Cohen DJ, editors. Understanding Other Minds: Perspectives from Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience. New York, NY: Oxford University Press (2000). p. 83–123.

Google Scholar

17. Astington JW, Jenkins JM. A longitudinal study of the relation between language and theory-of-mind development. Dev Psychol (1999) 35(5):1311–20. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.35.5.1311

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

18. Frith CD, Blakemore SJ, Wolpert DM. Abnormalities in the awareness and control of action. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci (2000) 355(1404):1771–88. doi:10.1098/rstb.2000.0734

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

19. Bretherton I, Beeghly M. Talking about internal states: the acquisition of an explicit theory of mind. Dev Psychol (1982) 18:906–21. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.18.6.906

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

20. Cicchetti D, Ganiban J, Barnett D. Contributions from the Study of High Risk Populations to Understanding the Development of Emotion Regulation. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press (1991).

Google Scholar

21. Bloom L. Language Development: From Two to Three. New York, NY: Cambridge University Press (1991).

Google Scholar

22. Dunn J, Brown J, Slomkowski C, Tesla C, Youngblade L. Young children’s understanding of other people’s feelings and beliefs: individual differences and their antecedents. Child Dev (1991) 62(6):1352–66. doi:10.2307/1130811

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

23. Meins E, Fernyhough C, Wainwright R, Das Gupta M, Fradley E, Tuckey M. Maternal mind-mindedness and attachment security as predictors of theory of mind understanding. Child Dev (2002) 73(6):1715–26. doi:10.1111/1467-8624.00501

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

24. Hughes C, Ensor R. Executive function and theory of mind: predictive relations from ages 2 to 4. Dev Psychol (2007) 43(6):1447–59. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.43.6.1447

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

25. Ontai LL, Thompson RA. Attachment, parent-child discourse and theory of mind development. Soc Dev (2008) 17:47–60. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9507.2007.00414.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

26. Pavarini G, de Hollanda Souza D, Kozak Hawk C. Parental practices and theory of mind development. J Child Fam Stud (2013) 22:844–53. doi:10.1007/s10826-012-9643-8

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

27. McAlister A, Peterson C. A longitudinal study of child siblings and theory of mind development. Cogn Dev (2007) 22:258–70. doi:10.1016/j.cogdev.2006.10.009

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

28. Lewis C, Freeman NH, Kyriakidou C, Maridaki-Kassotaki K, Berridge DM. Social influences on false belief access: specific sibling influences or general apprenticeship? Child Dev (1996) 67(6):2930–47. doi:10.2307/1131760

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

29. Rogosch FA, Cicchetti D, Shields A, Toth SL. Parenting dysfunction in child maltreatment. In: Bornstein MH, editor. Handbook of Parenting. (Vol. 4), Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum (1995). p. 127–59.

Google Scholar

30. Benarous X, Raffin M, Consoli A, Cohen D. Abuse, maltreatment and neglect: (1) epidemiology and long-term consequences. Neuropsychiatr Enfance Adolesc (2014) 62:299–312. doi:10.1016/j.neurenf.2014.04.005

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

31. Dvir Y, Ford JD, Hill M, Frazier JA. Childhood maltreatment, emotional dysregulation, and psychiatric comorbidities. Harv Rev Psychiatry (2014) 22(3):149–61. doi:10.1097/HRP.0000000000000014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

32. Brothers L, Ring B. A neuroethological framework for the representation of minds. J Cogn Neurosci (1992) 4(2):107–18. doi:10.1162/jocn.1992.4.2.107

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

33. Henry JD, Phillips LH, Ruffman T, Bailey PE. A meta-analytic review of age differences in theory of mind. Psychol Aging (2013) 28(3):826–39. doi:10.1037/a0030677

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

34. Blair RJ. Responding to the emotions of others: dissociating forms of empathy through the study of typical and psychiatric populations. Conscious Cogn (2005) 14(4):698–718. doi:10.1016/j.concog.2005.06.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

35. Green MF, Penn DL, Bentall R, Carpenter WT, Gaebel W, Gur RC, et al. Social cognition in schizophrenia: an NIMH workshop on definitions, assessment, and research opportunities. Schizophr Bull (2008) 34(6):1211–20. doi:10.1093/schbul/sbm145

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

36. Shamay-Tsoory SG, Harari H, Aharon-Peretz J, Levkovitz Y. The role of the orbitofrontal cortex in affective theory of mind deficits in criminal offenders with psychopathic tendencies. Cortex (2010) 46(5):668–77. doi:10.1016/j.cortex.2009.04.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

37. Schlaffke L, Lissek S, Lenz M, Juckel G, Schultz T, Tegenthoff M, et al. Shared and nonshared neural networks of cognitive and affective theory-of-mind: a neuroimaging study using cartoon picture stories. Hum Brain Mapp (2015) 36(1):29–39. doi:10.1002/hbm.22610

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

38. Jones AP, Happe FG, Gilbert F, Burnett S, Viding E. Feeling, caring, knowing: different types of empathy deficit in boys with psychopathic tendencies and autism spectrum disorder. J Child Psychol Psychiatry (2010) 51(11):1188–97. doi:10.1111/j.1469-7610.2010.02280.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

39. Roca M, Manes F, Gleichgerrcht E, Ibanez A, Gonzalez de Toledo ME, Marenco V, et al. Cognitive but not affective theory of mind deficits in mild relapsing-remitting multiple sclerosis. Cogn Behav Neurol (2014) 27(1):25–30. doi:10.1097/WNN.0000000000000017

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

40. Moher D, Liberati A, Tetzlaff J, Altman DG, Group P. Preferred reporting items for systematic reviews and meta-analyses: the PRISMA statement. BMJ (2009) 339:b2535. doi:10.1136/bmj.b2535

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

41. Gilbert R, Widom CS, Browne K, Fergusson D, Webb E, Janson S. Burden and consequences of child maltreatment in high-income countries. Lancet (2009) 373(9657):68–81. doi:10.1016/S0140-6736(08)61706-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

42. Cicchetti D, Rogosch FA, Maughan A, Toth SL, Bruce J. False belief understanding in maltreated children. Dev Psychopathol (2003) 15(4):1067–91. doi:10.1017/S0954579403000440

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

43. Pears KC, Fisher PA. Emotion understanding and theory of mind among maltreated children in foster care: evidence of deficits. Dev Psychopathol (2005) 17(1):47–65. doi:10.1017/S0954579405050030

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

44. Barahal RM, Waterman J, Martin HP. The social cognitive development of abused children. J Consult Clin Psychol (1981) 49(4):508–16. doi:10.1037/0022-006X.49.4.508

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

45. Walker EF, Downey G. The effects of familial risk factors on social-cognitive abilities in children. Child Psychiatry Hum Dev (1990) 20(4):253–67. doi:10.1007/BF00706018

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

46. Burack JA, Flanagan T, Peled T, Sutton HM, Zygmuntowicz C, Manly JT. Social perspective-taking skills in maltreated children and adolescents. Dev Psychol (2006) 42(2):207–17. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.42.2.207

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

47. Downey G, Walker E. Social cognition and adjustment in children at risk for psychopathology. Dev Psychol (1989) 25:835–45. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.25.5.835

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

48. Dodge KA, Pettit GS, Bates JE, Valente E. Social information-processing patterns partially mediate the effect of early physical abuse on later conduct problems. J Abnorm Psychol (1995) 104:632–43. doi:10.1037/0021-843X.104.4.632

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

49. Price JM, Glad K. Hostile attributional tendencies in maltreated children. J Abnorm Child Psychol (2003) 31(3):329–43. doi:10.1023/A:1023237731683

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

50. Ayoub CC, O’Connor E, Rappolt-Schlichtmann G, Fischer KW, Rogosch FA, Toth SL, et al. Cognitive and emotional differences in young maltreated children: a translational application of dynamic skill theory. Dev Psychopathol (2006) 18(3):679–706. doi:10.1017/S0954579406060342

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

51. Teisl M, Cicchetti D. Physical abuse, cognitive and emotional processes, and aggressive/disruptive behavior problems. Soc Dev (2008) 17:1–23. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9507.2007.00412.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

52. Sabourin Ward C, Haskett ME. Exploration and validation of clusters of physically abused children. Child Abuse Negl (2008) 32(5):577–88. doi:10.1016/j.chiabu.2007.07.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

53. Keil V, Price JM. Social information-processing patterns of maltreated children in two social domains. J Appl Dev Psychol (2009) 30:43–52. doi:10.1016/j.appdev.2008.10.003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

54. Happe FG. The role of age and verbal ability in the theory of mind task performance of subjects with autism. Child Dev (1995) 66(3):843–55. doi:10.2307/1131954

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

55. Ruby P, Decety J. What you believe versus what you think they believe: a neuroimaging study of conceptual perspective-taking. Eur J Neurosci (2003) 17(11):2475–80. doi:10.1046/j.1460-9568.2003.02673.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

56. Crick NR, Dodge KA. A review and reformulation of social information-processing mechanisms in children’s social adjustment. Psychol Bull (1994) 115:74–101. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.115.1.74

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

57. Dodge KA, Bates JE, Pettit GS. Mechanisms in the cycle of violence. Science (1990) 250(4988):1678–83. doi:10.1126/science.2270481

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

58. Finkelhor D. Childhood Victimization. Violence, Crime and Abuse in the Lives of Young People. Oxford: Oxford University Press (2008).

Google Scholar

59. Barnett D, Manly JT, Cicchetti D. Defining Child Maltreatment: The Interface Between Policy and Research. Norwood, NJ: Ablex Pub. Corp. (1993). p. 7–74.

Google Scholar

60. Nietlisbach G, Maercker A, Rossler W, Haker H. Are empathic abilities impaired in posttraumatic stress disorder? Psychol Rep (2010) 106(3):832–44. doi:10.2466/pr0.106.3.832-844

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

61. Cusi AM, Nazarov A, Holshausen K, Macqueen GM, McKinnon MC. Systematic review of the neural basis of social cognition in patients with mood disorders. J Psychiatry Neurosci (2012) 37(3):154–69. doi:10.1503/jpn.100179

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

62. Cohen D. Probabilistic epigenesis: an alternative causal model for conduct disorders in children and adolescents. Neurosci Biobehav Rev (2010) 34(1):119–29. doi:10.1016/j.neubiorev.2009.07.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

63. Shakoor S, Jaffee SR, Bowes L, Ouellet-Morin I, Andreou P, Happe F, et al. A prospective longitudinal study of children’s theory of mind and adolescent involvement in bullying. J Child Psychol Psychiatry (2012) 53(3):254–61. doi:10.1111/j.1469-7610.2011.02488.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

64. Sullivan PM, Knutson JF. Maltreatment and disabilities: a population-based epidemiological study. Child Abuse Negl (2000) 24(10):1257–73. doi:10.1016/S0145-2134(00)00190-3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

65. Frodi A, Smetana J. Abused, neglected, and nonmaltreated preschoolers’ ability to discriminate emotions in others: the effects of IQ. Child Abuse Negl (1984) 8(4):459–65. doi:10.1016/0145-2134(84)90027-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

66. Herrera VM, McCloskey LA. Gender differences in the risk for delinquency among youth exposed to family violence. Child Abuse Negl (2001) 25(8):1037–51. doi:10.1016/S0145-2134(01)00255-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

67. Rieffe C, Terwogt MM, Cowan R. Children’s understanding of mental states as causes of emotions. Infant Child Dev (2005) 14:259–72. doi:10.1002/icd.391

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

68. Pollak SD, Cicchetti D, Hornung K, Reed A. Recognizing emotion in faces: developmental effects of child abuse and neglect. Dev Psychol (2000) 36(5):679–88. doi:10.1037/0012-1649.36.5.679

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

69. Fonagy P, Redfern S, Charman T. The relationship between belief-desire reasoning and a projective measure of attachment security. Br J Dev Psychol (1997) 15:51–61. doi:10.1111/j.2044-835X.1997.tb00724.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

70. De Brito SA, Viding E, Sebastian CL, Kelly PA, Mechelli A, Maris H, et al. Reduced orbitofrontal and temporal grey matter in a community sample of maltreated children. J Child Psychol Psychiatry (2013) 54(1):105–12. doi:10.1111/j.1469-7610.2012.02597.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

71. Schore AN. The effects of early relational trauma on right brain development, affect regulation, and infant mental health. Infant Ment Health J (2001) 22:201–69. doi:10.1002/1097-0355(200101/04)22:1<201::AID-IMHJ8>3.0.CO;2-9

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

72. Symons DK, Clark SE. A longitudinal study of mother-child relationships and theory of mind in the preschool period. Soc Dev (2000) 3:3–23. doi:10.1111/1467-9507.00108

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: theory of mind, false-belief, perspective-taking, attribution biases, child abuse, maltreatment, systematic review, social cognition

Citation: Benarous X, Guilé J-M, Consoli A and Cohen D (2015) A systematic review of the evidence for impaired cognitive theory of mind in maltreated children. Front. Psychiatry 6:108. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2015.00108

Received: 24 May 2015; Accepted: 13 July 2015;
Published: 28 July 2015

Edited by:

John Vijay Sagar Kommu, National Institute of Mental Health and Neurosciences, India

Reviewed by:

Magdalena Romanowicz, Stanford University, USA
Suhash Chakraborty, Hindustan Aeronautics Limited Hospital, India

Copyright: © 2015 Benarous, Guilé, Consoli and Cohen. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Xavier Benarous, Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, Hôpital Pitié-Salpêtrière, 47-83 Boulevard de l’Hôpital, Paris 75013, France, xavierbenarous@gmail.com

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.