Skip to main content

HYPOTHESIS AND THEORY article

Front. Hum. Neurosci., 08 July 2020
Sec. Sensory Neuroscience
Volume 14 - 2020 | https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2020.00213

A Temporal Sampling Basis for Visual Processing in Developmental Dyslexia

  • 1Applied Psychology and Human Factors Laboratory, School of Psychology, University of Newcastle, Newcastle, NSW, Australia
  • 2Visual and Cognitive Neuroscience Laboratory, Department of Optometry and Vision Sciences, University of Melbourne, Parkville, VIC, Australia
  • 3The Florey Institute of Neuroscience and Mental Health, Parkville, VIC, Australia

Knowledge of oscillatory entrainment and its fundamental role in cognitive and behavioral processing has increasingly been applied to research in the field of reading and developmental dyslexia. Growing evidence indicates that oscillatory entrainment to theta frequency spoken language in the auditory domain, along with cross-frequency theta-gamma coupling, support phonological processing (i.e., cognitive encoding of linguistic knowledge gathered from speech) which is required for reading. This theory is called the temporal sampling framework (TSF) and can extend to developmental dyslexia, such that inadequate temporal sampling of speech-sounds in people with dyslexia results in poor theta oscillatory entrainment in the auditory domain, and thus a phonological processing deficit which hinders reading ability. We suggest that inadequate theta oscillations in the visual domain might account for the many magno-dorsal processing, oculomotor control and visual deficits seen in developmental dyslexia. We propose two possible models of a magno-dorsal visual correlate to the auditory TSF: (1) A direct correlate that involves “bottom-up” magnocellular oscillatory entrainment of the visual domain that occurs when magnocellular populations phase lock to theta frequency fixations during reading and (2) an inverse correlate whereby attending to text triggers “top-down” low gamma signals from higher-order visual processing areas, thereby organizing magnocellular populations to synchronize to a theta frequency to drive the temporal control of oculomotor movements and capturing of letter images at a higher frequency.

Introduction

Developmental dyslexia (which will henceforth be referred to simply as “dyslexia”) involves deficits in phonological processing of written and spoken language and visual processing of text (Snowling, 1981; Stein and Fowler, 1981; Wagner and Torgesen, 1987; Treisman, 1988; Goswami and Bryant, 1990; Livingstone et al., 1991; Cornelissen et al., 1995; Stein and Walsh, 1997; Vidyasagar, 1999; Giraldo-Chica et al., 2015). Substantial evidence suggests that impaired auditory processing reflects a deficit whereby neuronal populations fail to phase-lock to the temporal frequency of speech sounds. This underlies impaired coding of spoken syllables and phonemes, which is essential to phonological processing [Goswami’s temporal sampling framework (TSF), 2011]. Studies support the notion that temporal sampling of the rhythmic aspects of spoken language (such as syllables and prosody) enhances neural synchrony specifically at theta and gamma frequencies (Goswami and Bryant, 1990; Spironelli et al., 2006, 2008; Huss et al., 2011; Hämäläinen et al., 2012; Power et al., 2012; Goswami and Leong, 2013; Goswami et al., 2014; Colling et al., 2017; Goswami, 2018). Conversely, dyslexic readers have demonstrated impaired sensitivity to speech sounds, and lower theta and gamma synchronization in the auditory domain (Leong et al., 2011; Hämäläinen et al., 2012; Goswami et al., 2014; Leong and Goswami, 2014; Hyafil et al., 2015). However, investigation into oscillatory (brainwave) activity in the visual domain in dyslexia is limited. Cortical activity in general demonstrates remarkable consistency across operational domains – a cortical parsimony, where the brain uses similar mechanisms to support different types of cognitive function and behaviors (Bressler and Menon, 2010; Wang, 2010). Such cortical parsimony might logically apply to dyslexia, where deficits in the visual and auditory domains share common neurological mechanisms. It is well established that oscillatory phase-locking to visual stimuli occurs during visual processing (Gray and Singer, 1989; Engel et al., 1991, 2001; Kruse and Eckhorn, 1996; Herrmann, 2001; Thut and Miniussi, 2009; Mathewson et al., 2012; Thut et al., 2012; de Graaf et al., 2013; Spaak et al., 2014; Notbohm et al., 2016; Kizuk and Mathewson, 2017), and typical reading has been associated with changes at the theta (Klimesch et al., 2001; Bastiaansen et al., 2005, 2008; Meyer et al., 2015; McLelland et al., 2016), gamma (Penolazzi et al., 2009; Wang et al., 2018), and combined theta–gamma frequencies (Weiss et al., 2005; Hald et al., 2006; Bastiaansen et al., 2010; Wang et al., 2012b; Molinaro et al., 2013; Bedo et al., 2014). Early evidence also indicates that dyslexic readers have altered patterns of neuronal connectivity (Žarić et al., 2017) and lower levels of theta activity compared to typical readers (Stokić et al., 2011; González et al., 2016).

A generalized cross-domain temporal sampling deficit in dyslexia has been proposed, and already has some theoretical support (Llinás, 1993; Johnston et al., 2008; Goswami, 2011; Kraus, 2012; Vidyasagar, 2013, 2019; Gori et al., 2014; Pammer, 2014; Lewis and Bastiaansen, 2015; Hancock et al., 2017; Stein, 2019). Here, we aim to expand on the evidence of oscillatory phase-locking in visual processing and theta-gamma activity during typical reading and propose a visual correlate to the auditory TSF of dyslexia. Consistent with the auditory TSF we suggest that impaired oscillations in the visual domain – specifically at the theta and gamma frequencies – hinder effective visual temporal sampling and parsing of text.

We also aim to provide a possible link between the auditory TSF and another dominant theory of dyslexia, that is the magno-dorsal deficit hypothesis. Considerable evidence indicates the visual deficits in dyslexia are associated with changes to the magnocellular pathway within the dorsal visual stream1 which results in difficulties encoding and processing the low-spatial high-temporal characteristics of text (Badcock and Lovegrove, 1981; Stein, 1991; Cornelissen and Hansen, 1998; Slaghuis and Ryan, 1999; Vidyasagar, 1999, 2004; Vidyasagar and Pammer, 1999; Facoetti et al., 2006). We hypothesize that this magno-dorsal deficit is one which affects neural synchrony within and between magnocellular dominated visual pathways. Generalized magnocellular abnormalities as a result of early immune attack or genetic anomalies (Cowan, 1970; Cook and Stevens, 1973; Paracchini et al., 2016; Jiménez-Bravo et al., 2017) may influence the power and phase-coordination of neural firing (Eysel and Wolfhard, 1984; Yücel et al., 2003; Jiménez-Bravo et al., 2017). This could happen in one two ways. The generation of neuronal oscillations and synchrony within clusters of neurons and/or between clusters of neurons in different areas can be affected if timing of neuronal events is disrupted. Another additional factor is the morphological pathology in an area that could alter the resonance frequency of cells in responding to top-down signals, the resonance frequency of neurons being dependent on their biophysical characteristics of the cell and the local circuitry it is embedded in Hutcheon and Yarom (2000), Vidyasagar (2013). Remarkable parallels to the auditory TSF can be found in the magno-dorsal stream in reading and dyslexia at both theta and gamma frequencies (see Table 1). Eye movements during reading are driven by magno-dorsal areas, and in non-dyslexic readers, occur at a systematic theta frequency. However, dyslexic readers have erratic patterns of eye movements during reading, resulting in non-rhythmic fixations and saccades (Rayner, 1998). This could be an indication of theta frequency, fixation related, action potentials – or an impairment of them – in early visual sensory areas during reading and dyslexia, respectively. Evidence of theta and gamma neuronal synchronization during visual processing can be found specifically along the magno-dorsal pathway. At the “bottom-end,” retinal gamma activity has been identified in spatio-temporal tasks that isolate magnocellular activity in normal readers, yet it is not apparent in dyslexia (Pammer and Wheatley, 2001; Kevan and Pammer, 2008; Gori et al., 2014). At the “top-end” of the pathway [the posterior parietal cortex (PPC)], visual attention and processing involves increased low gamma (25–45 Hz) synchronization (Saalmann et al., 2007), which has already been shown to be significantly lower in the auditory cortex in dyslexia (Lehongre et al., 2011). We suggest that theta–gamma cross-frequency coupling, which occurs when processing spoken language, also occurs along the magno-dorsal pathway during top-down attentional modulation and bottom-up processing of visual stimuli (von Stein and Sarnthein, 2000; Demiralp et al., 2007; Holz et al., 2010; Wang et al., 2010).

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Parallels in neuronal activity in the auditory temporal sampling framework and magno-dorsal hypothesis of reading and dyslexia.

After reviewing the literature on the current neurobiological theories of reading and dyslexia and the evidence for them, we will consider the possibility of a visual TSF, where theta and gamma activity along the magno-dorsal pathway enable the visual cognitive processes required for reading. We will present two alternative models of a magno-dorsal TSF. Model 1 is a direct correlate to the auditory TSF, whereby theta frequency eye movements act as a stimulus that causes oscillations in visual sensory areas to synchronize at theta, and thereby induce higher-order magno-dorsal gamma activity. A potential flaw with this model is that, unlike listening to spoken language (an external environmental stimuli), attending to text is a process driven internally by the reader. Considering this, we also present Model 2, a top-down-bottom-up magno-dorsal oscillatory circuit where endogenous gamma synchronization enables top-down direction of visual attention to text, and couples with theta signals in early visual sensory areas for the bottom-up temporal filtering of text.

Cognitive Deficits in Developmental Dyslexia

Dyslexia is a universal and persistent form of low literacy in which a child fails to learn to read at an age-appropriate rate (e.g., Snowling, 2019). The disorder is found across languages and is not associated with intelligence, age, education or other neurological deficits (Snowling, 2019). It is well recognized that dyslexia is associated with cognitive deficits in auditory processing of spoken language and visual processing of text – both of which may be essential for a child to learn to read.

Auditory processing of spoken language is fundamental for the development of phonological and lexical knowledge. Phonological awareness in children is a strong predictor of reading ability later in life (Whalley and Hansen, 2006), and it is well recognized that this is impaired in most children with dyslexia (Goswami and Bryant, 1990; Snowling, 2001; Ziegler and Goswami, 2005; Goswami, 2011; Leong and Goswami, 2014), even though a clear causal connection has not been established (Castles and Coltheart, 2004; Vidyasagar and Pammer, 2010; Vidyasagar, 2012). In typical readers, phonological knowledge is amassed via decoding sound components (phonemes) in spoken language (Spiro and Myers, 2002). This requires recognizing the temporal order of speech sounds, rapid and accurate word recognition, rhythmic stress, and coding information in short-term memory (Hunt et al., 1975; Goswami and Leong, 2013). Decreased perceptual sensitivity to sound-segments in language, rhythmic stress, and timing of speech have been demonstrated in dyslexia, indicating problems in auditory processing (Goswami and Bryant, 1990; Snowling, 2001; Ziegler and Goswami, 2005; Goswami and Leong, 2013; Goswami et al., 2013).

During fluent reading, phonological knowledge is coordinated with visual processing to make lexical sense of images captured as the eye moves along a passage of text (Heth and Lavidor, 2015). This requires effective oculomotor control and rapid higher-order visual processing, both of which are disrupted in dyslexia. Dyslexic readers have difficulties conducting visual search and orienting attention among cluttered environments, (Vidyasagar and Pammer, 1999, 2010; Facoetti et al., 2006, 2010; Sireteanu et al., 2008; Moore and Schwitzgebel, 2018), processing low spatial frequency arrays (Badcock and Lovegrove, 1981; Slaghuis and Ryan, 1999), feature binding (Pammer et al., 2004a), coding the temporal sequences of words and letters within words (Stein, 1991; Cornelissen and Hansen, 1998; Vidyasagar, 1999, 2004; Vidyasagar and Pammer, 1999; Facoetti et al., 2006), and convergence of images from left and right eyes (Jainta and Kapoula, 2011).

Dyslexic children also display differences in oculomotor control during reading. Typical readers move their eyes linearly and rhythmically across text, making continuous saccades and fixations (Rayner, 1998). Saccades move the eye from one location to the next over a period of approximately 25–50 ms. Their purpose is to bring a new region of text into foveal vision – the area of the eye that provides the highest visual resolution. Between saccades, the eye remains relatively still for 200–300 ms in a fixation, allowing for more detailed analysis of a section of text (Rayner, 1998). Predominately, the eye progresses forward (left-to-right in left-to-right scripts or the reverse in right-to-left scripts) along a line of text making regular progressive fixations. Occasionally, the eye moves backward and fixates on a section of text that has already been read. This is known as regressive fixation and occurs when text needs revising for comprehension (Rayner, 1998). These eye movements differ significantly in dyslexia. Children with dyslexia also do not demonstrate normal developmental gains in eye movements when reading (see Figure 1). Typically, as children gain proficiency in cognitive processing during reading, they are able to move their eyes more rapidly along a passage of text. Progressive fixations become shorter, saccades travel further and fewer regressive fixations are required (Rayner, 1998). However, these oculomotor developmental gains are not evident to a similar extent in dyslexic children (Lefton et al., 1979; Stein, 1991; Benfatto et al., 2016). Child and adult dyslexic readers demonstrate erratic visual search patterns and make more frequent, lengthier fixations in comparison to non-dyslexic readers (Adler-Grinberg and Stark, 1978; Eden et al., 1994; Lefton et al., 1979; Martos and Vila, 1990; Stein, 1991). As such, dyslexic readers do not demonstrate an efficient rhythmic pattern of saccades and fixations.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Comparison of eye-movements during reading. (Left image): Normal reader. Fixations (represented by dots) and saccades (represented by the connecting lines) occur in a linear fashion at an approximate temporal frequency of 4 Hz. (Right image): Dyslexic reader. Fixations (represented by dots) occur erratically with no rhythmic temporal pattern (figure adapted from Prado et al., 2007).

Neurophysiological Theories and Evidence Pertinent to Reading and Dyslexia

The Auditory Temporal Sampling Framework

The neurobiological aetiology of impaired language processing is thought to be a deficit in the synchronization of oscillatory populations in the auditory domain, whereby coding of spoken language is impaired due to inept neuronal phase-locking to the theta temporal frequency of spoken language. It is well established that neuronal populations actively react to rhythmic events in the environment (Buzsáki and Draguhn, 2004; Lakatos et al., 2008; Calderone et al., 2014). When neuronal populations in sensory brain areas attend to repetitive stimuli they phase-lock (“tune in”) to the temporal structure of that stimulus and synchronize their firing rate to that frequency, a process called oscillatory entrainment (Lakatos et al., 2008; Calderone et al., 2014). Temporal sampling and oscillatory entrainment at low frequencies are thought to be crucial to the perceptual sensory processing, allowing environmental stimuli with repetitive and predictable rhythms – such as speech – to be distinguished more easily than unpredictable non-rhythmic stimuli (Buzsáki and Draguhn, 2004; Luo and Poeppel, 2007; Schroeder and Lakatos, 2009; Calderone et al., 2014).

The auditory temporal sampling hypothesis builds on the understanding that the regular rhythmic sequence of spoken syllabic sound-segments occurs at an approximate theta frequency (Goswami, 2011). That is, spoken syllables occur approximately every 200–300 ms. It is thought that neuronal populations in the auditory domain phase-lock to speech-sounds, synchronizing their oscillations to a theta frequency and initiating the cortical activity required for cognitive encoding of amplitude and temporal order information of phonemes (Goswami and Bryant, 1990; Luo and Poeppel, 2007; Spironelli et al., 2006, 2008; Goswami, 2011, 2018; Hämäläinen et al., 2012; Goswami and Leong, 2013; Goswami et al., 2014). It is thought that unstable theta phase-locking in sensory auditory areas results from an oscillatory synchronization deficit throughout the auditory pathway, disrupting effective phonological processing (Goswami, 2011) (refer to Figure 2). Compared to typical readers, children with dyslexia have significantly reduced theta activity when completing phonological tasks (Spironelli et al., 2006, 2008) and when listening to theta frequency auditory stimuli (Huss et al., 2011; Hämäläinen et al., 2012; Power et al., 2012; Colling et al., 2017). Decreased perception to speech-sound frequencies have also been identified in newborns at risk of dyslexia (Thiede et al., 2019). However, low-frequency theta waves in sensory areas alone are insufficient for enabling higher temporal rates of parsing needed for complex cognitive processing of language. It is thought that higher-order phonological processing is enabled by cross-frequency coupling of phase-locked theta oscillations to gamma oscillations (Lehongre et al., 2011, 2013; Gross et al., 2013; Lizarazu et al., 2015).

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Stylized representation of Goswami’s (2011) auditory temporal sampling framework (TSF). Syllabic sounds in spoken language occur at a theta frequency. It is hypothesized that when neuronal populations in the auditory domain attend to speech-sounds they phase-lock (entrain) to this theta frequency. Figure adapted from Pammer (2014).

Cross-Frequency Oscillatory Coupling

Cross-frequency coupling is a well-recognized mechanism by which an oscillatory population synchronized at one frequency modulates another neuronal population to synchronize at a distinct, second, frequency (Canolty et al., 2006; see Jensen and Colgin, 2007 for a review). Coupling of theta and gamma oscillations in particular occurs throughout the brain, and has been demonstrated in the hippocampus (Bragin et al., 1995; Axmacher et al., 2010; Belluscio et al., 2012), auditory cortex (Lakatos et al., 2007; Fontolan et al., 2014), posterior parietal cortex (Hawellek et al., 2016), and between the frontal and posterior parietal cortices (Friese et al., 2013). Coupling of alpha and gamma frequencies has also been shown between parietal and frontal areas (Wang et al., 2012a). Functionally, cross-frequency coupling leads to modulation of high frequency oscillations by low frequency oscillations, enabling entrainment to behaviorally relevant external sensory occurrences and internal cognitive events (Canolty and Knight, 2010). It is also an important mechanism for rapid neuronal communication between distant brain locations (Schroeder and Lakatos, 2009), with gamma oscillations operating as a functional mechanism at the local population level, and theta oscillations working to temporally coordinate activities of these populations across large-scale cortical networks (Lisman and Buzsaki, 2008).

Theta–gamma cross-frequency coupling is thought to be integral to speech encoding (Hyafil et al., 2011, 2015; Gross et al., 2013). After entraining to the syllabic structure of speech sounds, lower-order theta oscillations drive synchronized neuronal spiking at gamma frequencies (such that gamma activity becomes nested within synchronized theta spikes) to enable higher-order auditory encoding (Lakatos et al., 2005; Hyafil et al., 2011, 2015). When processing speech, theta-gamma coupling could enhance signal transfer, as well as increasing the amplitude of gamma waves to enable swift higher-order phonological decoding of speech (Lakatos et al., 2005; Hyafil et al., 2011, 2015; Gross et al., 2013).

Another possibility that underlies speech encoding is that coupled theta and gamma oscillations are in play throughout the auditory network in a bottom-up response to two distinct temporal frequencies in speech (Ghitza, 2011; Giraud and Poeppel, 2012). Generally, a theta–gamma “neural code” is thought to enable discrimination and temporal ordering of sequential sensory stimuli (Lisman and Buzsaki, 2008). As well as the theta frequency of syllabic chunks (approximately four syllables per second), speech acoustics simultaneously consist of high gamma frequency phonemes (the smallest constituent of language) (Ghitza, 2011; Giraud and Poeppel, 2012). In order to process syllables and phonemes, the neuronal response to speech might be temporal sampling at theta and gamma frequencies simultaneously (Ghitza, 2011; Giraud and Poeppel, 2012). Theta and gamma frequency oscillations might harmonize in a bottom-up manner for early encoding of both aspects of the speech acoustic. This model of theta–gamma activity has indeed been demonstrated whereby each frequency not only tracks the syllabic and phonetic components of speech respectively, but coordination of theta–gamma oscillations enables the listener to code the temporal order of phonemes within a syllable (Hyafil et al., 2015).

Thus, cross-frequency coupling enables sensory areas of the brain which capture language stimuli to communicate rapidly with higher-order brain areas for real-time processing of language input (Ghitza, 2011; Giraud and Poeppel, 2012; Hyafil et al., 2015). We propose that it is also possible that theta–gamma cross-frequency coupling occurs not only during auditory processing of spoken language, but also during visual processing of written text (von Stein and Sarnthein, 2000; Demiralp et al., 2007; Holz et al., 2010; Wang, 2010; Vidyasagar, 2013), and that such theta–gamma coupling may be impaired in the visual as well as auditory domains in dyslexia (Vidyasagar, 2013).

Theta and Gamma Oscillations During Reading

Investigations into cortical oscillatory activity during typical reading have found changes in gamma, theta, and combined gamma–theta activity during word and sentence processing. Significant increases in widespread cortical theta synchronization have been observed during reading (Klimesch et al., 2001), as well as localized theta increases over the occipital cortex (Bastiaansen et al., 2008), frontal cortex (Meyer et al., 2015), and posterior cortex (Meyer et al., 2015). These studies used word recognition and lexical-semantic violation tasks, leading to conclusions that theta synchronization enables working memory during reading (Meyer et al., 2015), for example lexical-semantic retrieval and word recognition (Klimesch et al., 2001; Bastiaansen et al., 2005, 2008), with the rationale that (i) temporal sequences are encoded within theta band oscillations (Lisman and Idiart, 1995; Lisman and Buzsaki, 2008) and (ii) theta waves play a key role in the maintenance of temporal order in working memory (Klimesch et al., 2001; Bastiaansen et al., 2005, 2008; Roberts et al., 2013). Remarkably, theta synchronization has been time-locked to the onset of sentence reading (Bastiaansen et al., 2010), akin to the theta phase-locking that occurs when listening to spoken language (Goswami and Bryant, 1990; Spironelli et al., 2006, 2008; Luo and Poeppel, 2007; Goswami, 2011, 2018; Hämäläinen et al., 2012; Goswami and Leong, 2013; Goswami et al., 2014).

Gamma activity has been observed during sentence reading with semantic violation tasks (Penolazzi et al., 2009) and language prediction tasks (Wang et al., 2018), leading to the suggestion that gamma frequency oscillations enable top-down linguistic predictions to pair with bottom-up input (Lewis and Bastiaansen, 2015; Wang et al., 2018). Studies have also identified increases in both theta and gamma oscillations during sentence reading (Weiss et al., 2005; Wang et al., 2012b; Mellem et al., 2013) and semantic processing tasks (Hald et al., 2006; Bastiaansen et al., 2010). Word-reading paradigms have demonstrated widespread (Bedo et al., 2014) and frontal-occipital (Molinaro et al., 2013) bi-directional theta and gamma phase-locking, indicating oscillatory activity is involved in both top-down attentional and predictive processes, as well as bottom-up analysis of incoming text stimuli (Molinaro et al., 2013).

Despite the above studies showing specific oscillatory dynamics in the visual areas associated with reading, there are only a few studies that have investigated oscillatory activity during reading in people with dyslexia. However, the evidence so far indicates that people with dyslexia have significantly fewer and altered neural network connections (Žarić et al., 2017), as well as lower levels of theta activity during reading (Stokić et al., 2011; González et al., 2016). Analysis of theta frequency network connections over the temporal and parietal cortices during reading indicate non-dyslexic readers have widespread oscillatory networks that are both quantitatively and qualitatively greater than those in dyslexic readers (Stokić et al., 2011). Furthermore, there is a positive correlation between reading ability and the number and power of neuronal networks in children with dyslexia (Žarić et al., 2017). Not only do connectivity networks differ during reading, EEG measures at resting-state with eyes closed show widespread theta activity is significantly less in dyslexic children compared to non-dyslexic children (González et al., 2016).

We propose that this increased theta and gamma activity during typical reading, as well as altered neuronal networks in dyslexia, are indicative of oscillatory activity in reading and dyslexia beyond just phonological processing in the auditory domain. It is possible that theta and gamma networks are involved in visual processing during reading in a manner similar to the auditory temporal sampling hypothesis (TSH). A visual TSF might also provide a theoretical link between the auditory TSF and another current influential theory of dyslexia, namely the magno-dorsal deficit hypothesis.

Magno-Dorsal Deficit Hypothesis of Dyslexia

At present, the prominent theory accounting for visual deficits in dyslexia is an impairment in the magnocellular-dominated dorsal pathway (see Stein, 2001, 2019 for reviews). The unique physical and functional characteristics of magnocells enable visual processes essential to reading (Lovegrove, 1996). Magnocellular axons are thick and myelinated, allowing for rapid signal conduction (i.e., high-temporal frequency processing). This enables magnocells to respond quickly to stimulation and provide rapid bottom-up information transfer for the visuo-spatial attention network essential for reading (Vidyasagar, 1999; Vidyasagar and Pammer, 1999).

Magno-dorsal processing can be examined using visual paradigms which exploit magno-dorsal characteristics (see Stein, 2001 for a full review) and compared to controls, dyslexic readers have significant difficulties in magno-dorsal isolating paradigms (typically involving low-spatial – high-temporal frequency stimuli). Dyslexic readers demonstrate destabilization of the binocular fixation necessary for convergence of images from the left and right eyes (Stein and Walsh, 1997) – suggesting a possible deficit in magnocellular activity. Hindrance of saccadic suppression results in the superimposition of successive images (Stein and Walsh, 1997) and may indicate a functional deficit in MT/V5 in dyslexia. Alterations in oculomotor control and motion perception (Eden et al., 1996; Demb et al., 1998) also indicate variances in MT/V5 and/or PPC functionality and can underlie poor orthographic skills. Higher-order dorsal processing deficits in dyslexia include difficulties in orienting and allocating visual attention (Facoetti et al., 2006, 2010), sub-lexical processing (Stein, 1991; Cornelissen and Hansen, 1998; Vidyasagar, 1999, 2004; Vidyasagar and Pammer, 1999; Facoetti et al., 2006), spatial processing (Badcock and Lovegrove, 1981; Slaghuis and Ryan, 1999), and feature binding (Pammer et al., 2004a).

Deficits in magnocellular processing have been identified as early as the retina using the spatial frequency doubling (FD) illusion (Pammer and Wheatley, 2001; Gori et al., 2014). This finding is significant in that it shows differences between dyslexic and non-dyslexic readers not only in magnocellular processing per se, but also in oscillatory synchronization at a gamma frequency at the earliest point of the magno-dorsal pathway. The illusion is produced by a 0.1-4 c/deg grating, flickering in temporal counterphase at >15Hz (i.e., extending to the gamma frequency range), which yields the perception that the grating is double the actual spatial frequency (Kelly, 1966, 1981; Maddess and Henry, 1990). The illusion occurs due to functional characteristics of M(y) cells in the retina (Benardete et al., 1992) and lateral geniculate nucleus (LGN; Marrocco et al., 1982), akin to full-wave rectification in signal processing (Kelly, 1981). Dyslexic readers have significantly higher spatial and temporal threshold to the FD illusion presented at 18 Hz (Gori et al., 2014), 25 Hz (Pammer and Wheatley, 2001), and 50 Hz (Kevan and Pammer, 2008) compared to non-dyslexic readers, indicating that a specific magnocellular oscillatory synchronization deficit may exist as early as the retina. Pre-readers at risk of dyslexia show the same threshold differences to the FD illusion presented at 50 Hz (Kevan and Pammer, 2008) and illiterate, semi-literate, and literate adults are all equally sensitive to the illusion presented at 50 Hz (Flint and Pammer, 2011). This suggests that sensitivity to the FD illusion is not dependant on magnocellular development as a child learns to read and is therefore not a consequence of failing to learn to read (Kevan and Pammer, 2008; Flint and Pammer, 2019). Furthermore, individual differences in sensitivity to the FD illusion are predictive of reading performance in dyslexic children (Gori et al., 2014).

That dyslexic readers are less sensitive to the FD illusion is important in linking deficits in retinal magnocellular populations to putative deficits in neural responses to stimuli that occur at the gamma frequency at the commencement of the magno-dorsal pathway. Building on this, as well as the evidence of theta and gamma activity during reading and decreased neural connectivity in dyslexia, the remainder of this paper will explore the possibility that the visual deficits in dyslexia may be due to inadequate oscillatory synchrony within the visual system (Johnston et al., 2008; Vidyasagar, 2013). We hypothesize that unstable phase-locking at the frequencies necessary for the ordered sampling and processing of text – especially between magnocellular populations and in their gating function on other pathways – may occur in the visual domain in a manner similar to the auditory Temporal Sampling Framework or TSF (Goswami, 2011).

Temporal Organization of Magnocells

We suggest that the deficit in dyslexia may be one of impaired functional connectivity between magnocellular-driven populations in the dorsal pathway. Damage to the neuronal structure or connections of magnocells might hinder synaptic communication within and between magnocellular-driven populations in the dorsal stream and also in their feedback signals to early visual areas, destabilizing oscillatory synchrony at the frequencies required for visual processing during reading.

The physical and functional characteristics of magnocells suggest magnocellular populations are ideal for rapid neuronal entrainment. Healthy magnocells are large and have thick, myelinated axons, which enable rapid signal communication of information. Their transient response characteristics (Dreher et al., 1976; Kaplan and Shapley, 1982; Hicks et al., 1983) make them ideally suited for achieving a high degree of precision in any kind of high frequency temporal sampling and in coincidence detection of neural spiking events, which are essential for creating a neural code based on synchronized oscillations. Thus, retinal and LGN magnocells have an inherent capacity for population-level phase-locking to repeated stimuli as their action potentials reset in phase with rapid stimulus shifts (Reinagel and Reid, 2000; Reich et al., 2001; Butts et al., 2011). By synchronizing their firing phases, magnocells dramatically increase their functional capacity and can encode larger volumes of text at a more detailed level (Destexhe et al., 1999; Agarwal and Sarma, 2012).

However, magnocells are highly vulnerable to damage – possibly from immunological attack in utero or genetic mutations (Cowan, 1970; Cook and Stevens, 1973; Paracchini et al., 2016; Jiménez-Bravo et al., 2017). Myelin damage has been found in dyslexic adults at autopsy (Humphreys et al., 1990), which is known to occur in response to immune attacks before neuronal myelination is complete, i.e., in utero or within the first year of life (Myers, 1969; Larroche, 1986). Immunological attack is also associated with neurons that are smaller in size and number (Cowan, 1970; Cook and Stevens, 1973), changes which correspond to a decline in neuronal function and transsynaptic communication (Eysel and Wolfhard, 1984; Yücel et al., 2003). Compared to controls, dyslexic readers have also been found to have fewer and smaller magnocells at autopsy, with magno-dorsal layers of the LGN being over 20% smaller than non-dyslexic readers (Livingstone et al., 1991; Galaburda et al., 1994; Fawcett et al., 1996). These alterations in cell morphology are likely to affect the functional capabilities of magnocells. Injury to myelin, impaired axonal development and dendritic arborization, as well as poor neuronal migration, would likely result in smaller, disorganized magnocellular populations which are impaired in both firing power and ability to synchronize their oscillatory phases at both the single-cell and population levels. Indeed, a number of genetic mutations in dyslexia have been identified to directly impact neuronal connectivity and have been linked to rhythmic dysfunction (see Paracchini et al., 2016; Jiménez-Bravo et al., 2017 for more detailed discussions on dyslexia and genetics).

Alongside the evidence of theta and gamma activity during reading, altered neural connections in dyslexia and retinal magnocellular temporal response functions as deduced from FD illusion studies, these genetic and physiological changes raise questions as to whether the impaired magno-dorsal processing in dyslexia may be due to impaired temporal processing in magnocellular-driven cortical circuitry. Given that impaired oscillatory synchrony occurs at theta and gamma frequencies in dyslexia in the auditory domain, cortical parsimony indicates oscillations in the visual domain in the magno-dorsal stream could also be impaired at gamma and theta frequencies, as well as affecting cross-frequency coupling of theta and gamma oscillations.

Impaired Theta Oscillatory Synchronization in Visual Processing During Reading

As discussed earlier, reading entails rapid shifts in visual stimuli that occur as the eye moves along a line of text (Rayner, 1998). Given that magnocells respond to alterations in stimuli by resetting their action potentials (Reinagel and Reid, 2000; Reich et al., 2001; Butts et al., 2011), each eye movement during reading might cause a phase-reset of retinal magnocells (Marrocco et al., 1982). Lateral geniculate nucleus single cell action potentials also consistently correlate with the temporal rate of visual stimulus presentation in animal studies, which is thought to enable coding of stimulus features (Creutzfeldt and Kuhnt, 1973; Kepecs and Lisman, 2003; Samengo et al., 2013). Eye movements during reading occur systematically and rhythmically, and as highlighted by Pammer (2014) and Vidyasagar (2013) occur at low theta frequencies. During reading, the average saccade takes roughly 30 ms (Rayner, 1998) and the average fixation lasts approximately 225 ms (Rayner, 1998). Thus, during reading a new fixation commences approximately every 255 ms, equating to a temporal sampling rate of approximately 4 Hz. We propose that this is a visual correlate to the temporal sampling of theta frequency syllabic speech-sounds hypothesized in the auditory TSF (refer to Figure 3). Theta frequency eye movements may be indicative of theta frequency fixation related potentials in lower-order areas of the visual pathway, correlating to the auditory domain’s theta phase-locking in response to spoken language. Indeed, visuospatial processing has been shown to correlate with ongoing theta activity around the time of saccades (McLelland et al., 2016), and a significant increase in theta synchronization has been time-locked to onset of sentence reading (Bastiaansen et al., 2010). Furthermore, given dyslexic readers have erratic, non-rhythmic eye movements during reading, we suggest that this is indicative of unstable theta activity early in the magno-dorsal pathway. Dyslexic readers typically have a decreased temporal frequency rate of fixations due to increased fixation frequency and increased fixation dwell time (Rayner, 1998), which could be inadequate for encoding visual information.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Stylized representation of a possible visual correlate to the auditory temporal sampling framework. It is hypothesized that in normal reading theta frequency fixations entrain theta brainwave activity in the visual domain, thus supporting cognitive visual processing during reading (figure adapted from Pammer, 2014).

Beyond cortical parsimony to temporal sampling of spoken language, theta frequency sampling of text makes sense both behaviorally and functionally. From a basic mechanical point of view, physiological limitations make it unlikely that systematic eye movements could occur at frequencies beyond theta. Cognitively, oscillations are thought to act as a mechanism for controlling the timing in serial processing of sensory input (Lisman and Idiart, 1995) and low frequency temporal sampling would act as a control mechanism for information overload. When encoding spoken language, theta frequency phase-locking acts as a temporal filter or funnel, whereby auditory information is segmented into chunks for systematic analysis (Stevens, 2002; Poeppel, 2003). This is because the volume capacity of neurons to encode information is bound by phase-locking to temporal changes in stimuli consistent with their temporal modulation transfer functions (Hubel and Wiesel, 1961; Hicks et al., 1983; Reinagel and Reid, 2000; Levine and Cleland, 2001; Nirenberg et al., 2001; Rathbun et al., 2010). Theta temporal sampling of text could act as a similar pre-processing or parsing of visual language. Text environments are cluttered with a low-spatial frequency between complex visual features of letters and words. The visual system could easily be overloaded and unable to effectively encode visual features if the eye were to shift too quickly along a passage of text. Low frequency temporal sampling and phase-locking could therefore act as a filter similar to the parsing of spoken language, maintaining a manageable signal-to-noise ratio of visual information (refer to Figure 4).

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. Summary of the rationales for theta-frequency eye-movements during reading, which may be orchestrated by low-frequency claustral oscillatory output.

Gamma Synchronization Along the Magno-Dorsal Pathway

Low-level theta encoding is clearly important in sampling text as it reflects the oculomotor rhythm at which the eye moves across the page and is disrupted in dyslexia by poorly controlled saccadic movements. However, just as theta synchronization in the auditory domain is insufficient for complex phonological processing of spoken language, theta synchronization in the magno-dorsal pathway is unlikely to be sufficient for higher-order cognitive processing of text. Indeed, we will present evidence of gamma activity that has been identified along the magno-dorsal pathway and is associated with visual information processing and attention (Herrmann and Mecklinger, 2000, 2001; Fries et al., 2002; Saalmann et al., 2007; Lehongre et al., 2011; Bastos et al., 2014). It is possible that magnocellular theta oscillations couple to gamma activity along the magno-dorsal pathway, mirroring the theta-gamma coupling that is hypothesized in the auditory TSF (von Stein and Sarnthein, 2000; Demiralp et al., 2007; Holz et al., 2010; Wang et al., 2010).

Building on the evidence discussed we will now present two possible models for a magno-dorsal visual TSF in reading and dyslexia. We will firstly consider the possibility of a direct visual correlate to the auditory TSF, where eye movements act as a stimulus to entrain theta oscillations in visual sensory areas, and couple to gamma oscillations along the magno-dorsal in a bottom-up manner (Model 1). We will then present an alternative hypothesis of a magno-dorsal TSF that is a top-down and bottom-up oscillatory circuit, where modulation of oscillations with reading initiates with gamma synchronization in the PPC to drive theta oscillations and theta frequency eye movements during reading (Model 2).

Model 1: a Visual-Temporal Sampling Model as a Bottom-Up Process

Analogous to the packets of auditory stimuli in spoken language, the actual oculomotor mechanics of reading might entrain magnocellular theta oscillations in the visual domain (Pammer, 2014). Fixation-related potentials that occur as the eye moves along the text might account for the increase in theta power that occurs at the onset of sentence reading (Bastiaansen et al., 2010). Just as the auditory domain phase-locks to the theta frequency of syllabic speech sounds to enable phonological processing, oscillatory activity early in the visual pathway might phase-lock to theta frequency fixations that occur during reading (Pammer, 2014). Support for this can be found in the LGN, where phase-locked oscillations encode information about stimulus shape (Gawne et al., 1991; McClurkin et al., 1991), indicating the LGN uses temporal coding to maximize efficiency of information coding (Dong and Atick, 1995; Funke and Wörgötter, 1997; Reinagel and Reid, 2000; Reich et al., 2001; Butts et al., 2011). It has been argued that saccades and the eye drifts between saccades favor the processing of low and high spatial frequencies respectively (Boi et al., 2017). This means, that when applied to the effect of eye movements during reading, saccades may favor the processing of words as blurred images, but during fixations, which are associated with slow eye drifts, the finer frequencies needed for letter identification are preferentially processed. As dyslexic readers have erratic patterns of fixations and saccades during reading (Stein, 1991; Sireteanu et al., 2008), it is therefore likely they would have no stable temporal low-frequency stimulus to which magnocellular oscillations can phase-lock during reading (Pammer, 2014), accounting for poor encoding of complex low-spatial text. Interestingly, re-training eye movements in dyslexic children using low theta frequency temporal stimuli has been shown to correspond with significant increases in visual perceptual capacity (Fischer and Hartnegg, 2000), which could be an indication of increased phase-locking to a stable theta stimulus.

However, as we described above, it is unlikely that theta entrainment alone would suffice for complex processing of text. We predict that visual temporal sampling as a bottom-up process could also drive modulation of gamma oscillations for higher-order processing of text. We also suggest that, parallel to the auditory TSF, poor theta phase-locking to eye movements in dyslexic subjects would impair effective theta-gamma coupling (refer to Figure 5).

FIGURE 5
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 5. Representation of a hypothesized bottom-up model of visual temporal sampling during normal reading (Model 1). (Top figure): Normal reading. 1. Eye-movements during reading occur at a theta-frequency and act as an entrainment stimulus. 2. Magnocellular oscillations in visual sensory areas phase-lock to the theta-rhythm of eye-movements, thus enabling visual coding of text. 3. Theta phase-locked oscillations drive cross-frequency oscillatory coupling to gamma. 4. Gamma oscillations become nested within entrained theta oscillations, thus enabling transfer of information to higher-order visual areas for processing of text. (Bottom figure): Reader with dyslexia. 1. Dyslexic readers have erratic eye-movements during reading, generating no steady rhythm of visual stimulus shifts. 2. The action potentials of magnocells respond to each stimulus shift. 3. However, a lack of theta eye-movements means there is no stable rhythm to which magnocellular populations can entrain and this impairs coding of text. 4. Lack of theta synchronization in visual sensory areas means that cross-frequency coupling to gamma cannot occur, thus hindering communication of information along the magno-dorsal pathway 5. Lack of increased gamma synchronization in the PPC results in impaired processing of text.

One problem with a stand-alone bottom-up model of a visual TSF is that it does not account for the neuronal activity required to drive oculomotor activity. Attending to speech and reading a text are quite different processes. Listening to language is an exogenous attentional task, which, in comparison, is relatively passive, whereas reading involves endogenous, top-down attention and active, intentional control of eye movements. Our brain has no control over when another person speaks, necessitating rapid activation of temporal sampling of speech sounds, and bottom-up processing of sensory input. However, reading requires visual and oculomotor attention to be decisively directed to text when we choose to read. A magno-dorsal attention network has been suggested to generate top-down signals that selectively bias visual cortical activity (Gitelman et al., 1999; Hopfinger et al., 2000; Corbetta and Shulman, 2002) and has been hypothesized to be activated during reading (Vidyasagar, 1999, 2013). Anticipatory control of visual attention during reading might activate a gaze control system that actively samples text in a temporal pattern that enhances processing of text by priming the visual system for input (Rajkai et al., 2007) and controlling for information overload. Thus, we will now present a hypothesis for a visual TSF – a temporal oscillatory network where top-down attention modulates neuronal activity to sample text at a theta frequency, prior to sequential letter encoding during each fixation.

Top-Down Processes in Oscillatory Synchronization During Reading

As discussed earlier, a cross-frequency coupling relationship between gamma and theta oscillations is well evidenced (Jensen and Colgin, 2007; Lisman and Jensen, 2013). Although this relationship is most commonly described as low-frequency oscillations modulating the amplitude of higher frequency oscillations, there is also evidence of reverse directional functional coupling. Neurons not only modulate their firing in response to changes in stimulus (bottom-up), they also modulate their firing when attention is directed to an anticipated visual task (top-down) (Itti and Koch, 2001). Synchronized gamma oscillations in higher-order processing areas predict action potentials in lower down neuronal populations, causing high-to-low frequency coupling (Volgushev et al., 1998; Tallon-Baudry and Bertrand, 1999; Di Garbo, 2008; Chelazzi et al., 2011; Bosman et al., 2012; Squire et al., 2013; Buschman and Miller, 2014; Peelen and Kastner, 2014). Top-down highlow frequency coupling has been suggested to enable task-dependent top-down neuronal communication between different brain areas (Tallon-Baudry and Bertrand, 1999; Jiang et al., 2015). In the task of attending to visual stimuli, gamma-driven theta synchronization has been suggested to enhance sensory encoding of visual environments (Itti and Koch, 2001). This mechanism may occur during reading, whereby attention modulated top-down gamma theta coupling along the magno-dorsal pathway could enable encoding of text. Indeed, increases in frontal-occipital theta and gamma synchronization have been observed immediately prior to reading target words (Molinaro et al., 2013).

Coupling of top-down attentional signals to lower frequencies during reading has also been suggested to contribute to word-to-word shifts of attention that occur with eye movements (Vidyasagar, 2019). This is consistent with the magno-dorsal pathway’s involvement in visual attention and oculomotor control (Nobre et al., 2000; Moore et al., 2003). Thus, an alternative hypothesis to Model 1 is that gamma theta coupling drives temporal sampling of text via theta frequency saccade-fixation movements. This is consistent with the auditory temporal sampling of theta frequency syllabic speech segments. We hypothesize in this model that theta frequency eye movements in turn reinforce theta phase-locking and propagates theta gamma coupling to complete an oscillatory circuit along the magno-dorsal pathway (see Figures 6, 7). Consistent with general properties of cortical oscillatory networks, gamma synchronization might enable neuronal functioning at the local magnocellular population level, while theta synchronization might enable long-range communication and coordination between discrete magnocellular populations along the dorsal pathway (von Stein and Sarnthein, 2000; Buzsáki and Draguhn, 2004). Furthermore, visual working memory appears to encode temporal information at a theta frequency and spatial information within gamma oscillations (Roberts et al., 2013). Thus, the low-spatial high-temporal functional characteristics of magnocells might function optimally at theta and gamma frequencies during visual processing, and particularly so, given the low-spatial high-temporal characteristics underlying eye movements during reading.

FIGURE 6
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 6. The magno-dorsal circuit. During bottom-up visual processing during reading signals from retinal magnocells are projected to magnocellular layers of the lateral geniculate nucleus (LGN). From the LGN, signals are projected from the LGN to V1 (visual cortex), where, the visual pathway diverges into the dorsal (“where”) and ventral (“what”) streams. The dorsal pathway, dominated by magnocells, is constituted by a hierarchy of cortical areas, namely, V2, V3, MT/V5 and the posterior parietal cortex (PPC). In top-down visual attention during reading, neuronal signals are sent from the PPC to MT/V5, V2, V1 and LGN. The PPC and MT/V5 also project to the pre-frontal cortex (PFC), which plays a key role in the control of eye-movements during reading. The ventral stream, proceeds to V4 and further on to the inferior temporal cortex (ITC).

FIGURE 7
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 7. Representation of the hypothesized magno-dorsal temporal sampling framework as a top-down and bottom-up process (Model 2). (Top figure) Normal reading: When visual attention in the posterior parietal cortex (PPC) is directed to text it drives a top-down modulation of oscillatory activity. Oscillations in the PPC synchronize to gamma and drive high-to-low cross-frequency coupling to theta. When oscillations in visual sensory areas synchronize at a theta frequency it causes eye-movements during reading to also occur at a theta frequency. (Bottom figure) Reader with dyslexia: When visual attention is directed to text, oscillations in the PPC fail to synchronize effectively at a gamma frequency. This in turn hinders also the effectiveness of the PPC in modulating top-down signals required for theta-controlled eye-movements during reading. Erratic eye-movements mean visual areas lack theta-phase-locking reinforcement, thus interrupting consolidation of theta-gamma activity and hindering bottom-up information transfer.

A possible source of the low, possibly theta, frequency input to early visual areas needed to parse text into chunks of letters in sync with eye movement shifts may be the claustrum. This is consistent with the recent proposal (Vidyasagar and Levichkina, 2019) of a detailed neuronal framework for a percipient suggestion by Crick and Koch (2005). In view of claustrum’s extensive anatomical connectivity with almost every cortical area, Crick and Koch (2005) suggested that its function may be to orchestrate and integrate activities across cortical areas, mediating a particular cognitive function at any instant of time. Smythies et al. (2014) have suggested that this may be done by facilitating neural synchrony between cortical areas. In providing a plausible neural basis for such a function, Vidyasagar and Levichkina (2019) suggest that claustrum’s low frequency output may serve to amplify, by cross-frequency coupling, synchronized oscillations between cortical areas that are working together at any one time to execute a cognitive function. Since claustrum is the one brain region that is connected nearly reciprocally with every cortical area, in the case of reading a text, claustral output may serve to synchronize eye movements with the coarse parsing of text and, simultaneously, boost neural synchrony in the beta/low gamma range that is needed for parsing graphemes (Vidyasagar, 2013). The model describes a framework for the fast feedforward inhibitory circuit (Bruno, 2011) implicated in the claustral model (Vidyasagar and Levichkina, 2019). This circuit may play a critical role not only in generating oscillations (Kremkow et al., 2010), but also in enhancing neural synchrony between the relevant cortical areas, pushing along the processing of graphemes at a gamma frequency and chunks of graphemes that are processed between saccades at a theta frequency. Reading requires processing of visual stimuli at multiple temporal scales (Graboi and Lisman, 2003) and if cross-frequency coupling is the mechanism that permits this, the claustrum is a good candidate to function as a central integrator that can facilitate synchrony at two temporal scales and also, by virtue of its unique connectivity, help to punctuate the stream of visual stimuli in the text, as described elsewhere (Vidyasagar and Levichkina, 2019). Though the application of the claustral hypothesis to reading is largely speculative and needs further empirical evidence, there is an intriguing fMRI study that shows strong bilateral recruitment of the claustrum by active learning of letters compared to passive viewing (Kersey and James, 2013). Imaging studies of claustrum and its functional connections in normal readers and dyslexic subjects may reveal whether claustral activity is intimately involved in reading as proposed here.

Model 2: A Visual-Temporal Sampling Model as a Top-Down Bottom-Up Magno-Dorsal Oscillatory Circuit

A possible alternative to Model 1 is a magno-dorsal TSF where visual processing during reading involves a dynamic interplay between top-down attention-driven oscillations and bottom-up stimulus evoked oscillatory synchronization. This is consistent with evidence presented earlier of widespread bi-directional theta and gamma phase-locking during word and sentence reading (Molinaro et al., 2013; Bedo et al., 2014). In this model, theta and gamma oscillations synchronize as a mechanism for top-down predictive optimization of magno-dorsal cortical activity, as well as a bottom-up adaptive operation to enhance magno-dorsal processing during reading. It is possible that temporal sampling of text is driven by attention-modulated synchronization of higher-order magnocellular oscillations to a gamma frequency. This could drive a top-down goal-oriented modulation of magnocellular populations to synchronize at theta for the organized temporal sampling of text (see Figure 7), a process that may be orchestrated by the claustrum (Vidyasagar and Levichkina, 2019). The combined evidence of altered neuronal networks, magnocellular damage, impaired magnocellular oscillatory activity, significantly lower gamma activity in magno-dorsal processing and the lack of theta frequency eye movements during reading, suggests this oscillatory circuit may be impaired in dyslexic readers.

Given that top-down attentional signals from PPC to frontal eye fields (FEF) (Buschman and Miller, 2007) and from PPC to middle temporal visual area (MT) (Saalmann et al., 2007) have been identified, as well as top-down signals involved in visual search from the PPC to MT and then on to V1 (Vidyasagar, 1998; Brefczynski and DeYoe, 1999; McAdams and Reid, 2005), modulation of gamma oscillations during reading might arise from the “back end” of the dorsal stream network (see Figure 6). We hypothesize that this occurs in the PPC, where increased gamma activity is shown to be linked to visual attention and planned saccadic movements (Saalmann et al., 2007; Siegel et al., 2008; Van Der Werf et al., 2008; Marshall et al., 2015; Levichkina et al., 2017; Richter et al., 2017). Attention-driven gamma signals directed from the PPC to the magno-dorsal V5/MT that have been identified (Saalmann et al., 2007) may be deployed by visual attention during eye movements and visually guided movements in general (Sereno, 1992; McPeek et al., 1999; Krauzlis, 2004; Bakola et al., 2007; Laycock and Crewther, 2008; Van Der Werf et al., 2008; Ronconi et al., 2014). Signals from the PPC also project to the FEF (Tian and Lynch, 1996), where neuronal activity is thought to predicate premotor deployment of visuo-spatial attention (Vernet et al., 2014), fixations (Izawa et al., 2004, 2009), smooth visual pursuit (Bizzi, 1968; MacAvoy et al., 1991), and intentional saccadic movements (Robinson and Fuchs, 1969; Bruce and Goldberg, 1985; Luna et al., 1998; Everling and Munoz, 2000; Lachaux et al., 2006). Furthermore, modulation of LGN oscillatory activity has been found to occur 300 ms before the beginning of a saccade (Lee and Malpeli, 1998), consistent with the notion of top-down modulation of neurons even in early sensory nuclei for the control of theta frequency eye movements.

This suggests that instead of eye movements during reading initiating theta phase-locking (as would occur in Model 1), top-down oscillatory synchrony could possibly drive the theta frequency oculomotor mechanics of attention, significantly enhanced by the claustrum as explained earlier. Anticipatory control of visual attention would enhance processing of attended stimuli by biasing the visual cortex before stimulus onset, priming the visual system for a temporal pattern of visual input that is a straightforward consequence of gaze control systems (Rajkai et al., 2007; Bosman et al., 2012). Synchronized gamma signals might enable the spatio-temporal filtering of text by instigating theta oscillatory synchronization that controls eye movements, in a manner inverse of bottom-up signals in the auditory TSF. In other words, eye movements during reading could be driven by top-down goal-orientated gammatheta signal along the magno-dorsal-dorsal pathway. Applying Model 2 to dyslexia, ineffective gamma production could result in a theta magno-dorsal oscillatory deficit, resulting in erratic eye movements and thus poor temporal sampling of text by magno-dorsal-cells. Poor temporal filtering of text would bombard the magno-dorsal visual system with an overload of disordered information, affecting the encoding of magno-specific text characteristics as well as affecting the gating of graphemic information into the ventral stream for identification of letters and their temporal sequence. Thus, the full story of visual temporal sampling in dyslexia may in fact be a synthesis of a bottom-up mechanism (mirroring the auditory TSF) and a top-down mechanism (driven by the PPC and boosted and sequenced by the claustrum), creating a magno-dorsal oscillatory circuit which is impaired in dyslexia (refer to Figure 7).

An Oscillatory Circuit

In an oscillatory “loop,” bottom-up entrained theta oscillations might strengthen neuronal communication during reading via reciprocal frequency coupling to gamma – in other words, top-down modulation of oscillations (as in Model 2) and bottom-up phase-locking (as in Model 1) might occur in concert to enable reading. Given that retinal magnocellular gamma activity is evidenced in the FD illusion (Pammer and Wheatley, 2001; Gori et al., 2014), as well as findings of gamma activity in the LGN (Neuenschwander and Singer, 1996; Castelo-Branco et al., 1998), it is possible that simultaneous theta and gamma activity occur along the entire magno-dorsal pathway, such that each frequency is nested within one another and modulating each other (Penttonen and Buzsáki, 2003; Le Van Quyen, 2011; Vidyasagar and Levichkina, 2019). Early support of such a circuit is in the identification of a rapid oscillatory loop between V1 and the LGN, where top-down signals and bottom-up signals influence each other, strengthening communication between the two areas (Briggs and Usrey, 2007).

The functional purpose of such a circuit could be three-fold. Firstly, it would enable the rapid communication between brain areas that is needed during reading. While gamma oscillations might enable operations at the local magnocellular population level (Lisman and Buzsaki, 2008), theta oscillations might temporally organize these population-level operations across different brain regions along the dorsal pathway (Lisman and Buzsaki, 2008). Secondly, reading graphemes (letters) in text occurs at a low gamma frequency. The average typical reading speed is 303 words per minute (Rubin and Turano, 1992), equating to approximately 23 graphemes per second – in other words, 23 Hz (Vidyasagar, 2013). This is also consistent with the temporal frequency of phonemes in spoken language (Vidyasagar, 2013). Theta–gamma synchronization in sensory visual areas might act as a “neural code” to enable discrimination and temporal ordering of sequential stimuli (Lisman and Buzsaki, 2008). While theta frequency eye movements “funnel” stimulus to control for information overload, simultaneous gamma oscillations might encode single letters, prior to processing letter-sequences (i.e., words) (Vidyasagar, 2013). Thirdly, as discussed earlier, gamma signals not only increase with attention before visual stimulus presentation, but also after stimulus onset, enabling detailed processing of bottom-up signals from external stimuli (Tallon-Baudry and Bertrand, 1999; Fries et al., 2002; Fell et al., 2003; Saalmann et al., 2007), consistent with concepts of top-down gating of incoming sensory stimuli (von der Malsburg, 1981; von der Malsburg and Schneider, 1986; Treisman, 1988; Vidyasagar, 1999). Thus, pre-stimulus top-down gamma synchronization might be enhanced by eye movement evoked phase-resetting of oscillations in visual sensory areas (Tallon-Baudry and Bertrand, 1999; Fries et al., 2001; Fell et al., 2003) and by the claustral circuit via theta-gamma coupling (Vidyasagar and Levichkina, 2019). In this way, the amalgamation and transfer of top-down attention and bottom-up information processing may be enabled by a two-way theta gamma coupling circuit (von Stein and Sarnthein, 2000; Demiralp et al., 2007; Holz et al., 2010; Bastos et al., 2012; Friese et al., 2013; Wang et al., 2014). Conversely in dyslexia, unstable theta activity in oculomotor control could destabilize theta gamma oscillatory coupling and affect bottom-up transfer of textual information (see Table 2 for a summary of some of the evidence supporting a top-down and bottom-up magno-dorsal oscillatory circuit).

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Some of the evidence of theta and gamma oscillatory processes along the magno-dorsal pathway during visual processing.

Implications for Future Research

Testing the Hypothesis of a Visual TSF

Much of the evidence for top-down and bottom-up oscillatory activity during visual processing so far comes from electrophysiological studies on monkeys. However, exploring the possibility of a magno-dorsal correlate to the auditory TSF would also require human studies using normal readers and dyslexic readers. One of the strengths of the visual TSF models we have presented is that they each generate a number of testable hypotheses. Identifying whether a magno-dorsal visual TSF exists in reading and is impaired in dyslexia could be investigated experimentally with functional imaging (for oscillatory activity and anatomical locations of that activity), eye tracking, transcranial stimulation and behavioral entrainment. Magnocellular-isolating stimuli have typically been used in studies to investigate differences between the two populations, however it will also be important to conduct experiments using reading tasks which require coding and cognitive processing of written language that cannot be fully replicated in other visual tasks. Preliminary aims of investigations would likely be to (1) identify if oscillatory synchronization at theta and gamma frequencies along the magno-dorsal pathway does indeed occur during reading; (2) identify whether magno-dorsal oscillatory synchronization occurs only in a bottom-up manner (i.e., do theta eye movements entrain theta oscillations and then drive gamma synchronization?) or in a top-down-bottom-up circuit (i.e., does gamma oscillatory activity initiate in PPC when a person starts reading? And is this followed by increased theta in sensory areas?) (3) is there a difference between dyslexic and non-dyslexic readers in the functioning of magno-dorsal oscillatory synchronization? What is the specific cognitive-behavioral effect of this difference? (4) Does the claustrum play a critical role in boosting the synchrony between cortical areas? and (5) Does claustral input to visual areas operate in the theta frequency apparent during reading, especially involving the areas classically known to be activated during reading, such as the parietal cortex and the visual word form area (VWFA)?

Isolating the specific phase-locked oscillatory activity between brain areas in visual processing using single cell recordings is one possibility. However, given that single-cell recordings are highly unlikely to involve human participants (these are typically conducted using monkeys) this method would be of less use in investigating magno-dorsal activity during normal and dyslexic reading. However, local field potentials from electrocorticogram (ECOG) from surface recordings in patients implanted with electrode arrays typically to investigate epileptogenic foci is a possibility for testing our models, if these areas happen to involve parietal and early visual areas.

Bottom-up oscillatory entrainment could be investigated by examining the relationship between individuals’ eye movements during reading and theta and gamma activity. The use of eye tracking technology combined with neural imaging (ideally fMRI and MEG) that is time-locked to the eye tracker would enable oscillatory patterns to be examined in direct relation to fixations and saccades during reading. Given that individuals vary in both reading speed and resting alpha activity, it would be expected that their individual eye movement frequency would directly reflect their individual theta waves (between 3 and 7 Hz) if there is a direct association between the two (i.e., average eye movement frequency within the theta window would be same theta frequency as magnocellular oscillatory activity, with significant coherence between their phases). It would also be expected that along with erratic eye movements, dyslexic readers would demonstrate significantly lower theta activity. Eye tracking and imaging would similarly enable investigation of gamma synchronization during reading. Time-sequencing of neural activity would be important in examining the order of oscillatory changes in specific brain locations along the magno-dorsal pathway and may further the identification of top-down or bottom-up activity during reading, and possible oscillatory impairments in dyslexic readers.

One non-invasive technique that has been suggested as being effective in influencing cognitive functions and that can be used to test the validity of the visual TSF is transcranial electrical stimulation (Fertonani and Miniussi, 2018). Admittedly, one still needs to be cautious of the efficacy of the method (see Horvath et al., 2016; Vöröslakos et al., 2018; Vidyasagar, 2019). The technique involves applying electrical stimulation on the scalp, either as direct current (tDCS) or alternating current (tACS). If the technique is proven to be of some effect, tACS can be used along with imaging and eye tracking studies to examine the direct relationship between synchronized oscillations and eye movements. The frequency of the alternating current can be adjusted according to experimental aims, with the conducting electrode placed on the scalp corresponding to a specific brain location of interest. When set to a specific frequency, the tACS electrical impulses are thought to stimulate neuronal firing in the underlying brain area. In this way, “enforced” oscillatory entrainment may be stimulated within neural populations. The effects of tACS can be measured both behaviorally and with imaging. Conducting tACS at a theta frequency over an area such as the FEF or MT/V5, combined with eye tracking and imaging, could be of interest in examining any top-down effects on eye movements. Furthermore, if bottom-up theta → gamma coupling occurs with theta frequency eye movements, it would be expected that an increase in gamma activity might be seen in the PPC. Alternatively, gamma frequency tACS could be applied over the PPC during reading. If a top-down gamma → theta network is in play, we would expect an increase in theta activity in sensory areas. If continuous theta ↔ gamma coupling occurs along the magno-dorsal pathway, we might expect to see an increase in gamma activity as well as theta activity in areas such as area V1 and perhaps even the LGN when gamma tACS is applied over the PPC. It would be important to examine any cognitive effects of tACS entrainment and we would expect that it would increase the effectiveness of visual processing during reading – particularly in readers with dyslexia. This raises the possibility of therapeutic entrainment studies in dyslexic readers. However, given the controversy surrounding the effectiveness of transcranial electrical stimulation (reviewed in Vidyasagar, 2019), much work is needed to investigate the usefulness of this approach for correcting any temporal sampling deficit in dyslexia.

Therapeutic Neuronal Entrainment in Dyslexia

Although in its infancy, behavioral brainwave entrainment as a remedial tool is gathering evidence. Whereas tACS is not endorsed for use in children due to unknown long-term adverse effects, behavioral entrainment is safe and non-invasive. Given that dyslexia is a childhood developmental disorder, a neurological therapy should be child-friendly and could be of great use in targeting the brain-basis of dyslexia. The effectiveness of visual entrainment was demonstrated by Herrmann (2001) who used custom-made goggles that emitted a rapid flashing light at various frequencies (between 1 and 100 Hz). Oscillatory activity in response to the light significantly increased corresponding to the frequencies of 10, 20, 40 and 80 Hz. This light flicker entrainment mechanism has been since demonstrated in other imaging studies (de Graaf et al., 2013; Spaak et al., 2014).

Positive effects of oscillatory entrainment have been demonstrated in attentional disorders (Weaver et al., 2012), epilepsy (Auvichayapat et al., 2013), motor control (Valle et al., 2007), depression (Bloch et al., 2008), Tourette’s syndrome (Kwon et al., 2011), communication and behavioral difficulties related to Autism Spectrum Disorder (Baruth et al., 2010), and schizophrenia (Mondino et al., 2015a, b). Applied to dyslexia, visual theta and/or gamma entrainment might optimize the condition of oscillatory activity in the magno-dorsal pathway. Combining reading exercises with behavioral brainwave entrainment in dyslexic readers could be a way of priming neuronal networks in the brain to be optimally receptive to reading.

There is evidence for significant improvements in reading scores (speed and comprehension), selective attention, both visual and auditory working memory, and phonological processing in dyslexics from discrimination training in visual motion perception when conducted relative to a stationary background frame of reference, thereby being an effective training stimulus to improve magno-parvo integration deficits at both early and higher levels of motion processing (Lawton, 2016; Lawton and Shelley-Tremblay, 2017). The background frame of reference entrains motion discrimination, since it repeats at the same periodicity (harmonically-related) as the test pattern moving left or right (Lawton, 1985). Increasing the ease of magno-parvo integration by oscillatory entrainment of motion discrimination facilitates figure/ground discrimination within a wider window of focused attention, improving reading skills by strengthening coupled theta/gamma and alpha/gamma activity (Lawton, 2016; Lawton and Shelley-Tremblay, 2017).

In addition, the possibility of a multi-modal auditory and visual temporal sampling deficit in dyslexia opens the door for remedial cross-domain behavioral entrainment. For example, auditory entrainment (typically a rhythmic pulse) has been shown to improve temporal allocation of visual attention (Ronconi et al., 2016a) as well as conscious visual processing (Ronconi et al., 2016b). Furthermore, cross-domain motor-to-auditory entrainment can be seen with tactile behavioral tasks (Plöchl et al., 2016). Along with entrainment in the visual and auditory domains, behavioral entrainment that also incorporates the motor-kinaesthetic domain might optimally enhance phase-locking required for reading. Remarkably, studies have demonstrated a remedial effect of playing action video games (AVGs) – which by design incorporate rapid and repetitive auditory, visual and motor stimulation – in children with dyslexia. After two weeks of daily AVG playing, children with dyslexia have demonstrated significant improvements in attentional and reading abilities (Franceschini et al., 2013), reading speed – without any cost in reading accuracy (Franceschini et al., 2013), word recognition, visuo-spatial attention, visual-to-auditory attention shifting (Franceschini et al., 2017), phonological decoding and short-term phonological memory (Franceschini et al., 2017; Franceschini and Bertoni, 2019). Furthermore, AVG players show improvements in dorsal processing, including motion discrimination (Hutchinson and Stocks, 2013) and contrast sensitivity (Li et al., 2009). Given the high-temporal low-spatial visual and auditory characteristics of AVGs, as well as the high-temporal motor-kinaesthetic response necessitated when playing, it is possible that AVGs stimulate oscillations across sensory domains in multi-modal magnocellular pathway.

Could Magnocellular Oscillatory Impairment Underlie the Auditory and Motor Deficits in Dyslexia?

Magnocells are not unique to the visual system, and communication between the visual and auditory domains, which is essential for rapid visuo-phonological mapping during reading, may occur via magnocellular oscillations. Magnocellular layers are also found in the medial geniculate body (MGB) of the auditory thalamus (Wu et al., 2015). Action potentials of MGB magnocells are tone-evoked and have the capacity for rapid development of discharge plasticity in response to auditory signals, enabling the temporal sequencing of transient sounds (Gerren and Weinberger, 1983; Wu et al., 2015). Furthermore, synaptic communication between sensory domains relies heavily on temporal sampling (Wu et al., 2015), and there is significant evidence that oscillatory entrainment in response to behavioral stimuli occurs simultaneously in the auditory and visual domains (Cooper, 1974; Calvert et al., 1997; Hasselmo, 2005; Meltzer et al., 2007; Jaušovec and Jaušovec, 2014). As well as auditory input, the magnocellular MGB receives visual afferents, contributing to a cross-sensory “conditional circuitry” (Gerren and Weinberger, 1983; Wu et al., 2015). Conversely, the PPC receives auditory as well as dorsal stream visual input. Furthermore, cross-modal oscillatory communication appears to occur with gamma synchronization (Fell et al., 2003), consistent with evidence of gamma involvement in reading and dyslexia (Lehongre et al., 2011). Thus, as well as enabling cognitive processing during reading, gamma oscillations might be integral to neuronal interaction between the visual and auditory pathways.

Beyond auditory and visual processing difficulties, children with dyslexia often display difficulties with fine motor skills (Nicolson and Fawcett, 1994; Fawcett and Nicolson, 1995), balance (Nicolson and Fawcett, 1994) and articulation (which requires verbal motor-kinaesthetic skills) (Fawcett and Nicolson, 1995). As well as oculomotor control during reading, magnocells are involved in generalized motor signals (Kennedy, 1987; Robinson et al., 1987; Van Kan and McCurdy, 2001), and it has been suggested that the motor deficits in dyslexia are also the result of a damaged magnocellular network (Stein and Talcott, 1999; Stein, 2019).

A possible region of interest for future investigations into a multi-modal magnocellular entrainment deficit could be the posterior parietal cortex. The PPC is in one way a “hub” for cross-domain neuronal interactions – it receives visual, auditory and somatosensory input, as well as processing premotor signals. Differences between dyslexic and non-dyslexic readers in processing temporal information in the magnocellular layers of the PPC would support the hypothesis of a multimodal magnocellular oscillatory network.

Conclusion

Like the temporal sampling of syllabic speech-sounds hypothesized in the auditory TSF, visual processing of text requires the rapid serial sampling of text. This occurs via sequential fixations that enable effective coordination of the cognitive visual process required to read, e.g., visual attention, feature binding and spatial coding (Cornelissen and Hansen, 1998; Rayner, 1998; Vidyasagar, 1999, 2013; Vidyasagar and Pammer, 1999, 2010; Pammer et al., 2004a, b; Facoetti et al., 2006, 2010; Sireteanu et al., 2008; Pammer, 2014; Moore and Schwitzgebel, 2018). Given that text is a static stimulus, temporal coding occurs via rapid processing of visual information that is gathered during saccades and fixations. It has been highlighted that during reading, these fixations occur every 200–300 ms (Rayner, 1998) – a theta frequency (Pammer, 2014). Here we propose that this is reflective of theta synchronized oscillatory activity in lower-order areas of the visual pathway, mirroring the auditory domain’s theta synchronization in response to speech sounds. However, dyslexic readers demonstrate significant differences in oculomotor control during reading, with no regular rhythm of saccades and fixations. We suggest that this signifies a deficit in visual temporal sampling of text and synchronization of oscillatory populations – specifically magnocellular populations in the dorsal stream. Given the evidence for differences in gamma synchronization between typical and dyslexic readers in higher order dorsal stream areas, along with evidence for a functional cross-frequency coupling relationship between theta and gamma oscillations, we suggest that theta and gamma oscillations couple along the dorsal stream to enable coding of text.

We have presented two alternate hypotheses for a magno-dorsal theta-gamma TSF. Model 1 is a bottom-up neuronal entrainment that directly parallels temporal sampling of spoken language in the auditory domain. In this model, we hypothesize that theta frequency eye movements during reading might entrain theta phase-locking in magnocellular populations, initiating theta–gamma coupling along the magno-dorsal pathway for the higher-order processing of text. Conversely, dyslexic readers who have unstable eye movements during reading might have no stable theta stimulus to entrain to, resulting in impaired theta phase-locking and a theta-gamma coupling deficit. The alternative hypothesis, Model 2, is a dynamic oscillatory network where theta and gamma oscillations interact in a top-down-bottom-up neuronal circuit. This visual TSF involves goal orientated “top-down” gamma signals from higher-order visual processing which couple with theta oscillatory activity in lower-order areas. Temporal sampling of text at a theta frequency may bolster theta oscillatory synchronization and reinforce coupling to gamma as visual input is fed back along the dorsal and ventral streams for higher-order processing.

The unique characteristics of magnocells suggest magnocellular oscillations play a key role in spatio-temporal capture and coding of text. Thus, a visual neuronal network which is hindered due to magnocellular anomalies in dyslexia may not only account for difficulties in low-spatial/high-temporal frequency processing, but also be the theoretical link between the TSF and magno-dorsal deficit hypotheses of dyslexia. Future testing of the presented hypotheses of a magno-dorsal TSF may begin to unite a number of disparate areas of visual processing and dyslexia to provide a cohesive, neurophysiological model of dyslexia.

Author Contributions

KA, KP, and TV contributed to the concepts and hypotheses, manuscript revision, and read and approved the submitted version. KA wrote the first draft of the manuscript.

Funding

KA’s research is funded by a Research Training Scheme scholarship, TV’s experimental work cited in this article was supported by grants from the Australian National Health and Medical Research Council and the Australian Research Council.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Footnotes

  1. ^ For convenience, from this point on, we will use the term “magno-dorsal” as we recognize that it is difficult to disentangle impairments in the cellular makeup (magno) of the dorsal pathway from functioning of the pathway itself.

References

Adler-Grinberg, D., and Stark, L. (1978). Eye movements, scanpaths, and dyslexia. Am. J. Optom. Physiol. Optics 55:557. doi: 10.1097/00006324-197808000-00005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Agarwal, R., and Sarma, S. V. (2012). Performance limitations of relay neurons. PLoS Comput. Biol. 8:e1002626. doi: 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1002626

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Auvichayapat, N., Rotenberg, A., Gersner, R., Ngodklang, S., Tiamkao, S., Tassaneeyakul, W., et al. (2013). Transcranial direct current stimulation for treatment of refractory childhood focal epilepsy. Brain Stimul. 6, 696–700. doi: 10.1016/j.brs.2013.01.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Axmacher, N., Henseler, M. M., Jensen, O., Weinreich, I., Elger, C. E., and Fell, J. (2010). Cross-frequency coupling supports multi-item working memory in the human hippocampus. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 107, 3228–3233. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0911531107

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Badcock, D., and Lovegrove, W. (1981). The effects of contrast, stimulus duration, and spatial frequency on visible persistence in normal and specifically disabled readers. J. Exp. Psychol. 7:495. doi: 10.1037/0096-1523.7.3.495

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bakola, S., Gregoriou, G. G., Moschovakis, A. K., Raos, V., and Savaki, H. E. (2007). Saccade-related information in the superior temporal motion complex: quantitative functional mapping in the monkey. J. Neurosci. 27, 2224–2229. doi: 10.1523/jneurosci.4224-06.2007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Baruth, J. M., Casanova, M. F., El-Baz, A., Horrell, T., Mathai, G., Sears, L., et al. (2010). Low-frequency repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation (rTMS) modulates evoked-gamma frequency oscillations in autism spectrum disorder (ASD). J. Neurother. 14, 179–194. doi: 10.1080/10874208.2010.501500

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bastiaansen, M., Magyari, L., and Hagoort, P. (2010). Syntactic unification operations are reflected in oscillatory dynamics during on-line sentence comprehension. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 22, 1333–1347. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2009.21283

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bastiaansen, M. C., Linden, M. V. D., Keurs, M. T., Dijkstra, T., and Hagoort, P. (2005). Theta responses are involved in lexical—Semantic retrieval during language processing. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 17, 530–541. doi: 10.1162/0898929053279469

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bastiaansen, M. C., Oostenveld, R., Jensen, O., and Hagoort, P. (2008). I see what you mean: theta power increases are involved in the retrieval of lexical semantic information. Brain Lang. 106, 15–28. doi: 10.1016/j.bandl.2007.10.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bastos, A. M., Briggs, F., Alitto, H. J., Mangun, G. R., and Usrey, W. M. (2014). Simultaneous recordings from the primary visual cortex and lateral geniculate nucleus reveal rhythmic interactions and a cortical source for gamma-band oscillations. J. Neurosci. 34, 7639–7644. doi: 10.1523/jneurosci.4216-13.2014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bastos, A. M., Usrey, W. M., Adams, R. A., Mangun, G. R., Fries, P., and Friston, K. J. (2012). Canonical microcircuits for predictive coding. Neuron 76, 695–711. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2012.10.038

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bedo, N., Ribary, U., and Ward, L. M. (2014). Fast dynamics of cortical functional and effective connectivity during word reading. PLoS One 9:e88940. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0088940

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Belluscio, M. A., Mizuseki, K., Schmidt, R., Kempter, R., and Buzsáki, G. (2012). Cross-frequency phase–phase coupling between theta and gamma oscillations in the hippocampus. J. Neurosci. 32, 423–435. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4122-11.2012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Benardete, E. A., Kaplan, E., and Knight, B. W. (1992). Contrast gain control in the primate retina: P cells are not X-like, some M cells are. Vis. Neurosci. 8, 483–486. doi: 10.1017/s0952523800004995

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Benfatto, M. N., Seimyr, G. Ö, Ygge, J., Pansell, T., Rydberg, A., and Jacobson, C. (2016). Screening for dyslexia using eye tracking during reading. PLoS One 11:e0165508. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0165508

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bizzi, E. (1968). Discharge of frontal eye field neurons during saccadic and following eye movements in unanesthetized monkeys. Exp. Brain Res. 6, 69–80.

Google Scholar

Bloch, Y., Grisaru, N., Harel, E. V., Beitler, G., Faivel, N., Ratzoni, G., et al. (2008). Repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation in the treatment of depression in adolescents: an open-label study. J. ECT 24, 156–159. doi: 10.1097/YCT.0b013e318156aa49c

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Boi, M., Poletti, M., Victor, J. D., and Rucci, M. (2017). Consequences of the oculomotor cycle for the dynamics of perception. Curr. Biol. 27, 1268–1277. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2017.03.034

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bosman, C. A., Schoffelen, J. M., Brunet, N., Oostenveld, R., Bastos, A. M., Womelsdorf, T., et al. (2012). Attentional stimulus selection through selective synchronization between monkey visual areas. Neuron 75, 875–888. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2012.06.037

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bragin, A., Jandó, G., Nádasdy, Z., Hetke, J., Wise, K., and Buzsáki, G. (1995). Gamma (40-100 Hz) oscillation in the hippocampus of the behaving rat. J. Neurosci. 15, 47–60. doi: 10.1523/jneurosci.15-01-00047.1995

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brefczynski, J. A., and DeYoe, E. A. (1999). A physiological correlate of the’spotlight’of visual attention. Nat. Neurosci. 2, 370–374. doi: 10.1038/7280

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bressler, S. L., and Menon, V. (2010). Large-scale brain networks in cognition: emerging methods and principles. Trends Cogn. Sci. 14, 277–290. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2010.04.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Briggs, F., and Usrey, W. M. (2007). A fast, reciprocal pathway between the lateral geniculate nucleus and visual cortex in the macaque monkey. J. Neurosci. 27, 5431–5436. doi: 10.1523/jneurosci.1035-07.2007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bruce, C. J., and Goldberg, M. E. (1985). Primate frontal eye fields. I. Single neurons discharging before saccades. J. Neurophysiol. 53, 603–635. doi: 10.1152/jn.1985.53.3.603

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bruce, C. J., Goldberg, M. E., Bushnell, M. C., and Stanton, G. B. (1985). Primate frontal eye fields. II. Physiological and anatomical correlates of electrically evoked eye movements. J. Neurophysiol. 54, 714–734. doi: 10.1152/jn.1985.54.3.714

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bruno, R. M. (2011). Synchrony in sensation. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 21, 701–708. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2011.06.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Buschman, T. J., and Miller, E. K. (2007). Top-down versus bottom-up control of attention in the prefrontal and posterior parietal cortices. Science 315, 1860–1862. doi: 10.1126/science.1138071

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Buschman, T. J., and Miller, E. K. (2014). Goal-direction and top-down control. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B Biol. Sci. 369:20130471. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2013.0471

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Butts, D. A., Weng, C., Jin, J., Alonso, J. M., and Paninski, L. (2011). Temporal precision in the visual pathway through the interplay of excitation and stimulus-driven suppression. J. Neurosci. 31, 11313–11327. doi: 10.1523/jneurosci.0434-11.2011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Buzsáki, G., and Draguhn, A. (2004). Neuronal oscillations in cortical networks. Science 304, 1926–1929. doi: 10.1126/science.1099745

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Calderone, D. J., Lakatos, P., Butler, P. D., and Castellanos, F. X. (2014). Entrainment of neural oscillations as a modifiable substrate of attention. Trends Cogn. Sci. 18, 300–309. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2014.02.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Calvert, G. A., Bullmore, E. T., Brammer, M. J., Campbell, R., Williams, S. C., McGuire, P. K., et al. (1997). Activation of auditory cortex during silent lipreading. Science 276, 593–596. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2001.0812

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Canolty, R. T., Edwards, E., Dalal, S. S., Soltani, M., Nagarajan, S. S., Kirsch, H. E., et al. (2006). High gamma power is phase-locked to theta oscillations in human neocortex. Science 313, 1626–1628. doi: 10.1126/science.1128115

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Canolty, R. T., and Knight, R. T. (2010). The functional role of cross-frequency coupling. Trends Cogn. Sci. 14, 506–515. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2010.09.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Castles, A., and Coltheart, M. (2004). Is there a causal link from phonological awareness to success in learning to read? Cognition 91, 77–111. doi: 10.1016/s0010-0277(03)00164-1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Castelo-Branco, M., Neuenschwander, S., and Singer, W. (1998). Synchronization of visual responses between the cortex, lateral geniculate nucleus, and retina in the anesthetized cat. J. Neurosci. 18, 6395–6410. doi: 10.1523/jneurosci.18-16-06395.1998

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chelazzi, L., Della Libera, C., Sani, I., and Santandrea, E. (2011). Neural basis of visual selective attention. Wiley Interdiscipl. Rev. 2, 392–407. doi: 10.1002/wcs.117

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Colling, L. J., Noble, H. L., and Goswami, U. (2017). Neural entrainment and sensorimotor synchronization to the beat in children with developmental dyslexia: an EEG study. Front. Neurosci. 11:360. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2017.00360

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cook, M. L., and Stevens, J. G. (1973). Pathogenesis of herpetic neuritis and ganglionitis in mice: evidence for intra-axonal transport of infection. Infect. Immun. 7, 272–288. doi: 10.1128/iai.7.2.272-288.1973

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cooper, R. M. (1974). The control of eye fixation by the meaning of spoken language: a new methodology for the real-time investigation of speech perception, memory, and language processing. Cogn. Psychol. 96, B23–B32.

Google Scholar

Corbetta, M., and Shulman, G. L. (2002). Control of goal-directed and stimulus-driven attention in the brain. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 3:201. doi: 10.1038/nrn755

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cornelissen, P., Richardson, A., Mason, A., Fowler, S., and Stein, J. (1995). Contrast sensitivity and coherent motion detection measured at photopic luminance levels in dyslexics and controls. Vis. Res. 35, 1483–1494. doi: 10.1016/0042-6989(95)98728-r

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cornelissen, P. L., and Hansen, P. C. (1998). Motion detection, letter position encoding, and single word reading. Ann. Dyslexia 48, 155–188. doi: 10.1007/s11881-998-0008-3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cowan, W. M. (1970). “Anterograde and retrograde transneuronal degeneration in the central and peripheral nervous system,” in Contemporary Research Methods in Neuroanatomy, eds W. J. H. Nauta and S. O. E. Ebbesson (Berlin: Springer), 217–251. doi: 10.1007/978-3-642-85986-1_11

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Creutzfeldt, O. D., and Kuhnt, U. (1973). “Electrophysiology and topographical distribution of visual evoked potentials in animals,” in Visual Centers in the Brain, Ed. R. Jung (Berlin: Springer), 595–646. doi: 10.1007/978-3-642-65495-4_15

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Crick, F. C., and Koch, C. (2005). What is the function of the claustrum? Philos. Trans. Roy. Soc. Lond. B Biol. Sci. 36, 1271–1279. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2005.1661

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

de Graaf, T. A., Gross, J., Paterson, G., Rusch, T., Sack, A. T., and Thut, G. (2013). Alpha-band rhythms in visual task performance: phase-locking by rhythmic sensory stimulation. PLoS One 8:e60035. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0060035

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Demb, J. B., Boynton, G. M., and Heeger, D. J. (1998). Functional magnetic resonance imaging of early visual pathways in dyslexia. J. Neurosci. 18, 6939–6951. doi: 10.1523/jneurosci.18-17-06939.1998

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Demiralp, T., Bayraktaroglu, Z., Lenz, D., Junge, S., Busch, N. A., Maess, B., et al. (2007). Gamma amplitudes are coupled to theta phase in human EEG during visual perception. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 64, 24–30. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2006.07.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Destexhe, A., Contreras, D., and Steriade, M. (1999). Cortically-induced coherence of a thalamic-generated oscillation. Neuroscience 92, 427–443. doi: 10.1016/s0306-4522(99)00024-x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Di Garbo, A. (2008). The electrical coupling confers to a network of interneurons the ability of transmitting excitatory inputs with high temporal precision. Brain Res. 1225, 47–56. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2008.05.021

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dong, D. W., and Atick, J. J. (1995). Temporal decorrelation: a theory of lagged and nonlagged responses in the lateral geniculate nucleus. Network 6, 159–178. doi: 10.1088/0954-898x_6_2_003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dreher, B., Fukada, Y., and Rodieck, R. W. (1976). Identification, classification and anatomical segregation of cells with X-like and Y-like properties in the lateral geniculate nucleus of old-world primates. J. Physiol. 258, 433–452. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.1976.sp011429

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Eden, G. F., Stein, J. F., Wood, F. B., and Wood, H. M. (1994). Differences in eye movements and reading problems in dyslexic and normal children. Vis. Res. 34, 1345–1358. doi: 10.1016/0042-6989(94)90209-7

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Eden, G. F., VanMeter, J. W., Rumsey, J. M., Maisog, J. M., Woods, R. P., and Zeffiro, T. A. (1996). Abnormal processing of visual motion in dyslexia revealed by functional brain imaging. Nature 382:66. doi: 10.1038/382066a0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Engel, A. K., Fries, P., and Singer, W. (2001). Dynamic predictions: oscillations and synchrony in top-down processing. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2, 704–716. doi: 10.1038/35094565

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Engel, A. K., Kreiter, A. K., König, P., and Singer, W. (1991). Synchronization of oscillatory neuronal responses between striate and extrastriate visual cortical areas of the cat. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 88, 6048–6052. doi: 10.1073/pnas.88.14.6048

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Everling, S., and Munoz, D. P. (2000). Neuronal correlates for preparatory set associated with pro-saccades and anti-saccades in the primate frontal eye field. J. Neurosci. 20, 387–400. doi: 10.1523/jneurosci.20-01-00387.2000

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Eysel, U. T., and Wolfhard, U. (1984). The effects of partial retinal lesions on activity and size of cells in the dorsal lateral geniculate nucleus. J. Comp. Neurol. 229, 301–309. doi: 10.1002/cne.902290214

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Facoetti, A., Trussardi, A. N., Ruffino, M., Lorusso, M. L., Cattaneo, C., Galli, R., et al. (2010). Multisensory spatial attention deficits are predictive of phonological decoding skills in developmental dyslexia. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 22, 1011–1025. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2009.21232

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Facoetti, A., Zorzi, M., Cestnick, L., Lorusso, M. L., Molteni, M., Paganoni, P., et al. (2006). The relationship between visuo-spatial attention and nonword reading in developmental dyslexia. Cogn. Neuropsychol. 23, 841–855. doi: 10.1162/jocn.2009.21232

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fawcett, A. J., and Nicolson, R. I. (1995). Persistent deficits in motor skill of children with dyslexia. J. Mot. Behav. 27, 235–240. doi: 10.1080/00222895.1995.9941713

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fawcett, A. J., Nicolson, R. I., and Dean, P. (1996). Impaired performance of children with dyslexia on a range of cerebellar tasks. Ann. Dyslexia 46, 259–283. doi: 10.1007/bf02648179

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fell, J., Fernandez, G., Klaver, P., Elger, C. E., and Fries, P. (2003). Is synchronized neuronal gamma activity relevant for selective attention? Brain Res. Rev. 42, 265–272. doi: 10.1016/s0165-0173(03)00178-4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fertonani, A., and Miniussi, C. (2018). Transcranial electrical stimulation: what we know and do not know about mechanisms. Neuroscientist 23, 109–123. doi: 10.1177/1073858416631966

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fischer, B., and Hartnegg, K. (2000). Effects of visual training on saccade control in dyslexia. Perception 29, 531–542. doi: 10.1068/p2666c

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Flint, S., and Pammer, K. (2011). Coherent motion detection in illiterate, literate and dyslexic readers in Papua New Guinea. i-Perception 2, 268–268. doi: 10.1068/ic268

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Flint, S., and Pammer, K. (2019). It is the egg, not the chicken; dorsal visual deficits present in dyslexia are not present in illiterate adults. Dyslexia 25, 69–83.

Google Scholar

Fontolan, L., Morillon, B., Liegeois-Chauvel, C., and Giraud, A. L. (2014). The contribution of frequency-specific activity to hierarchical information processing in the human auditory cortex. Nat. Commun. 5:4694.

Google Scholar

Franceschini, S., and Bertoni, S. (2019). Improving action video games abilities increases the phonological decoding speed and phonological short-term memory in children with developmental dyslexia. Neuropsychologia 130, 100–106. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2018.10.023

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Franceschini, S., Gori, S., Ruffino, M., Viola, S., Molteni, M., and Facoetti, A. (2013). Action video games make dyslexic children read better. Curr. Biol. 23, 462–466. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2013.01.044

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Franceschini, S., Trevisan, P., Ronconi, L., Bertoni, S., Colmar, S., Double, K., et al. (2017). Action video games improve reading abilities and visual-to-auditory attentional shifting in English-speaking children with dyslexia. Sci. Rep. 7, 1–12.

Google Scholar

Frien, A., Eckhorn, R., Bauer, R., Woelbern, T., and Kehr, H. (1994). Stimulus-specific fast oscillations at zero phase between visual areas V1 and V2 of awake monkey. Neuroreport 5, 2273–2277. doi: 10.1097/00001756-199411000-00017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fries, P., Reynolds, J. H., Rorie, A. E., and Desimone, R. (2001). Modulation of oscillatory neuronal synchronization by selective visual attention. Science 291, 1560–1563. doi: 10.1126/science.1055465

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fries, P., Schröder, J. H., Roelfsema, P. R., Singer, W., and Engel, A. K. (2002). Oscillatory neuronal synchronization in primary visual cortex as a correlate of stimulus selection. J. Neurosci. 22, 3739–3754. doi: 10.1523/jneurosci.22-09-03739.2002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Friese, U., Köster, M., Hassler, U., Martens, U., Trujillo-Barreto, N., and Gruber, T. (2013). Successful memory encoding is associated with increased cross-frequency coupling between frontal theta and posterior gamma oscillations in human scalp-recorded EEG. Neuroimage 66, 642–647. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.11.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Funke, K., and Wörgötter, F. (1997). On the significance of temporally structured activity in the dorsal lateral geniculate nucleus (LGN). Progr. Neurobiol. 53, 67–119. doi: 10.1016/S0301-0082(97)00032-4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Galaburda, A. M., Menard, M. T., and Rosen, G. D. (1994). Evidence for aberrant auditory anatomy in developmental dyslexia. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 91, 8010–8013. doi: 10.1073/pnas.91.17.8010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gawne, T. J., Richmond, B. J., and Optican, L. M. (1991). Interactive effects among several stimulus parameters on the responses of striate cortical complex cells. J. Neurophysiol. 66, 379–389. doi: 10.1152/jn.1991.66.2.379

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gerren, R. A., and Weinberger, N. M. (1983). Long term potentiation in the magnocellular medial geniculate nucleus of the anesthetized cat. Brain Res. 265, 138–142. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(83)91344-6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ghitza, O. (2011). Linking speech perception and neurophysiology: speech decoding guided by cascaded oscillators locked to the input rhythm. Front. Psychol. 2:130. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00130

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Giraldo-Chica, M., Hegarty, J. P. II, and Schneider, K. A. (2015). Morphological differences in the lateral geniculate nucleus associated with dyslexia. Neuroimage 7, 830–836. doi: 10.1016/j.nicl.2015.03.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Giraud, A. L., and Poeppel, D. (2012). Cortical oscillations and speech processing: emerging computational principles and operations. Nat. Neurosci. 15:511. doi: 10.1038/nn.3063

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gitelman, D. R., Nobre, A. C., Parrish, T. B., LaBar, K. S., Kim, Y. H., Meyer, J. R., et al. (1999). A large-scale distributed network for covert spatial attention: further anatomical delineation based on stringent behavioral and cognitive controls. Brain 122, 1093–1106. doi: 10.1093/brain/122.6.1093

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

González, G. F., Van der Molen, M. J. W., Žarić, G., Bonte, M., Tijms, J., Blomert, L., et al. (2016). Graph analysis of EEG resting state functional networks in dyslexic readers. Clin. Neurophysiol. 127, 3165–3175. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2016.06.023

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gori, S., Cecchini, P., Bigoni, A., Molteni, M., and Facoetti, A. (2014). Magnocellular-dorsal pathway and sub-lexical route in developmental dyslexia. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 8:460. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2014.00460

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Goswami, U. (2011). A temporal sampling framework for developmental dyslexia. Trends Cogn. Sci. 15, 3–10. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2010.10.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Goswami, U. (2018). A neural basis for phonological awareness? An oscillatory temporal-sampling perspective. Curr. Direct. Psychol. Sci. 27, 56–63. doi: 10.1177/0963721417727520

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Goswami, U., and Bryant, P. (1990). Phonological Skills and Learning to Read. London: Erlbaum.

Google Scholar

Goswami, U., and Leong, V. (2013). Speech rhythm and temporal structure: converging perspectives. Lab. Phonol. 4, 67–92. doi: 10.1515/lp-2013-0004

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Goswami, U., Mead, N., Fosker, T., Huss, M., Barnes, L., and Leong, V. (2013). Impaired perception of syllable stress in children with dyslexia: a longitudinal study. J. Mem. Lang. 69, 1–17. doi: 10.1016/j.jml.2013.03.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Goswami, U., Power, A. J., Lallier, M., and Facoetti, A. (2014). Oscillatory “temporal sampling” and developmental dyslexia: toward an over-arching theoretical framework. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 8:904. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2014.00904

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Graboi, D., and Lisman, J. (2003). Recognition by top-down and bottom-up processing in cortex: the control of selective attention. J. Neurophysiol. 90, 798–810. doi: 10.1152/jn.00777.2002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gray, C. M., and Singer, W. (1989). Stimulus-specific neuronal oscillations in orientation columns of cat visual cortex. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 86, 1698–1702. doi: 10.1073/pnas.86.5.1698

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gross, J., Hoogenboom, N., Thut, G., Schyns, P., Panzeri, S., Belin, P., et al. (2013). Speech rhythms and multiplexed oscillatory sensory coding in the human brain. PLoS Biol. 11:e1001752. doi: 10.1371/journal.pbio.1001752

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hald, L. A., Bastiaansen, M. C., and Hagoort, P. (2006). EEG theta and gamma responses to semantic violations in online sentence processing. Brain Lang. 96, 90–105. doi: 10.1016/j.bandl.2005.06.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hämäläinen, J. A., Rupp, A., Soltész, F., Szücs, D., and Goswami, U. (2012). Reduced phase locking to slow amplitude modulation in adults with dyslexia: an MEG study. Neuroimage 59, 2952–2961. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.09.075

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hancock, R., Pugh, K. R., and Hoeft, F. (2017). Neural noise hypothesis of developmental dyslexia. Trends Cogn. Sci. 21, 434–448. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2017.03.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hasselmo, M. E. (2005). What is the function of hippocampal theta rhythm?—Linking behavioral data to phasic properties of field potential and unit recording data. Hippocampus 15, 936–949. doi: 10.1002/hipo.20116

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hawellek, D. J., Wong, Y. T., and Pesaran, B. (2016). Temporal coding of reward-guided choice in the posterior parietal cortex. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 113, 13492–13497. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1606479113

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Herrmann, C. S. (2001). Human EEG responses to 1–100 Hz flicker: resonance phenomena in visual cortex and their potential correlation to cognitive phenomena. Exp. Brain Res. 137, 346–353. doi: 10.1007/s002210100682

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Herrmann, C. S., and Mecklinger, A. (2000). Magnetoencephalographic responses to illusory figures: early evoked gamma is affected by processing of stimulus features. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 38, 265–281. doi: 10.1016/s0167-8760(00)00170-7

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Herrmann, C. S., and Mecklinger, A. (2001). Gamma activity in human EEG is related to highspeed memory comparisons during object selective attention. Vis. Cogn. 8, 593–608. doi: 10.1080/13506280143000142

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Heth, I., and Lavidor, M. (2015). Improved reading measures in adults with dyslexia following transcranial direct current stimulation treatment. Neuropsychologia 70, 107–113. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2015.02.022

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hicks, T. P., Lee, B. B., and Vidyasagar, T. R. (1983). The responses of cells in the macaque lateral geniculate nucleus to sinusoidal gratings. J. Physiol. 337, 183–200. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.1983.sp014619

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Holz, E. M., Glennon, M., Prendergast, K., and Sauseng, P. (2010). Theta–gamma phase synchronization during memory matching in visual working memory. Neuroimage 52, 326–335. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.04.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hopfinger, J. B., Buonocore, M. H., and Mangun, G. R. (2000). The neural mechanisms of top-down attentional control. Nat. Neurosci. 3:284. doi: 10.1038/72999

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Horvath, J. C., Carter, O., and Forte, J. D. (2016). No significant effect of transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS) found on simple motor reaction time comparing 15 different simulation protocols. Neuropsychologia 91, 544–552. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2016.09.017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hubel, D. H., and Wiesel, T. N. (1961). Integrative action in the cat’s lateral geniculate body. J. Physiol. 155, 385–398. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.1961.sp006635

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Humphreys, G. W., Evett, L. J., and Quinlan, P. T. (1990). Orthographic processing in visual word identification. Cognit. Psychol. 22, 517–560. doi: 10.1016/0010-0285(90)90012-S

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hunt, E., Lunneborg, C., and Lewis, J. (1975). What does it mean to be high verbal? Cogn. Psychol. 7, 194–227. doi: 10.1016/0010-0285(75)90010-9

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Huss, M., Verney, J. P., Fosker, T., Mead, N., and Goswami, U. (2011). Music, rhythm, rise time perception and developmental dyslexia: perception of musical meter predicts reading and phonology. Cortex 47, 674–689. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2010.07.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hutcheon, B., and Yarom, Y. (2000). Resonance, oscillation and intrinsic frequency preference of neurons. Trends Neurosci. 23, 216–222. doi: 10.1016/s0166-2236(00)01547-2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hutchinson, C. V., and Stocks, R. (2013). Selectively enhanced motion perception in core video gamers. Perception 42, 675–677. doi: 10.1068/p7411

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hyafil, A., Fontolan, L., Kabdebon, C., Gutkin, B., and Giraud, A. L. (2015). Speech encoding by coupled cortical theta and gamma oscillations. eLife 4:e06213. doi: 10.7554/eLife.06213

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hyafil, A., Gutkin, B., and Giraud, A. L. (2011). “A theoretical exploration of speech/neural oscillation alignment for speech parsing,” in Proceedings of the Conference Abstract: XI International Conference on Cognitive Neuroscience (ICON XI), (Oldenburg: University of Oldenburg).

Google Scholar

Itti, L., and Koch, C. (2001). Computational modelling of visual attention. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2:194.

Google Scholar

Izawa, Y., Suzuki, H., and Shinoda, Y. (2004). Suppression of visually and memory-guided saccades induced by electrical stimulation of the monkey frontal eye field. I. Suppression of ipsilateral saccades. J. Neurophysiol. 92, 2248–2260. doi: 10.1152/jn.01021.2003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Izawa, Y., Suzuki, H., and Shinoda, Y. (2009). Response properties of fixation neurons and their location in the frontal eye field in the monkey. J. Neurophysiol. 102, 2410–2422. doi: 10.1152/jn.00234.2009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jainta, S., and Kapoula, Z. (2011). Dyslexic children are confronted with unstable binocular fixation while reading. PLoS One 6:e18694. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0018694

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jaušovec, K., and Jaušovec, N. (2014). Increasing working memory capacity with theta transcranial alternating current stimulation (tACS). Biol. Psychol. 96, 42–47. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2013.11.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jensen, O., and Colgin, L. L. (2007). Cross-frequency coupling between neuronal oscillations. Trends Cogn. Sci. 11, 267–269. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2007.05.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jiang, H., Bahramisharif, A., van Gerven, M. A., and Jensen, O. (2015). Measuring directionality between neuronal oscillations of different frequencies. Neuroimage 118, 359–367. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.05.044

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jiménez-Bravo, M., Marrero, V., and Benitez-Burraco, A. (2017). An oscillopathic approach to developmental dyslexia: from genes to speech processing. Behav. Brain Res. 329, 84–95. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2017.03.048

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Johnston, A., Bruno, A., Watanabe, J., Quansah, B., Patel, N., Dakin, S., et al. (2008). Visually-based temporal distortion in dyslexia. Vis. Res. 48, 1852–1858. doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2008.04.029

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kaplan, E., and Shapley, R. M. (1982). X and Y cells in the lateral geniculate nucleus of macaque monkeys. J. Physiol. 330, 125–143. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.1982.sp014333

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kelly, D. H. (1966). Frequency doubling in visual responses. JOSA 56, 1628–1633. doi: 10.1364/JOSA.56.001628

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kelly, D. H. (1981). Nonlinear visual responses to flickering sinusoidal gratings. JOSA 71, 1051–1055. doi: 10.1364/JOSA.71.001051

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kennedy, P. R. (1987). Parametric relationships of individual digit movements to neuronal discharges in primate magnocellular red nucleus. Brain Res. 417, 185–189. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(87)90198-3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kepecs, A., and Lisman, J. (2003). Information encoding and computation with spikes and bursts. Network 14, 103–118. doi: 10.1080/net.14.1.103.118

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kersey, A. J., and James, K. H. (2013). Brain activation patterns resulting from learning letter forms through active self-production and passive observation in young children. Front. Psychol. 4:567. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00567

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kevan, A., and Pammer, K. (2008). Visual processing deficits in preliterate children at familial risk for dyslexia. Vis. Res. 48, 2835–2839. doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2008.09.022

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kizuk, S. A., and Mathewson, K. E. (2017). Power and phase of alpha oscillations revealan interaction between spatial and temporal visual attention. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 29, 480–494. doi: 10.1162/jocn_a_01058

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Klimesch, W., Doppelmayr, M., Yonelinas, A., Kroll, N. E., Lazzara, M., Roehm, D., et al. (2001). Theta synchronization during episodic retrieval: neural correlates of conscious awareness. Cogn. Brain Res. 12, 33–38. doi: 10.1016/s0926-6410(01)00024-6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kraus, N. (2012). Atypical brain oscillations: a biological basis for dyslexia? Trends Cogn. Sci. 16, 12–13. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2011.12.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Krauzlis, R. J. (2004). Recasting the smooth pursuit eye movement system. J. Neurophysiol. 91, 591–603. doi: 10.1152/jn.00801.2003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kreiter, A. K., and Singer, W. (1996). Stimulus-dependent synchronization of neuronal responses in the visual cortex of the awake macaque monkey. J. Neurosci. 16, 2381–2396. doi: 10.1523/jneurosci.16-07-02381.1996

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kremkow, J., Perrinet, L. U., Masson, G. S., and Aertsen, A. (2010). Functional consequences of correlated excitatory and inhibitory conductances in cortical networks. J. Comput. Neurosci. 28, 579–594. doi: 10.1007/s10827-010-0240-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kruse, W., and Eckhorn, R. (1996). Inhibition of sustained gamma oscillations (35-80 Hz) by fast transient responses in cat visual cortex. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 93, 6112–6117. doi: 10.1073/pnas.93.12.6112

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kwon, H. J., Lim, W. S., Lim, M. H., Lee, S. J., Hyun, J. K., Chae, J. H., et al. (2011). 1-Hz low frequency repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation in children with Tourette’s syndrome. Neurosci. Lett. 492, 1–4. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2011.01.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lachaux, J. P., Hoffmann, D., Minotti, L., Berthoz, A., and Kahane, P. (2006). Intracerebral dynamics of saccade generation in the human frontal eye field and supplementary eye field. Neuroimage 30, 1302–1312. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.11.023

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lakatos, P., Chen, C. M., O’Connell, M. N., Mills, A., and Schroeder, C. E. (2007). Neuronal oscillations and multisensory interaction in primary auditory cortex. Neuron 53, 279–292. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2006.12.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lakatos, P., Karmos, G., Mehta, A. D., Ulbert, I., and Schroeder, C. E. (2008). Entrainment of neuronal oscillations as a mechanism of attentional selection. Science 320, 110–113. doi: 10.1126/science.1154735

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lakatos, P., Shah, A. S., Knuth, K. H., Ulbert, I., Karmos, G., and Schroeder, C. E. (2005). An oscillatory hierarchy controlling neuronal excitability and stimulus processing in the auditory cortex. J. Neurophysiol. 94, 1904–1911. doi: 10.1152/jn.00263.2005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Larroche, J. C. (1986). Fetal encephalopathies of circulatory origin. Neonatology 50, 61–74. doi: 10.1159/000242570

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lawton, T. (2016). Improving dorsal stream function in dyslecxics by training figure/ground motion discrimination improves attention, reading fliuency and working memory. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 10:397. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2016.00397

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lawton, T., and Shelley-Tremblay, J. (2017). Training on movement figure-ground discrimination remediates low-level visual timing deficits in the dorsal stream, improving high-level cognitive functioning, including attention, reading fluency, and memory. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 11:236. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2017.00236/full

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lawton, T. B. (1985). Spatial frequency spectrum of patterns changes the visibility of spatial-phase differences. J. Optical Soc. Am. A 2, 1140–1152.

Google Scholar

Laycock, R., and Crewther, S. G. (2008). Towards an understanding of the role of the ‘magnocellular advantage’ in fluent reading. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 32, 1494–1506. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2008.06.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Le Van Quyen, M. (2011). The brainweb of cross-scale interactions. New Ideas Psychol. 29, 57–63. doi: 10.1016/j.newideapsych.2010.11.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lee, D., and Malpeli, J. G. (1998). Effects of saccades on the activity of neurons in the cat lateral geniculate nucleus. J. Neurophysiol. 79, 922–936. doi: 10.1152/jn.1998.79.2.922

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lefton, L. A., Nagle, R. J., Johnson, G., and Fisher, D. F. (1979). Eye movement dynamics of good and poor readers: then and now. J. Literacy Res. 11, 319–328. doi: 10.1080/10862967909547338

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lehongre, K., Morillon, B., Giraud, A. L., and Ramus, F. (2013). Impaired auditory sampling in dyslexia: further evidence from combined fMRI and EEG. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 7:454. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00454

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lehongre, K., Ramus, F., Villiermet, N., Schwartz, D., and Giraud, A. L. (2011). Altered low-gamma sampling in auditory cortex accounts for the three main facets of dyslexia. Neuron 72, 1080–1090. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2011.11.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Leong, V., and Goswami, U. (2014). Impaired extraction of speech rhythm from temporal modulation patterns in speech in developmental dyslexia. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 8:96. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2014.00096

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Leong, V., Hämäläinen, J., Soltész, F., and Goswami, U. (2011). Rise time perception and detection of syllable stress in adults with developmental dyslexia. J. Mem. Lang. 64, 59–73. doi: 10.1016/j.jml.2010.09.003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Levichkina, E., Saalmann, Y. B., and Vidyasagar, T. R. (2017). Coding of spatial attention priorities and object features in the macaque lateral intraparietal cortex. Physiol. Rep. 5:e13136. doi: 10.14814/phy2.13136

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Levine, M., and Cleland, B. G. (2001). An analysis of the effect of retinal ganglion cell impulses upon the firing probability of neurons in the dorsal lateral geniculate nucleus of the cat. Brain Res. 902, 244–254. doi: 10.1016/s0006-8993(01)02411-8

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lewis, A. G., and Bastiaansen, M. (2015). A predictive coding framework for rapid neural dynamics during sentence-level language comprehension. Cortex 68, 155–168. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2015.02.014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Li, R., Polat, U., Makous, W., and Bavelier, D. (2009). Enhancing the contrast sensitivity function through action video game training. Nat. Neurosci. 12:549. doi: 10.1038/nn.2296

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lisman, J., and Buzsaki, G. (2008). A neural coding scheme formed by the combined function of gamma and theta oscillations. Schizophrenia Bull. 34, 974–980. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbn060

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lisman, J. E., and Idiart, M. A. (1995). Storage of 7±2 short-term memories in oscillatory subcycles. Science 267, 1512–1515. doi: 10.1126/science.7878473

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lisman, J. E., and Jensen, O. (2013). The Theta-gamma neural code. Neuron 77, 1002–1016. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2013.03.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Livingstone, M. S., Rosen, G. D., Drislane, F. W., and Galaburda, A. M. (1991). Physiological and anatomical evidence for a magnocellular defect in developmental dyslexia. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 88, 7943–7947. doi: 10.1073/pnas.88.18.7943

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lizarazu, M., Lallier, M., Molinaro, N., Bourguignon, M., Paz-Alonso, P. M., Lerma-Usabiaga, G., et al. (2015). Developmental evaluation of atypical auditory sampling in dyslexia: functional and structural evidence. Hum. Brain Mapp. 36, 4986–5002. doi: 10.1002/hbm.22986

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Llinás, R. (1993). Is dyslexia a dyschronia? Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 682, 48–56. doi: 10.1111/j.1749-6632.1993.tb22958.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lovegrove, B. (1996). Dyslexia and a transient/magnocellular pathway deficit: the current situation and future directions. Aust. J. Psychol. 48, 167–171. doi: 10.1080/00049539608259525

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Luna, B., Thulborn, K. R., Strojwas, M. H., McCurtain, B. J., Berman, R. A., Genovese, C. R., et al. (1998). Dorsal cortical regions subserving visually guided saccades in humans: an fMRI study. Cereb. Cortex 8, 40–47. doi: 10.1093/cercor/8.1.40

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Luo, H., and Poeppel, D. (2007). Phase patterns of neuronal responses reliably discriminate speech in human auditory cortex. Neuron 54, 1001–1010. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2007.06.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lutzenberger, W., Pulvermüller, F., Elbert, T., and Birbaumer, N. (1995). Visual stimulation alters local 40-Hz responses in humans: an EEG-study. Neurosci. Lett. 83, 39–42. doi: 10.1016/0304-3940(94)11109-v

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

MacAvoy, M. G., Gottlieb, J. P., and Bruce, C. J. (1991). Smooth-pursuit eye movement representation in the primate frontal eye field. Cereb. Cortex 1, 95–102. doi: 10.1093/cercor/1.1.95

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Maddess, T., and Henry, G. H. (1990). Density of Nonlinear visual units and glaucoma. Ophthalmol. Vis. Sci. 31:230.

Google Scholar

Marrocco, R. T., McClurkin, J. W., and Young, R. A. (1982). Spatial summation and conduction latency classification of cells of the lateral geniculate nucleus of macaques. J. Neurosci. 2, 1275–1291. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.02-09-01275.1982

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Marshall, T. R., Bergmann, T. O., and Jensen, O. (2015). Frontoparietal structural connectivity mediates the top-down control of neuronal synchronization associated with selective attention. PLoS Biol. 13:e1002272. doi: 10.1371/journal.pbio.1002272

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Martos, F. J., and Vila, J. (1990). Differences in eye movements control among dyslexic, retarded and normal readers in the Spanish population. Read. Writ. 2, 175–188. doi: 10.1007/bf00401801

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mathewson, K. E., Prudhomme, C., Fabiani, M., Beck, D. M., Lleras, A., and Gratton, G. (2012). Making waves in the stream of consciousness: entraining oscillations in EEG alpha and fluctuations in visual awareness with rhythmic visual stimulation. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 24, 2321–2333. doi: 10.1162/jocn_a_00288

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McAdams, C. J., and Reid, R. C. (2005). Attention modulates the responses of simple cells in monkey primary visual cortex. J. Neurosci. 25, 11023–11033. doi: 10.1523/jneurosci.2904-05.2005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McClurkin, J. W., Gawne, T. J., Optican, L. M., and Richmond, B. J. (1991). Lateral geniculate neurons in behaving primates. II. Encoding of visual information in the temporal shape of the response. J. Neurophysiol. 66, 794–808. doi: 10.1152/jn.1991.66.3.794

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McLelland, D., Lavergne, L., and VanRullen, R. (2016). The phase of ongoing EEG oscillations predict the amplitude of peri-saccadic mislocalization. Sci. Rep. 6:29335. doi: 10.1038/srep29335

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McPeek, R. M., Maljkovic, V., and Nakayama, K. (1999). Saccades require focal attention and are facilitated by a short-term memory system. Vis. Res. 39, 1555–1566. doi: 10.1016/s0042-6989(98)00228-4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mellem, M. S., Friedman, R. B., and Medvedev, A. V. (2013). Gamma-and theta band synchronization during semantic priming reflect local and long-range lexical–semantic networks. Brain Lang. 127, 440–451. doi: 10.1016/j.bandl.2013.09.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Meltzer, J. A., Negishi, M., Mayes, L. C., and Constable, R. T. (2007). Individual differences in EEG theta and alpha dynamics during working memory correlate with fMRI responses across subjects. Clin. Neurophysiol. 118, 2419–2436. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2007.07.023

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Meyer, L., Grigutsch, M., Schmuck, N., Gaston, P., and Friederici, A. D. (2015). Frontal–posterior theta oscillations reflect memory retrieval during sentence comprehension. Cortex 71, 205–218. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2015.06.027

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Molinaro, N., Barraza, P., and Carreiras, M. (2013). Long-range neural synchronization supports fast and efficient reading: EEG correlates of processing expected words in sentences. Neuroimage 72, 120–132. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.01.031

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mondino, M., Haesebaert, F., Poulet, E., Suaud-Chagny, M. F., and Brunelin, J. (2015a). Fronto-temporal transcranial Direct Current Stimulation (tDCS) reduces source-monitoring deficits and auditory hallucinations in patients with schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Res. 161, 515–516. doi: 10.1016/j.schres.2014.10.054

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mondino, M., Jardri, R., Suaud-Chagny, M. F., Saoud, M., Poulet, E., and Brunelin, J. (2015b). Effects of fronto-temporal transcranial direct current stimulation on auditory verbal hallucinations and resting-state functional connectivity of the left temporo-parietal junction in patients with schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Bull. 42, 318–326. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbv114

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Moore, A. T., and Schwitzgebel, E. (2018). The experience of reading. Conscious. Cogn. 62, 57–68.

Google Scholar

Moore, T., Armstrong, K. M., and Fallah, M. (2003). Visuomotor origins of covert spatial attention. Neuron 40, 671–683. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(03)00716-5

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Myers, R. E. (1969). Cystic brain alteration after incomplete placental abruption in monkey. Arch. Neurol. 21, 133–141. doi: 10.1001/archneur.1969.00480140033003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Neuenschwander, S., and Singer, W. (1996). Long-range synchronization of oscillatory light responses in the cat retina and lateral geniculate nucleus. Nature 379:728. doi: 10.1038/379728a0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nicolson, R. I., and Fawcett, A. J. (1994). Comparison of deficits in cognitive and motor skills among children with dyslexia. Ann. Dyslexia 44, 147–164. doi: 10.1007/bf02648159

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nirenberg, S., Carcieri, S. M., Jacobs, A. L., and Latham, P. E. (2001). Retinal ganglion cells act largely as independent encoders. Nature 411:698. doi: 10.1038/35079612

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nobre, A. C., Gitelman, D. R., Dias, E. C., and Mesulam, M. M. (2000). Covert visual spatial orienting and saccades: overlapping neural systems. Neuroimage 11, 210–216. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2000.0539

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Notbohm, A., Kurths, J., and Herrmann, C. S. (2016). Modification of brain oscillations via rhythmic light stimulation provides evidence for entrainment but not for superposition of event-related responses. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 10:10. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2016.00010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pammer, K. (2014). Temporal sampling in vision and the implications for dyslexia. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 7:933. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00933

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pammer, K., Hansen, P., Kringelbach, M., Holliday, I., Barnes, G., Hillebrand, A., et al. (2004a). Visual word recognition: the first half second. Neuroimage 22, 1819–1825. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.05.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pammer, K., Lavis, R., and Cornelissen, P. (2004b). Visual encoding mechanisms and their relationship to text presentation preference. Dyslexia 10, 77–94. doi: 10.1002/dys.264

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pammer, K., and Wheatley, C. (2001). Isolating the M (y)-cell response in dyslexia using the spatial frequency doubling illusion. Vis. Res. 41, 2139–2147. doi: 10.1016/S0042-6989(01)00092-X

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Paracchini, S., Diaz, R., and Stein, J. (2016). “Advances in dyslexia genetics—new insights into the role of brain asymmetries,” in Advances in Genetics, Vol. 96, Ed. D. Kumar (Cambridge, MA: Academic Press), 53–97. doi: 10.1016/bs.adgen.2016.08.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Peelen, M. V., and Kastner, S. (2014). Attention in the real world: toward understanding its neural basis. Trends Cogn. Sci. 18, 242–250. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2014.02.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Penolazzi, B., Angrilli, A., and Job, R. (2009). Gamma EEG activity induced by semantic violation during sentence reading. Neurosci. Lett. 465, 74–78. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2009.08.065

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Penttonen, M., and Buzsáki, G. (2003). Natural logarithmic relationship between brain oscillators. Thalamus Relat. Syst. 2, 145–152. doi: 10.1016/s1472-9288(03)00007-4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Plöchl, M., Gaston, J., Mermagen, T., König, P., and Hairston, D. (2016). Oscillatory activity in auditory cortex reflects the perceptual level of audio-tactile integration. Sci. Rep. 6:33693.

Google Scholar

Poeppel, D. (2003). The analysis of speech in different temporal integration windows: cerebral lateralization as ‘asymmetric sampling in time’. Speech Commun. 41, 245–255. doi: 10.1016/S0167-6393(02)00107-3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Power, A. J., Mead, N., Barnes, L., and Goswami, U. (2012). Neural entrainment to rhythmically presented auditory, visual, and audio-visual speech in children. Front. Psychol. 3:216. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00905

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Prado, C., Dubois, M., and Valdois, S. (2007). The eye movements of dyslexic children during reading and visual search: impact of the visual attention span. Vis. Res. 47, 2521–2530. doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2007.06.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rajkai, C., Lakatos, P., Chen, C. M., Pincze, Z., Karmos, G., and Schroeder, C. E. (2007). Transient cortical excitation at the onset of visual fixation. Cereb. Cortex 18, 200–209. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhm046

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rathbun, D. L., Warland, D. K., and Usrey, W. M. (2010). Spike timing and information transmission at retinogeniculate synapses. J. Neurosci. 13, 13558–13566. doi: 10.1523/jneurosci.0909-10.2010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rayner, K. (1998). Eye movements in reading and information processing: 20 years of research. Psychol. Bull. 124, 372–422. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.124.3.372

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Reich, D. S., Mechler, F., and Victor, J. D. (2001). Temporal coding of contrast in primary visual cortex: when, what, and why. J. Neurophysiol. 85, 1039–1050. doi: 10.1152/jn.2001.85.3.1039

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Reinagel, P., and Reid, R. C. (2000). Temporal coding of visual information in the thalamus. J. Neurosci. 20, 5392–5400. doi: 10.1523/jneurosci.20-14-05392.2000

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Richter, C. G., Thompson, W. H., Bosman, C. A., and Fries, P. (2017). Top-Down Beta Enhances Bottom-Up Gamma. J. Neurosci. 6, 1855–1866.

Google Scholar

Roberts, B. M., Hsieh, L. T., and Ranganath, C. (2013). Oscillatory activity during maintenance of spatial and temporal information in working memory. Neuropsychologia 51, 349–357. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2012.10.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Robinson, D. A., and Fuchs, A. F. (1969). Eye movements evoked by stimulation of frontal eye fields. J. Neurophysiol. 32, 637–648. doi: 10.1152/jn.1969.32.5.637

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Robinson, F. R., Houk, J. C., and Gibson, A. R. (1987). Limb specific connections of the cat magnocellular red nucleus. J. Comp. Neurol. 257, 553–577. doi: 10.1002/cne.902570406

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ronconi, L., Basso, D., Gori, S., and Facoetti, A. (2014). TMS on right frontal eye fields induces an inflexible focus of attention. Cereb. Cortex 24, 396–402. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhs319

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ronconi, L., Pincham, H. L., Cristoforetti, G., Facoetti, A., and Szûcs, D. (2016a). Shaping prestimulus neural activity with auditory rhythmic stimulation improves the temporal allocation of attention. Neuroreport 27:487. doi: 10.1097/wnr.0000000000000565

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ronconi, L., Pincham, H. L., Szûcs, D., and Facoetti, A. (2016b). Inducing attention not to blink: auditory entrainment improves conscious visual processing. Psychol. Res. 80, 774–784. doi: 10.1007/s00426-015-0691-8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rubin, G. S., and Turano, K. (1992). Reading without saccadic eye movements. Vis. Res. 32, 895–902. doi: 10.1016/0042-6989(92)90032-e

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ruff, C. C., Blankenburg, F., Bjoertomt, O., Bestmann, S., Freeman, E., Haynes, J. D., et al. (2006). Concurrent TMS-fMRI and psychophysics reveal frontal influences on human retinotopic visual cortex. Curr. Biol. 16, 1479–1488. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2006.06.057

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Saalmann, Y. B., Pigarev, I. N., and Vidyasagar, T. R. (2007). Neural mechanisms of visual attention: how top-down bottom-up highlights relevant locations. Science 316, 1612–1615. doi: 10.1126/science.1139140

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Samengo, I., Elijah, D., and Montemurro, M. (2013). “Spike-train analysis,” in Principles of Neural Coding, eds R. Q. Quiroga and S. Panzeri (Boca Raton, FL: CRC Press), 75–96. doi: 10.1201/b14756-7

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schroeder, C. E., and Lakatos, P. (2009). The gamma oscillation: master or slave? Brain Topogr. 22, 24–26. doi: 10.1007/s10548-009-0080-y

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sereno, A. B. (1992). “Programming saccades: the role of attention,” in Eye Movements and Visual Cognition, Ed. K. Rayner (New York, NY: Springer), 89–107. doi: 10.1007/978-1-4612-2852-3_6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Siegel, M., Donner, T. H., Oostenveld, R., Fries, P., and Engel, A. K. (2008). Neuronal synchronization along the dorsal visual pathway reflects the focus of spatial attention. Neuron 60, 709–719. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2008.09.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sireteanu, R., Goebel, C., Goertz, R., Werner, I., Nalewajko, M., and Thiel, A. (2008). Impaired serial visual search in children with developmental dyslexia. Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci. 1145, 199–211. doi: 10.1196/annals.1416.021

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Slaghuis, W. L., and Ryan, J. F. (1999). Spatio-temporal contrast sensitivity, coherent motion, and visible persistence in developmental dyslexia. Vis. Res. 39, 651–668. doi: 10.1016/j.visres.2006.05.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Smythies, J., Edelstein, L., and Ramachandran, V. (2014). “Hypothesis relating to the function of the claustrum,” in The Claustrum, eds J. Smythies et al. (San Diego, CA: Elsevier), 299–352. doi: 10.1016/b978-0-12-404566-8.00013-1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Snowling, M. J. (1981). Phonemic deficits in developmental dyslexia. Psychol. Res. 43, 219–234. doi: 10.1007/bf00309831

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Snowling, M. J. (2001). From language to reading and dyslexia 1. Dyslexia 7, 37–46. doi: 10.1002/dys.185

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Snowling, M. J. (2019). Dyslexia: A Very Short Introduction. Oxford: Oxford University Press.

Google Scholar

Spaak, E., Lange, F. P. D., and Jensen, O. (2014). Local entrainment of alpha oscillations by visual stimuli causes cyclic modulation of perception. J. Neurosci. 34, 3536–3544. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4385-13.2014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Spiro, R., and Myers, A. (2002). “Individual differences and underlying cognitive processes in reading,” in Handbook of Reading Research, eds R. Barr, D. Pearson, M. Kamil, and P. Mosenthal (London: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates).

Google Scholar

Spironelli, C., Penolazzi, B., and Angrilli, A. (2008). Dysfunctional hemispheric asymmetry of theta and beta EEG activity during linguistic tasks in developmental dyslexia. Biol. Psychol. 77, 123–131. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2007.09.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Spironelli, C., Penolazzi, B., Vio, C., and Angrilli, A. (2006). Inverted EEG theta lateralization in dyslexic children during phonological processing. Neuropsychologia 44, 2814–2821. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2006.06.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Squire, R. F., Noudoost, B., Schafer, R. J., and Moore, T. (2013). Prefrontal contributions to visual selective attention. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 36, 451–466. doi: 10.1146/annurev-neuro-062111-150439

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stein, J. (2001). The magnocellular theory of developmental dyslexia. Dyslexia 7, 12–36. doi: 10.1002/dys.186

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stein, J. (2019). The current status of the magnocellular theory of developmental dyslexia. Neuropsychologia 130, 66–77. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2018.03.022

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stein, J., and Fowler, S. (1981). Visual dyslexia. Trends Neurosci. 4, 77–80.

Google Scholar

Stein, J., and Talcott, J. (1999). Impaired neuronal timing in developmental dyslexia—the magnocellular hypothesis. Dyslexia 5, 59–77. doi: 10.1002/(sici)1099-0909(199906)5:2<59::aid-dys134>3.0.co;2-f

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stein, J. F. (1991). Vision and Visual Dyslexia, Vol. 13. Boca Raton, FL: CRC Press.

Google Scholar

Stein, J. F., and Walsh, V. (1997). To see but not to read: the magnocellular theory of dyslexia. Trends Neurosci. 20, 147–152. doi: 10.1016/s0166-2236(96)01005-3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stevens, K. N. (2002). Toward a model for lexical access based on acoustic landmarks and distinctive features. J. Acoust. Soc. Am. 111, 1872–1891. doi: 10.1121/1.1458026

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stokić, M., Milosavljeviæ, Z., and Subotiæ, M. (2011). Specific features of brain connectivity during silent reading in children with developmental dyslexia. Spec. Edukac. Rehabil. 10, 479–492.

Google Scholar

Tallon-Baudry, C., and Bertrand, O. (1999). Oscillatory gamma activity in humans and its role in object representation. Trends Cogn. Sci. 3, 151–162. doi: 10.1016/s1364-6613(99)01299-1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tallon-Baudry, C., Bertrand, O., Delpuech, C., and Pernier, J. (1996). Stimulus specificity of phase-locked and non-phase-locked 40 Hz visual responses in human. J. Neurosci. 16, 4240–4249. doi: 10.1523/jneurosci.16-13-04240.1996

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tallon-Baudry, C., Bertrand, O., Delpuech, C., and Pernier, J. (1997). Oscillatory γ-band (30–70 Hz) activity induced by a visual search task in humans. J. Neurosci. 17, 722–734. doi: 10.1523/jneurosci.17-02-00722.1997

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Thiede, A., Virtala, P., Ala-Kurikka, I., Partanen, E., Huotilainen, M., Mikkola, K., et al. (2019). An extensive pattern of atypical neural speech-sound discrimination in newborns at risk of dyslexia. Clin. Neurophysiol. 130, 634–646. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2019.01.019

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Thut, G., and Miniussi, C. (2009). New insights into rhythmic brain activity from TMS– EEG studies. Trends Cogn. Sci. 13, 182–189. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2009.01.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Thut, G., Miniussi, C., and Gross, J. (2012). The functional importance of rhythmic activity in the brain. Curr. Biol. 22, R658–R663.

Google Scholar

Tian, J. R., and Lynch, J. C. (1996). Corticocortical input to the smooth and saccadic eye movement subregions of the frontal eye field in Cebus monkeys. J. Neurophysiol. 76, 2754–2771. doi: 10.1152/jn.1996.76.4.2754

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Treisman, A. (1988). Features and objects: the fourteenth Bartlett memorial lecture. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. Sect. A 40, 201–237. doi: 10.1098/rstb.1998.0284

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Valle, A. C., Dionisio, K., Pitskel, N. B., Pascual-Leone, A., Orsati, F., Ferreira, M. J., et al. (2007). Low and high frequency repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation for the treatment of spasticity. Dev. Med. Child Neurol. 49, 534–538. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8749.2007.00534.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Van Der Werf, J., Jensen, O., Fries, P., and Medendorp, W. P. (2008). Gamma-band activity in human posterior parietal cortex encodes the motor goal during delayed prosaccades and antisaccades. J. Neurosci. 28, 8397–8405. doi: 10.1523/jneurosci.0630-08.2008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Van Kan, P. L., and McCurdy, M. L. (2001). Role of primate magnocellular red nucleus neurons in controlling hand preshaping during reaching to grasp. J. Neurophysiol. 85, 1461–1478. doi: 10.1152/jn.2001.85.4.1461

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vernet, M., Quentin, R., Chanes, L., Mitsumasu, A., and Valero-Cabré, A. (2014). Frontal eye field, where art thou? Anatomy, function, and non-invasive manipulation of frontal regions involved in eye movements and associated cognitive operations. Front. Integr. Neurosci. 8:66. doi: 10.3389/fnint.2014.00066

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vidyasagar, T. R. (1998). Gating of neuronal responses in macaque primary visual cortex by an attentional spotlight. Neuroreport 9, 1947–1952. doi: 10.1097/00001756-199806220-00006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vidyasagar, T. R. (1999). A neuronal model of attentional spotlight: parietal guiding the temporal. Brain Res. Rev. 30, 66–76. doi: 10.1016/s0165-0173(99)00005-3

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vidyasagar, T. R. (2004). Neural underpinnings of dyslexia as a disorder of visuo-spatial attention. Clin. Exp. Optom. 87, 4–10. doi: 10.1111/j.1444-0938.2004.tb03138.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vidyasagar, T. R. (2012). “Aetiology of dyslexia: a visual perspective on a phonological marker,” in Visual Aspects of Dyslexia, eds Z. Kapoula and J. Stein (Oxford: Oxford University Press), 151–170. doi: 10.1093/acprof:oso/9780199589814.003.0010

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vidyasagar, T. R. (2013). Reading into neuronal oscillations in the visual system: implications for developmental dyslexia. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 7:811. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00811

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vidyasagar, T. R. (2019). Visual attention and neural oscillations in reading and dyslexia: are they possible targets for remediation? Neuropsychologia 130, 59–65. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2019.02.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vidyasagar, T. R., and Levichkina, E. (2019). An integrated neuronal model of claustral function in timing the synchrony between cortical areas. Front. Neural Circ. 13:3. doi: 10.3389/fncir.2019.00003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vidyasagar, T. R., and Pammer, K. (1999). Impaired visual search in dyslexia relates to the role of the magnocellular pathway in attention. Neuroreport 10, 1283–1287. doi: 10.1097/00001756-199904260-00024

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vidyasagar, T. R., and Pammer, K. (2010). Dyslexia: a deficit in visuo-spatial attention, not in phonological processing. Trends Cogn. Sci. 14, 57–63. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2009.12.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Volgushev, M., Chistiakova, M., and Singer, W. (1998). Modification of discharge patterns of neocortical neurons by induced oscillations of the membrane potential. Neuroscience 83, 15–25. doi: 10.1016/s0306-4522(97)00380-1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

von der Malsburg, C. (1981). The Correlation Theory of Brain Function (Internal Report 81-2). Goettingen: Department of Neurobiology, Max Planck Institute for Biophysical Chemistry.

Google Scholar

von der Malsburg, C., and Schneider, W. (1986). A neural cocktail-party processor. Biol. Cybern. 54, 29–40. doi: 10.1007/bf00337113

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

von Stein, A., and Sarnthein, J. (2000). Different frequencies for different scales of cortical integration: from local gamma to long range alpha/theta synchronization. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 38, 301–313. doi: 10.1016/s0167-8760(00)00172-0

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Vöröslakos, M., Takeuchi, Y., Brinyiczki, K., Zombori, T., Oliva, A., Fernández-Ruiz, A., et al. (2018). Direct effects of transcranial electric stimulation on brain circuits in rats and humans. Nat. Commun. 9:483. doi: 10.1038/s41467-018-02928-2923

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wagner, R. K., and Torgesen, J. K. (1987). The nature of phonological processing and its causal role in the acquisition of reading skills. Psychol. Bull. 101:192. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.101.2.192

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wang, J., Gao, D., Li, D., Desroches, A. S., Liu, L., and Li, X. (2014). Theta–gamma coupling reflects the interaction of bottom-up and top-down processes in speech perception in children. Neuroimage 102, 637–645. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.08.030

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wang, J. J., Bi, H. Y., Gao, L. Q., and Wydell, T. N. (2010). The visual magnocellular pathway in Chinese-speaking children with developmental dyslexia. Neuropsychologia 48, 3627–3633. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2010.08.015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wang, L., Hagoort, P., and Jensen, O. (2018). Gamma oscillatory activity related to language prediction. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 30, 1075–1085. doi: 10.1162/jocn_a_01275

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wang, L., Saalmann, Y. B., Pinsk, M. A., et al. (2012a). Electrophysiological low-frequency coherence and cross-frequency coupling contribute to BOLD connectivity. Neuron 76, 1010–1020. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2012.09.033

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wang, L., Zhu, Z., and Bastiaansen, M. (2012b). Integration or predictability? A further specification of the functional role of gamma oscillations in language comprehension. Front. Psychol. 3:187. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00187

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wang, X. J. (2010). Neurophysiological and computational principles of cortical rhythms in cognition. Physiol. Rev. 90, 1195–1268. doi: 10.1152/physrev.00035.2008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Weaver, L., Rostain, A. L., Mace, W., Akhtar, U., Moss, E., and O’Reardon, J. P. (2012). Transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) in the treatment of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder in adolescents and young adults: a pilot study. J. ECT 28, 98–103. doi: 10.1097/yct.0b013e31824532c8

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Weiss, S., Mueller, H. M., Schack, B., King, J. W., Kutas, M., and Rappelsberger, P. (2005). Increased neuronal communication accompanying sentence comprehension. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 57, 129–141. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2005.03.013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Whalley, K., and Hansen, J. (2006). The role of prosodic sensitivity in children’s reading development. J. Res. Read. 29, 288–303. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-9817.2006.00309.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wu, C., Stefanescu, R. A., Martel, D. T., and Shore, S. E. (2015). Listening to another sense: somatosensory integration in the auditory system. Cell Tissue Res. 361, 233–250. doi: 10.1007/s00441-014-2074-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Yücel, Y. H., Zhang, Q., Weinreb, R. N., Kaufman, P. L., and Gupta, N. (2003). Effects of retinal ganglion cell loss on magno-, parvo-, koniocellular pathways in the lateral geniculate nucleus and visual cortex in glaucoma. Progr. Retinal Eye Res. 22, 465–481. doi: 10.1016/s1350-9462(03)00026-0

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Žarić, G., Correia, J. M., González, G. F., Tijms, J., van der Molen, M. W., Blomert, L., et al. (2017). Altered patterns of directed connectivity within the reading network of dyslexic children and their relation to reading dysfluency. Dev. Cogn. Neurosci. 23, 1–13. doi: 10.1016/j.dcn.2016.11.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ziegler, J. C., and Goswami, U. (2005). Reading acquisition, developmental dyslexia, and skilled reading across languages: a psycholinguistic grain size theory. Psychol. Bull. 131:3. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.131.1.3

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: dyslexia, reading, temporal sampling, magnocellular, dorsal, theta, gamma, oscillations

Citation: Archer K, Pammer K and Vidyasagar TR (2020) A Temporal Sampling Basis for Visual Processing in Developmental Dyslexia. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 14:213. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2020.00213

Received: 12 December 2019; Accepted: 11 May 2020;
Published: 08 July 2020.

Edited by:

Teri Lawton, Perception Dynamics Institute (PDI), United States

Reviewed by:

Andrea Facoetti, University of Padua, Italy
John Frederick Stein, University of Oxford, United Kingdom

Copyright © 2020 Archer, Pammer and Vidyasagar. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Kim Archer, Kimberly.Archer@uon.edu.au; Kristen Pammer, Kristen.Pammer@newcastle.edu.au

Download