Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Neurosci., 09 May 2022
Sec. Autonomic Neuroscience
This article is part of the Research Topic Interoception and the Autonomic Nervous System: Investigating Affect, Decision-Making, and Mental Health View all 8 articles

The Effects of a 3-Week Heartbeat Perception Training on Interoceptive Abilities

  • 1Department of Clinical and Health Psychology, Faculty of Engineering, Computer Science and Psychology, Institute of Psychology and Education, University of Ulm, Ulm, Germany
  • 2Department of Work and Organizational Psychology, Faculty of Engineering, Compurter Science and Psychology, Institute of Psychology and Education, University of Ulm, Ulm, Germany

Recent studies showed promising short-term effects of heartbeat perception training on interoceptive abilities. Research on the effects of heartbeat perception training on interoceptive abilities over time is sparse. Therefore, the aim of this study was to examine the short-term effects and the effects of a 3-week heartbeat perception training over time on interoceptive abilities, namely, cardiac interoceptive accuracy (IAc) and interoceptive sensibility (IS). A total of 40 healthy participants were randomized to the intervention group (n = 20) or the control group (n = 20). The intervention group conducted three cardiac biofeedback sessions (one per week) at the laboratory, whereas the control group watched a documentary instead. Interoceptive abilities were assessed via the heartbeat perception task (IAc) and confidence ratings (IS) at baseline, after each laboratory session, and 1 week after the last session (post-measurement). IAc was significantly increased in the intervention group compared to the control group after the first training session (short-term effect). There were no significant improvements in IS due to the first session, and neither on IAc nor IS over time. Descriptive trends of improved interoceptive abilities over time were found in both groups. Single session of heartbeat perception training seems to be a promising approach to improve IAc. Future research should further investigate the long-term effects of diverse heartbeat perception training varying in frequency and intensity of the training sessions in diverse samples aiming to improve interoceptive abilities.

Introduction

Interoception describes the process by which the nervous system senses, interprets, and integrates signals from the internal body aiming at a moment-by-moment internal bodily landscape across conscious and unconscious levels (Khalsa et al., 2018). Corresponding to the three-dimensional model of interoception developed by Garfinkel et al. (2015), interoceptive accuracy, i.e., cardiac interoceptive accuracy (IAc), is defined as the objective performance in detecting interoceptive (cardiac) signals. It can be assessed via performance tasks, such as the heartbeat counting task (Schandry, 1981) or the heartbeat discrimination task (Whitehead et al., 1977). Interoceptive sensibility (IS) comprises the subjective beliefs about the own ability to focus on internal bodily sensations, quantified by self-report measures, such as the Body Perception Questionnaire (Porges, 1993, 2015), the Interoceptive Accuracy Scale (Murphy et al., 2020), or confidence ratings concerning IAc (Garfinkel et al., 2015).

In previous research, interoception emerged as a health-related variable. Exemplarily, high interoceptive abilities were shown to be related to more intense perception and better regulation of emotions (e.g., Barrett et al., 2004; Herbert and Pollatos, 2008; Dunn et al., 2010; Füstös et al., 2013; Zamariola et al., 2019a) and higher empathy (Grynberg and Pollatos, 2015). However, Zamariola et al. (2019b) found that interoceptive abilities did not modulate negative affect. In contrast, impaired interoceptive abilities were found in various mental disorders, such as anorexia nervosa (Pollatos et al., 2008; Fischer et al., 2016), depression (Pollatos et al., 2009; Eggart et al., 2019; Schultchen et al., 2021), somatoform disorders (Weiss et al., 2014), obsessive-compulsive disorders (Schultchen et al., 2019b), or schizophrenia (Koreki et al., 2020). However, it needs to be noted that evidence concerning the association between interoceptive abilities and somatic symptoms and related disorders (e.g., Mussgay et al., 1999; Scholz et al., 2001) as well as regarding anxiety or panic disorders (e.g., Domschke et al., 2010; Stevens et al., 2011; Krautwurst et al., 2016) is mixed. These inconsistent findings might result from methodological differences between the studies, such as various instructions. Moreover, associations between interoceptive abilities and stress were found (e.g., Schulz and Vögele, 2015; Maeda et al., 2019; Schultchen et al., 2019a; Schulz et al., 2020; Opdensteinen et al., 2021). Furthermore, decreased interoceptive abilities have been assumed to play a role in the perception of somatic symptoms (van den Bergh et al., 2017, 2019). Consequently, investigating different approaches to improve interoceptive abilities is of high relevance.

A growing body of studies showed increased interoceptive abilities due to diverse interventions of various lengths, such as self-focused procedures (Weisz et al., 1988; Tsakiris et al., 2011; Ainley et al., 2012; Ainley and Tsakiris, 2013; Pollatos et al., 2016), mindfulness-based interventions (e.g., Bornemann et al., 2014; Parkin et al., 2014; Bornemann and Singer, 2017; Fischer et al., 2017), floating (Feinstein et al., 2018), power posing (Weineck et al., 2019), or cognitive behavioral therapy in depressive patients (Karanassios et al., 2021). Concerning the effects of mindfulness-based interventions on interoceptive abilities, previous findings are incongruent. For example, Fischer et al. (2017) found improvements in IAc but no changes in IS due to an 8-week body scan intervention. In contrast, short-term mindfulness-based interventions, such as a single yoga session (Schillings et al., 2021) or two meditation sessions (Fairclough and Goodwin, 2007), had no significant effect on IAc. Consequently, further research to investigate effective interventions aiming to improve interoceptive abilities that also include repeated intervention sessions might be necessary.

First studies (Schandry and Weitkunat, 1990; Schaefer et al., 2014; Meyerholz et al., 2019) showed increased interoceptive abilities due to heartbeat perception training. In an early study by Schandry and Weitkunat (1990) investigating a cardiac awareness training, participants were instructed to press a button after every perceived heartbeat over several training blocks. For correct responses, they received acoustic feedback. In two training groups (i.e., consistent-tone and fading-tone groups), IAc, as measured via the heartbeat discrimination task (Whitehead et al., 1977), increased. Meyerholz et al. (2019) reported higher IAc in healthy participants who conducted a contingent heartbeat perception training compared to a non-contingent feedback condition, a mindfulness practice, and a waiting condition. This heartbeat perception training paradigm developed by Schaefer et al. (2014) consisted of an interoceptive learning task concerning cardiac perception, including phases with and without feedback on individual performance. Similarly, Schenk et al. (2020) showed an increase in IAc in healthy participants after a single session of the heartbeat perception training paradigm in a stress condition (cold pressor stress test). Contrarily, in the study conducted by Schaefer et al. (2014), the same training showed no effect as compared to a waiting control group for a sample of patients with somatoform disorders, i.e., a population with low interoceptive abilities (Weiss et al., 2014). In contrast to the other aforementioned studies, Rominger et al. (2021) used the heartbeat discrimination task (Whitehead et al., 1977) to assess IAc, which might account for the heterogeneous results. To conclude, there is first but also mixed evidence that heartbeat perception training improves individual cardiac perception (Schandry and Weitkunat, 1990; Meyerholz et al., 2019; Schenk et al., 2020).

Referring to more common cardiac trainings, e.g., biofeedback which is based on heart rate (e.g., Peira et al., 2013, 2014) or heart rate variability (e.g., Kotozaki et al., 2014; Goessl et al., 2017), positive changes in health-related variables were found. In particular, previous research has showed improvements in clinical symptoms in several physical and mental disorders, such as cardiovascular diseases, chronic obstructive pulmonary disease, fibromyalgia, major depressive disorder, and post-traumatic stress disorders (for a review, refer to Wheat and Larkin, 2010), cardiac control in emotional reactions, i.e., emotion regulation (Peira et al., 2013, 2014), and increases and decreases in baroreceptor functions. A meta-analysis proposed by Goessl et al. (2017) on the basis of healthy and clinical samples’ reports reduced stress levels due to biofeedback based on heart rate variability. It needs to be noted that these studies differ in their lengths of the implemented biofeedback training interventions and in their number of sessions. In contrast, e.g., Peira et al. (2013, 2014) examined the short-term effects of a single biofeedback session, Del Paso and González (2004) applied a 3-week biofeedback training targeting the baroreceptor sensitivity with one session per week, and Kotozaki et al. (2014), as one of the studies included in the meta-analysis by Goessl et al. (2017), showed decreased stress levels due to a 4-week biofeedback intervention with daily sessions.

Consequently, differentiating between short- and long-term effects needs to be considered, also depending on the exact outcome. Moreover, there is a need to investigate the long-term effects of heartbeat perception trainings, as previous studies (Schandry and Weitkunat, 1990; Meyerholz et al., 2019; Schenk et al., 2020) have examined only short-term effects due to a single heartbeat perception training session.

Extending previous studies using a specifically designed heartbeat perception training, this study investigates both the short- and long-term effects of a 3-week heartbeat perception training on two dimensions of interoception (i.e., IAc and IS). The intervention group conducted three heartbeat perception training sessions in the laboratory (one per week) and was compared to the control group watching a neutral film. We assumed that (a) IAc and (b) IS increase due to the first heartbeat perception training (short-term effect) as well as over time (from pre- to post-measurement after 4 weeks, long-term effect) as compared to the control group.

Materials and Methods

Participants

Based on the location of Ulm University, Germany, participants were recruited via flyers, e-mais, and social media. The inclusion criteria were the following: (1) age of 18 years or above, (2) sufficient knowledge of the German language, (3) Internet access, (4) no cardiovascular diseases, (5) no heart medication or psychotropic drugs, (6) no psychotherapy during the last 12 months, and (7) no current attendance of any kind of heartbeat perception training or mindfulness-based intervention. One participant from the intervention group was excluded from the data analysis because he did not meet the inclusion criteria (refer to the abovementioned criteria). Thus, the data analysis was conducted based on a sample of N = 39.

Procedure and Material

The study was conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki and approved by the local ethics committee. Initially, participants received and signed the informed consent. They were randomly assigned to the intervention (n = 20; 25% male participants) or to the control group (n = 20; 30% male participants). All participants underwent four assessments (at baseline, at each training or film session, and 1 week after the last session) in a laboratory over 4 weeks (one session per week) and filled out the online questionnaires (e.g., demographic data; Perceived Stress Scale, Cohen et al., 1983) 1 day before each laboratory session. In each of the first three sessions, the intervention group conducted a 20-min cardiac heartbeat perception training, as suggested by Meyerholz et al. (2019), whereas the control group watched a 20-min documentary film instead. At the beginning of each session, after each session, and 1 week after the last session, participants of both groups performed the heartbeat counting task (Schandry, 1981) to measure IAc and rated their confidence concerning their counted heartbeats in each interval (IS). After the participants had successfully completed all parts of the study, they received 6-course credits or 50 euros.

Cardiac Interoceptive Accuracy

Cardiac interoceptive accuracy was assessed using the heartbeat counting task (Schandry, 1981). The task was to focus on their own heartbeats and to count them silently during four randomized intervals (lengths: 25, 35, 45, and 60 s), without getting any information about the lengths of the intervals or any feedback about their performance quality. The participants were instructed to sit in a relaxed position, to avoid movements, and not to use manipulating strategies, such as to stop breathing and to take their pulse. Importantly, they should count solely those heartbeats they were sure of sensing. For every interval, an automated start and stop signal was presented after which the participants had to report their counted heartbeats and the confidence ratings. First, a 15-s training interval was conducted to get familiar with the task. The task instructions were presented via the software Presentation (Neurobehavioral Systems, version 22.1; 30 April 2021). Biopac MP150 with a sampling rate of 1,000 Hz was used for recording the heartbeats. The averaged heartbeat perception scores were calculated using the following equation:

IAcScore=14(1-(|recordedheartbeats-countedheartbeats|)recordedheartbeats

The score ranges from 0 to 1; higher scores indicate a higher IAc for cardiovascular signals.

Interoceptive Sensibility

Interoceptive sensibility (i.e., self-reported beliefs concerning IAc) was measured via confidence ratings, meaning that the participants had to rate how confident they felt regarding their counted heartbeats after each interval of the heartbeat counting task (Schandry, 1981). The rating scale ranged from 0 (=no confidence) to 10 (=complete confidence). The confidence items were presented via the software Presentation (Neurobehavioral Systems, version 22.1; 30 April 2021) and recorded.

Intervention: Heartbeat Perception Training vs. Control Group

The heartbeat perception training paradigm was based on the paradigm by Schaefer et al. (2014). Participants conducted three training sessions (one per week) in a small, soundproof room within the laboratory which lasted for around 20 min. Prior to the actual heartbeat perception training, the participants underwent a phase of 15 s where only the heartbeat symbol according to the individual heartbeat was presented. Following, each training consisted of three training blocks of 48 trials, and each block was composed of two phases (24 trials per phase). In each first phase, visual biofeedback was presented 200 ms after an R-wave detection in the form of a red heart symbol. In the second phase, no such visual biofeedback was presented. In each trial of the phase, the participants were instructed to press a button when they perceived the instructed second, third, or fourth heartbeat as displayed. For this purpose, the numbers two, three, or four were pseudorandomly presented. The reaction was classified as correct if the button was pressed within 450 ms after the detection of the last heartbeat. The feedback concerning their performance, presented in all phases, consisted of a green checkmark for correct responses and a red “X” for wrong responses. At the end of each phase, the percentage of correct responses was displayed. The procedure of each heartbeat perception training session is depicted in Figure 1. Between the two phases of each block, a pause of 15 s was implemented. Twelve practice trials were followed to acquaint the participant with the procedure.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Procedure of the heartbeat perception training paradigm.

The control group watched a 20-min documentary film about architectural stylistic epochs in the laboratory instead of the heartbeat perception training. This control condition was selected as a quite neutral stimulus with an external focus only.

Data Analysis

We reported descriptive statistics, IAc and IS mean scores, and t-tests for independent samples regarding demographic data and relevant variables at baseline (e.g., age, body mass index, IAcpre, and ISpre). To analyze the short-term effects of the first training session on IAc and IS, we conducted repeated-measures ANOVAs with the factors of time and group. All these analyses were calculated using the Statistical Package for Social Science (SPSS; version 27).

To investigate the effects of all three trainings over time, we used regression models. Due to the nested longitudinal data structure, we employed hierarchical linear models. The measurement points (level 1) were nested within participants (level 2). The hierarchical linear models and model comparisons were estimated in R using the packages lme4 (Bates et al., 2014), lmerTest (Kuznetsova et al., 2017), and r2mlm (Rights and Sterba, 2019). In our regression analyses, we included the baseline measure (T1), the pre-measures of the second and the third training session (T2pre and T3pre), and a measure that took place 1 week after the third training session (follow-up measure, T4). Thus, slopes indicate the predicted change in IAc and IS, respectively, over the course of 1 week after a training. The dichotomous predictor variable group was dummy-coded and the predictor time had an interpretable zero point (the pre-measurement before the first training). Therefore, centering the predictors group and time was not necessary. In one model, we used IAc as a predictor variable (refer to Table 3), which was grand-mean centered in this case. Due to a sample size of N = 39, the restricted maximum likelihood (REML) estimator was applied. To judge the model fit, the Akaike information criterion (AIC), the Bayesian information criterion (BIC), and, where appropriate, likelihood ratio tests were calculated to evaluate relative model fit. For the likelihood ratio tests, the Maximum Likelihood (ML) estimator was applied. In addition, we calculated Rt2(f) describing the proportion of total outcome variance explained by predictors via fixed slopes (Rights and Sterba, 2019, Table 5, Formula 1) and Rt2(fvm)describing the proportion of total outcome variance explained by predictors via fixed and random slope variation/covariation and by person-specific outcome means (Rights and Sterba, 2019, Table 5, Formula 5). The significance level for all analyses was p ≤ 0.05.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Descriptive statistics at baseline (T1) per group.

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Model 1: Random intercept and slope model for cardiac interoceptive accuracy with the predictors time, group, and the interaction of time and group.

TABLE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Table 3. Model 5: Random intercept and random slope model for interoceptive sensibility with the predictors time, group, cardiac interoceptive accuracy, and the interaction of time and group.

Results

The relevant descriptive statistics are summarized in Table 1. There were no significant differences between the groups at baseline (T1).

Short-Term Effect on Cardiac Interoceptive Accuracy

First, we tested for the short-term effect after the first training. Results of the repeated-measures ANOVA revealed a significant time × group interaction effect [F(1, 37) = 14.268, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.278], indicating an increase in IAc due to the first heartbeat perception training at T1 in the intervention group (intervention group: mean IAct1pre = 0.676, SE = 0.222; mean IAct1post = 0.777, SD = 0.224; control group: IAct1pre = 0.627, SD = 0.262; IAct1post = 0.577; SD = 0.285). The main effects of time [F(1, 37) = 1.587, p = 0.216, ηp2 = 0.041] and group [F(1, 37) = 2.584, p = 0.116, ηp2 = 0.065] were not significant. Mean values and standard errors are depicted in Figure 2.

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Mean cardiac interoceptive accuracy and standard errors in the intervention and the control group before (T1pre) and after (T1post) the first heartbeat perception training session (***p < 0.001).

Short-Term Effect on Interoceptive Sensibility

Regarding IS, descriptive data indicated an increase in IS due to the first heartbeat perception training at T1 in the intervention group (mean ISt1pre = 4.711, SD = 1.755; mean ISt1post = 4.829, SD = 1.581) as compared to a descriptive decrease in the control group (ISt1pre = 4.338, SD = 1.677; ISt1post = 4.025; SE = 2.071). Nevertheless, the results of the repeated-measures ANOVA revealed neither a significant effect of time [F(1, 37) = 0.211; p = 0.648; ηp2 = 0.006], nor of group [F(1, 37) = 1.229; p = 0.275; ηp2 = 0.032] or the time × group interaction [F(1, 37) = 1.041; p = 0.314; ηp2 = 0.027].

Effect on Cardiac Interoceptive Accuracy Over Time

Turning our attention from the effects of the first (single) training to the effects of all three trainings over time, the descriptive data revealed the trends of improved IAc in both groups beginning from T1 (refer to means in Figure 3). Moreover, besides the significant increase in IAc from T1pre to T1post, on the descriptive level, results showed slight increases in IAc from T1pre to T2pre and from T2pre to T3pre in the intervention group, whereas the control group exhibited slight increases in IAc over time from T2pre to T3pre and from T3pre to T4.

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 3. Mean cardiac interoceptive accuracy and standard errors in the intervention and the control group at baseline (T1pre), before and after each of the three heartbeat perception training sessions (pre vs. post), and post-measurement (T4).

To test our hypothesis on the effects of repeated trainings, we predicted IAc by time, group, and the interaction of both (Model 1). In the models, four measurement points were included, namely, the baseline measure (T1), the pre-measures of the second and the third training session (T2pre and T3pre), and a measure that took place 1 week after the third training session (follow-up measure, T4). Since repeated trainings are often accompanied by saturation effects that cannot be adequately described by linear equations, we estimated two models, one of which additionally included a quadratic effect for time (Model 2). Model 1 showed lower, i.e., better information criteria (AIC = −120.50, BIC = −96.098) than Model 2 (AIC = −120.94, BIC = −93.493). The likelihood ratio test did not show a significant difference of the deviances [devianceM1 = −136.50; devianceM2 = −138.94, χ2(1) = 2.445, p = 0.118]. The proportion of variance explained by the fixed slopes, Rt2(f), was 0.08 for Model 1 and similarly for Model 2. The variance explained by fixed slopes, random slope variation/covariation, and person-specific IAc means, Rt2(fvm), was 0.80 for both models. Thus, Model 1 was chosen for our regression analysis regarding IAc.

Table 2 shows the results of this random intercept and random slope model (Model 1). According to this model, the significant fixed effect of the intercept indicates an estimated mean IAc of β00 = 0.623 (SE = 0.049; p < 0.001) in the control group (i.e., the reference group) before the first training (T1pre). The variance of intercepts σ2u0j = 0.041 (SD = 0.202) describes the heterogeneity in participants’ IAc scores at T1pre. The fixed effect of the level-1-predictor time (β01 = 0.028; SE = 0.015; p = 0.061) was not significant, indicating that the factor of time did not predict IAc. Random effects for this coefficient indicate large differences between participants in growth over time (σ2u01j = 0.002, SD = 0.046). The fixed effect of the level-2-predictor group was not significant (β02 = 0.078; SE = 0.070; p = 0.272), indicating no significant differences in IAc between the groups. Furthermore, the cross-level interaction of the variables time and group (β03 = 0.016; SE = 0.021; p = 0.443) was not significant. Thus, the increase in IAc was not stronger in the intervention group than in the control group.

Effect on Interoceptive Sensibility Over Time

Figure 4 shows the means for IS (i.e., the confidence ratings) over time. There are slight descriptive trends of increased IS in both groups, with a slight decrease in the control group to T4. Again, we first selected an appropriate model for the data. We compared a model that used time, group, and the interaction of time and group (Model 3), a model that used the same predictors plus a quadratic term for time to model potential non-linear effects (Model 4), and a model without quadratic effects but with IAc as an additional predictor (Model 5). Model 5 showed lower (i.e., better) information criteria (AIC = 521.59, BIC = 558.18) than Model 3 (AIC = 555.40, BIC = 579.80) and Model 4 (AIC = 554.68, BIC = 582.13). The proportion of variance explained by the fixed slopes, Rt2(f), was 0.04 for Model 0.04 for Model 4, and 0.22 for Model 5. The variance explained by fixed slopes, random slope variation/covariation, and person-specific IS means, Rt2(fvm), was 0.67 for Model 0.068 for Model 4, and 0.80 for Model 5. Accordingly, the results of the likelihood ratio test comparing Model 3 and Model 5 showed a significant better fit of Model 5 [devianceM3 = 539.40; devianceM5 = 497.59; χ2(4) = 41.818, p < 0.001].

FIGURE 4
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 4. Mean confidence levels (interoceptive sensibility) and standard errors in the intervention and the control group at baseline (T1pre), before and after each of the three heartbeat perception training sessions (pre vs. post), and post-measurement (T4).

Table 3 shows the results for this model with a random intercept and random slopes for the predictors of time and group, the interaction of time and group, and IAc (Model 5). The intercept of β00 = 4.931 (SE = 0.296; p < 0.001) is the predicted IS for a control group participant with an average IAc at the first measurement point (T1pre). The variance of random intercepts (σ2u0j = 0.895; SD = 0.946) represents heterogeneity of IS levels at the beginning of the study (T1pre). The non-significant fixed effect of the level-1-predictor time (β01 = −0.28; SE = 0.118; p = 0.816) shows that IS did not significantly increase over the course of time. The fixed effect of the level-2-predictor group was also not significant (β02 = −0.377; SE = 0.411; p = 0.366), indicating no significant differences in IS between the groups. The significant fixed effect of the level-1-predictor IAc (β03 = 3.662; SE = 0.955; p < 0.001) indicates that higher levels of IAc are associated with higher levels of IS. Furthermore, the cross-level-interaction of the variables time and group (β03 = 0.177; SE = 0.165; p = 0.292) was not significant. Thus, there was no superior change in IS in the intervention group compared with the control group.

Discussion

The aim of this study was to examine the effects of a 3-week heartbeat perception training on interoceptive abilities in a randomized controlled trial. After the first training session, IAc was significantly increased in the intervention group, indicating a short-term effect on IAc. There was no significant short-term effect on IS due to the first training session. Regarding the long-term effects, there was neither a significant effect on IAc nor on IS.

Short-Term Effect on Cardiac Interoceptive Accuracy

As we hypothesized, IAc increased significantly due to the first session of the heartbeat perception training in the intervention group with higher effects as compared to the control group. This short-term effect is in line with the results by Meyerholz et al. (2019) and Schenk et al. (2020). In contrast, in the study by Schaefer et al. (2014) and in the recent replication study by Rominger et al. (2021), no short-term effect on IAc could be found. Schaefer et al. (2014), Meyerholz et al. (2019), and Schenk et al. (2020) as well as the present study used the same cardiac heartbeat perception training paradigm by Schaefer et al. (2014) and the heartbeat counting task (Schandry, 1981) to quantify IAc. Whereas Schaefer et al. (2014) tested a clinical sample and 17 conducted a second heartbeat counting task after the training session 14 days later, in the studies 18 by Meyerholz et al. (2019) and Schenk et al. (2020) as well as in our study, healthy samples were 19 investigated and IAc was assessed immediately before and after the training (short-term effect). In this context, it should be emphasized that Rominger et al. (2021) used the heartbeat discrimination task (Whitehead et al., 1977) to assess IAc directly before and after the heartbeat perception training paradigm by Meyerholz et al. (2019), comparing the effects with a breathing intervention and a film condition. As the tasks differ in their focus of attention, namely, both an external and an internal focus on the heartbeat discrimination task vs. an exclusive focus on internal signals in the heartbeat counting task (Schulz et al., 2013; Couto et al., 2015; Ring and Brener, 2018), it is possible that these inconsistent findings result from methodological differences. These differences exist in the measurement instrument, the investigated sample, and the time of measurement.

In line with the explanatory approaches by Meyerholz et al. (2019) and Schenk et al. (2020), our results could be explained by an immediate interoceptive learning effect due to the heartbeat perception training. Moreover, we assumed that the training evoked a high internal focus of attention, especially in the training phases without the performance-related feedback. This focus might have supported the individual heartbeat perception in contrast to an external focus on the control condition generated by the neutral film. Complementing this approach, Petzschner et al. (2019) showed that the heartbeat-evoked potential, an index of the cortical processing of cardiac interoceptive signals (e.g., Pollatos and Schandry, 2004; Mai et al., 2018), is modulated by attention, as the heartbeat-evoked potential was higher during interoceptive compared to exteroceptive attention.

Explaining the interoceptive learning effect within the framework of predictive coding (e.g., Seth et al., 2012; Friston et al., 2014; Allen and Tsakiris, 2019; Petzschner et al., 2021), perceptual learning, based on cardiac stimuli, individual IAc and IS could have resulted in the minimization of prediction errors (i.e., discrepancies between current and previous individual perceptions). Integrating the approaches proposed by Canales-Johnson et al. (2015) and Hodossy and Tsakiris (2020), reduced prediction errors could have induced updated predictive models of interoceptive signals including changes in priors (i.e., individual models of perception), presumably on the perception of cardiac signals, representing a learning effect. Canales-Johnson et al. (2015) showed that cardiac interoceptive learning based on auditory feedback is associated with changes in heartbeat-evoked potential amplitudes. Furthermore, they found evidence for a network hub in the insular cortex for cardiac interoceptive learning due to a heartbeat-tapping task and auditory feedback. In addition, Schenk et al. (2020) also suggested that the anterior insula and the anterior cingulate cortex, which were identified as central for IAc (e.g., Pollatos et al., 2005a,b), might play a pivotal role in interoceptive learning and might account for the neurophysiological base.

Short-Term Effect on Interoceptive Sensibility

Contrarily to our hypothesis, IS did not increase after the first heartbeat perception training session. It needs to be noted that our study is the first that investigated IS in the context of a heartbeat perception training. Similarly, Weineck et al. (2020b) found increased IAc due to a single session of power posing in healthy women, whereas there was also no change in IS as assessed via confidence ratings and via the Body Perception Questionnaire (Porges, 1993, 2015). The authors argue that interventions targeting individuals’ self-focus might not be directly associated with increases in performance confidence. Transferring the approach by Weineck et al. (2020b), the heartbeat perception training might have evoked a high internal focus of attention (i.e., self-focus) and might account for the missing effect on IS due to a single session of heartbeat perception training. Moreover, the performance feedback in the heartbeat perception training could have had an impact on the perceived confidence regarding IAc, for instance, by unsettling the participants in their confidence due to the feedback if their response was incorrect. Finally, this result is in accordance with other studies (Garfinkel et al., 2015; Forkmann et al., 2016; Meessen et al., 2016; Murphy et al., 2019; Weineck et al., 2019), showing that IAc and IS are distinct dimensions of interoception that might differentially benefit from various interventions.

Effect on Cardiac Interoceptive Accuracy Over Time

Contrary to our hypothesis, there was no significant increase in IAc over the course of the three trainings. Descriptive trends of improved IAc were found in both groups. Similarly, Schaefer et al. (2014) reported increased IAc in the waiting control group after 2 weeks and explained this result by a time effect or a learning effect from the heartbeat counting task itself, which might also account for the present findings.

More importantly, Weineck et al. (2020b) found only a short-term effect on IAc due to a single session of power posing as an intervention aiming to improve interoceptive abilities, but no effect after 1 week. Central reasons for the missing effects could be that the duration or the intensity (number of sessions) of the intervention was not sufficient to improve IAc. In particular, previous research on body-oriented interventions aiming to improve interoceptive abilities mostly showed effects after longer interventions of at least 8 weeks (Bornemann et al., 2014; Bornemann and Singer, 2017; Fischer et al., 2017).

A further contributing factor of interest could be that the whole heartbeat perception training was too unspecific to influence IAc over time, as the focus is on different body sensations as discussed in the context of mindfulness-based interventions (Khalsa et al., 2008; Meyerholz et al., 2019).

Effect on Interoceptive Sensibility Over Time

In contrast to our hypothesis, IS did not increase significantly over the course of the intervention. Contrarily to the present findings, Fischer et al. (2017) showed significantly higher confidence due to an 8-week mindfulness-based intervention in both the intervention and the active control group. Moreover, Parkin et al. (2014) found increased confidence due to an 8-week mindfulness-based intervention. As compared to the previous findings regarding IS in studies with mindfulness-based or self-focus-related interventions (Parkin et al., 2014; Fischer et al., 2017; Weineck et al., 2019) and similar to the results concerning IAc over time, the missing effects could be explained by an insufficiently low duration or intensity of the intervention. Furthermore, as discussed regarding the short-term effect on IS, feedback on the performance in the heartbeat perception training could have had an impact on the perceived confidence regarding IAc. Future studies should control for this aspect by explicitly instructing the participant that the confidence levels concerning IAc have to be rated independently from the performance in the biofeedback paradigm.

Importantly, although there was no significant change in IS, the present results indicate that IAc is associated with IS. This is in line with the findings by Garfinkel et al. (2015) who report an association between IAc assessed via the heartbeat counting task and IS quantified via confidence ratings. However, the present result needs to be interpreted carefully, as IS was not directly manipulated in our experiment and did not change significantly over time. Thus, the variance of IS explained by IAc is likely between-person variance, that is, even though IS was constant or did not systematically covary with time or group membership, participants with higher levels of IAc also showed higher levels of IAc independently from our experimental manipulations. Nonetheless, IAc and IS need to be considered distinct facets as shown in previous studies (e.g., Garfinkel et al., 2015; Forkmann et al., 2016; Weineck et al., 2019).

Strengths, Limitations, and Future Research

A strength of this study is that both short-term effects and the effects over the time of a 3-week heartbeat perception training on health-related interoceptive abilities were investigated. Furthermore, by calculating hierarchical linear models, intraindividual and interindividual differences in the outcome variables were considered. Moreover, in contrast to previous studies, two dimensions of interoception, namely, IAc and IS, were assessed. In addition, we replicated the short-term effect on IAc of the study by Meyerholz et al. (2019), which highlights the effectiveness of single heartbeat perception training sessions aiming to improve IAc. In addition, examining the effects of several heartbeat perception training sessions on interoceptive abilities can be underlined as an innovative approach.

Concerning the debate about the heartbeat counting task including criticism (e.g., Ring et al., 2015; Murphy et al., 2018; Ring and Brener, 2018; Zamariola et al., 2018) and contrary approaches regarding the criticism (Ainley et al., 2020; Zimprich et al., 2020), it can be mentioned that the strict instruction (i.e., to count exclusively those heartbeats of which the participants actually perceived) reduces the risk that the knowledge about the individual heartbeat influences IAc (Schulz and Vögele, 2015). It might be important to shed light on the type of instructions applied in the heartbeat counting task. In this study, the strict instruction was used. For example, in similar intervention studies aiming to improve IAc in healthy samples, mean IAc scores, assessed pre-intervention, are comparable with the IAc mean scores found in this study. These mean scores were reported between 0.56 and 0.70 (Fischer et al., 2017; Meyerholz et al., 2019; Weineck et al., 2019; Schenk et al., 2020). Furthermore, it needs to be noted that in previous research using the heartbeat counting task (e.g., Kever et al., 2015; Fischer et al., 2017; Meyerholz et al., 2019), commonly, information about the exact instruction (strict vs. standard) were not reported. In contrast and comparably to our study, Schenk et al. (2020) described similar IAc mean scores as compared to our reported scores by using the strict instruction of the heartbeat counting task. Desmedt et al. (2018) reported significantly lower mean IAc scores due to the strict instruction of the heartbeat counting task. Nevertheless, the study design in terms of conducting the (adapted) heartbeat counting task differed as compared to other studies (e.g., Fischer et al., 2017; Meyerholz et al., 2019; Weineck et al., 2019; Schenk et al., 2020). Due to diverse instructions and a lack of information on the detailed instructions in previous research, comparing the mean scores in IAc might be difficult. Consequently, the type of instruction might be a potential influencing factor on IAc.

Pointing out the limitations, first, the study investigated a non-clinical sample that was not characterized by low interoceptive abilities. Therefore, future research should examine diverse samples, especially those with low interoceptive abilities, such as clinical populations or samples with high-stress levels aiming to improve interoceptive abilities as related to health benefits, such as better regulation of emotions (e.g., Barrett et al., 2004; Herbert and Pollatos, 2008; Dunn et al., 2010; Füstös et al., 2013; Zamariola et al., 2019a), or a better perception of bodily symptoms (van den Bergh et al., 2019). Moreover, future studies should design interventions over a longer period, implement a higher frequency of sessions, or include psychoeducative elements. Integrating training elements into the participants’ everyday life might be important to demonstrate the practical health relevance of improving interoceptive abilities. Therefore, further training elements could be smartphone-based biofeedback (Dillon et al., 2016) or portable biofeedback devices (Lemaire et al., 2011). A possible approach for future training to improve interoceptive abilities might be a combination of training sessions in the laboratory and via portable devices or smartphones to better integrate the training into everyday life. In addition, further extensions could be innovative smartphone-based heartbeat counting tasks (Plans et al., 2021) or breathing-related perception training or tasks, such as the filter detection task (Harrison et al., 2021), to focus on another interoceptive dimension. According to the 2 × 2 factorial model of interoceptive abilities by Murphy et al. (2019), distinguishing between accuracy and attention to interoceptive signals (i.e., factor 1) and between objective measures and self-reported beliefs concerning interoceptive signals (i.e., factor 2) is essential. In line with the model by Murphy et al. (2019), ecological momentary assessments might be used to assess interoceptive states in everyday life (Bornemann and Singer, 2017; Kok and Singer, 2017) and to sensitize for a more frequent attention to interoceptive signals, i.e., potentially, not only focusing on cardiac but also respiratory or gastrointestinal signals. Furthermore, in addition to confidence ratings, appropriate questionnaires, such as the Interoceptive Accuracy Scale (Murphy et al., 2019) or the Interoceptive Confusion Questionnaire (Murphy et al., 2019), to quantify IS. Finally, the study should be replicated based on a larger sample to increase the power.

To sum up, a single session of a cardiac heartbeat perception training seems to be a promising approach to improve IAc. A heartbeat perception training over 3 weeks might be insufficient to improve interoceptive abilities from pre- to post-intervention. Future research should further investigate the effects of various heartbeat perception trainings over at least 3 weeks or longer, varying in frequency and intensity of the training sessions and, potentially, complementary mobile elements in diverse samples aiming to improve interoceptive abilities. Furthermore, heartbeat perception training could be a promising approach for clinical samples.

Data Availability Statement

The original contributions presented in the study are included in the article/Supplementary Material, further inquiries can be directed to the corresponding author.

Ethics Statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by the Ethics Commitee of Ulm University. The patients/participants provided their written informed consent to participate in this study.

Author Contributions

CS, GK, and OP designed the study. CS and GK conducted the study. CS and NS analyzed the data. CS wrote the first draft of the manuscript. GK, DS, and OP edited the manuscript. All authors discussed the results and approved the final version of the manuscript for submission.

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s Note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Acknowledgments

We would like to thank all students who were involved in assessing and post-processing the data.

Supplementary Material

The Supplementary Material for this article can be found online at: https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fnins.2022.838055/full#supplementary-material

References

Ainley, V., Tajadura-Jiménez, A., Fotopoulou, A., and Tsakiris, M. (2012). Looking into myself: changes in interoceptive sensitivity during mirror self-observation. Psychophysiology 49, 1504–1508. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2012.01468.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ainley, V., and Tsakiris, M. (2013). Body conscious? Interoceptive awareness, measured by heartbeat perception, is negatively correlated with self-objectification. PLoS One 8:e55568. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0055568

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ainley, V., Tsakiris, M., Pollatos, O., Schulz, A., and Herbert, B. M. (2020). Comment on “Zamariola etal. (2018), interoceptive accuracy scores are problematic: evidence from simple bivariate correlations”-The empirical data base, the conceptual reasoning and the analysis behind this statement are misconceived and do not support the authors’ conclusions. Biol. Psychol. 152:107870. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2020.107870

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Allen, M., and Tsakiris, M. (2019). “The body as first prior: interoceptive predictive processing and the primacy of selfmodels,” in The Interoceptive Mind: From Homeostasis to Awareness, eds M. Tsakiris and H. de Preester (Oxford: Oxford University Press), 27–45.

Google Scholar

Barrett, L. F., Quigley, K. S., Bliss-Moreau, E., and Aronson, K. R. (2004). Interoceptive sensitivity and self-reports of emotional experience. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 87, 684–697. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.87.5.684

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bates, D., Mächler, M., Bolker, B., and Walker, S. (2014). Fitting linear mixed-effects models using lme4. J. Stat. Soft. 67, 1–48.

Google Scholar

Bornemann, B., Herbert, B. M., Mehling, W. E., and Singer, T. (2014). Differential changes in self-reported aspects of interoceptive awareness through 3 months of contemplative training. Front. Psychol. 5:1504. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01504

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bornemann, B., and Singer, T. (2017). Taking time to feel our body: steady increases in heartbeat perception accuracy and decreases in alexithymia over 9 months of contemplative mental training. Psychophysiology 54, 469–482. doi: 10.1111/psyp.12790

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Canales-Johnson, A., Silva, C., Huepe, D., Rivera-Rei, Á, Noreika, V., Garcia, M. D. C., et al. (2015). Auditory feedback differentially modulates behavioral and neural markers of objective and subjective performance when tapping to your heartbeat. Cereb. Cortex 25, 4490–4503. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhv076

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cohen, S., Kamarck, T., and Mermelstein, R. (1983). A global measure of perceived stress. J. Health Soc. Behav. 24, 385–396. doi: 10.2307/2136404

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Couto, B., Adolfi, F., Sedeño, L., Salles, A., Canales-Johnson, A., Alvarez-Abut, P., et al. (2015). Disentangling interoception: insights from focal strokes affecting the perception of external and internal milieus. Front. Psychol. 6:503. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00503

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Del Paso, G. A. R., and González, M. I. (2004). Modification of baroreceptor cardiac reflex function by biofeedback. Appl. Psychophysiol. Biofeedback 29, 197–211. doi: 10.1023/b:apbi.0000039058.68746.ad

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Desmedt, O., Luminet, O., and Corneille, O. (2018). The heartbeat counting task largely involves non-interoceptive processes: evidence from both the original and an adapted counting task. Biol. Psychol. 138, 185–188. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2018.09.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dillon, A., Kelly, M., Robertson, I. H., and Robertson, D. A. (2016). Smartphone applications utilizing biofeedback can aid stress reduction. Front. Psychol. 7:832. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00832

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Domschke, K., Stevens, S., Pfleiderer, B., and Gerlach, A. L. (2010). Interoceptive sensitivity in anxiety and anxiety disorders: an overview and integration of neurobiological findings. Clin. Psychol. Rev. 30, 1–11. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2009.08.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dunn, B. D., Stefanovitch, I., Evans, D., Oliver, C., Hawkins, A., and Dalgleish, T. (2010). Can you feel the beat? Interoceptive awareness is an interactive function of anxiety- and depression-specific symptom dimensions. Behav. Res. Ther. 48, 1133–1138. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2010.07.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Eggart, M., Lange, A., Binser, M. J., Queri, S., and Müller-Oerlinghausen, B. (2019). Major depressive disorder is associated with impaired interoceptive accuracy: a systematic review. Brain Sci. 9:131. doi: 10.3390/brainsci9060131

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fairclough, S. H., and Goodwin, L. (2007). The effect of psychological stress and relaxation on interoceptive accuracy: implications for symptom perception. J. Psychosom. Res. 62, 289–295. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychores.2006.10.017

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Feinstein, J. S., Khalsa, S. S., Yeh, H., Al Zoubi, O., Arevian, A. C., Wohlrab, C., et al. (2018). The elicitation of relaxation and interoceptive awareness using floatation therapy in individuals with high anxiety sensitivity. Biol. Psychiatry 3, 555–562. doi: 10.1016/j.bpsc.2018.02.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fischer, D., Berberich, G., Zaudig, M., Krauseneck, T., Weiss, S., and Pollatos, O. (2016). Interoceptive processes in anorexia nervosa in the time course of cognitive-behavioral therapy: a pilot study. Front. Psychiatry 7:199. doi: 10.3389/fpsyt.2016.00199

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fischer, D., Messner, M., and Pollatos, O. (2017). Improvement of interoceptive processes after an 8-week body scan intervention. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 11:452. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2017.00452

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Forkmann, T., Scherer, A., Meessen, J., Michal, M., Schächinger, H., Vögele, C., et al. (2016). Making sense of what you sense: disentangling interoceptive awareness, sensibility and accuracy. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 109, 71–80. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2016.09.019

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Friston, K. J., Stephan, K. E., Montague, R., and Dolan, R. J. (2014). Computational psychiatry: the brain as a phantastic organ. Lancet Psychiatry 1, 148–158. doi: 10.1016/S2215-0366(14)70275-5

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Füstös, J., Gramann, K., Herbert, B. M., and Pollatos, O. (2013). On the embodiment of emotion regulation: interoceptive awareness facilitates reappraisal. Soc. Cognit. Affect. Neurosci. 8, 911–917. doi: 10.1093/scan/nss089

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Garfinkel, S. N., Seth, A. K., Barrett, A. B., Suzuki, K., and Critchley, H. D. (2015). Knowing your own heart: distinguishing interoceptive accuracy from interoceptive awareness. Biol. Psychol. 104, 65–74. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2014.11.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Goessl, V. C., Curtiss, J. E., and Hofmann, S. G. (2017). The effect of heart rate variability biofeedback training on stress and anxiety: a meta-analysis. Psychol. Med. 47, 2578–2586. doi: 10.1017/S0033291717001003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Grynberg, D., and Pollatos, O. (2015). Perceiving one’s body shapes empathy. Physiol. Behav. 140, 54–60. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2014.12.026

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Harrison, O. K., Garfinkel, S. N., Marlow, L., Finnegan, S. L., Marino, S., Köchli, L., et al. (2021). The filter detection task for measurement of breathing-related interoception and metacognition. Biol. Psychol. 165:108185. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2021.108185

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Herbert, B. M., and Pollatos, O. (2008). Interozeptive Sensitivität, Gefühle und Verhaltensregulation [Interoceptive sensitivity, feelings and behavorial control]. Z. Neuropsychol. 19, 125–137. doi: 10.1024/1016-264X.19.3.125

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hodossy, L., and Tsakiris, M. (2020). Wearing your heart on your screen: investigating congruency-effects in autonomic responses and their role in interoceptive processing during biofeedback. Cognition 194:104053. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2019.104053

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Karanassios, G., Schultchen, D., Möhrle, M., Berberich, G., and Pollatos, O. (2021). The effects of a standardized cognitive-behavioural therapy and an additional mindfulness-based training on interoceptive abilities in a depressed cohort. Brain Sci. 11:1355. doi: 10.3390/brainsci11101355

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kever, A., Pollatos, O., Vermeulen, N., and Grynberg, D. (2015). Interoceptive sensitivity facilitates both antecedent- and response-focused emotion regulation strategies. Pers. Individ. Differ. 87, 20–23. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2015.07.014

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Khalsa, S. S., Adolphs, R., Cameron, O. G., Critchley, H. D., Davenport, P. W., Feinstein, J. S., et al. (2018). Interoception and mental health: a roadmap. Biol. Psychiatry 3, 501–513. doi: 10.1016/j.bpsc.2017.12.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Khalsa, S. S., Rudrauf, D., Damasio, A. R., Davidson, R. J., Lutz, A., and Tranel, D. (2008). Interoceptive awareness in experienced meditators. Psychophysiology 45, 671–677. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.2008.00666.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kok, B. E., and Singer, T. (2017). Phenomenological fingerprints of four meditations: differential state changes in affect, mind-wandering, meta-cognition, and interoception before and after daily practice across 9 months of training. Mindfulness 8, 218–231. doi: 10.1007/s12671-016-0594-9

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Koreki, A., Funayama, M., Terasawa, Y., Onaya, M., and Mimura, M. (2020). Aberrant interoceptive accuracy in patients with schizophrenia performing a heartbeat counting task. Schizophr. Bull. Open 2:sgaa067. doi: 10.1093/schizbullopen/sgaa067

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kotozaki, Y., Takeuchi, H., Sekiguchi, A., Yamamoto, Y., Shinada, T., Araki, T., et al. (2014). Biofeedback-based training for stress management in daily hassles: an intervention study. Brain Behav. 4, 566–579. doi: 10.1002/brb3.241

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Krautwurst, S., Gerlach, A. L., and Witthöft, M. (2016). Interoception in pathological health anxiety. J. Abnorm. Psychol. 125, 1179–1184. doi: 10.1037/abn0000210

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kuznetsova, A., Brockhoff, P. B., and Christensen, R. H. B. (2017). LmerTest package: tests in linear mixed effects models. J. Stat. Soft. 82, 1–26. doi: 10.18637/jss.v082.i13

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lemaire, J. B., Wallace, J. E., Lewin, A. M., Grood, J., and de Schaefer, J. P. (2011). The effect of a biofeedback-based stress management tool on physician stress: a randomized controlled clinical trial. Open Med. 5, e154–e165.

Google Scholar

Maeda, S., Ogishima, H., and Shimada, H. (2019). Acute cortisol response to a psychosocial stressor is associated with heartbeat perception. Physiol. Behav. 207, 132–138. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2019.05.013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mai, S., Wong, C. K., Georgiou, E., and Pollatos, O. (2018). Interoception is associated with heartbeat-evoked brain potentials (HEPs) in adolescents. Biol. Psychol. 137, 24–33. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2018.06.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Meessen, J., Mainz, V., Gauggel, S., Volz-Sidiropoulou, E., Sütterlin, S., and Forkmann, T. (2016). The relationship between interoception and metacognition. J. Psychophysiol. 30, 76–86. doi: 10.1027/0269-8803/a000157

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Meyerholz, L., Irzinger, J., Witthöft, M., Gerlach, A. L., and Pohl, A. (2019). Contingent biofeedback outperforms other methods to enhance the accuracy of cardiac interoception: a comparison of short interventions. J. Behav. Ther. Exp. Psychiatry 63, 12–20. doi: 10.1016/j.jbtep.2018.12.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Murphy, J., Brewer, R., Plans, D., Khalsa, S. S., Catmur, C., and Bird, G. (2020). Testing the independence of self-reported interoceptive accuracy and attention. Q. J. Exp. Psychol. 73, 115–133. doi: 10.1177/1747021819879826

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Murphy, J., Catmur, C., and Bird, G. (2019). Classifying individual differences in interoception: implications for the measurement of interoceptive awareness. Psychonom. Bull. Rev. 26, 1467–1471. doi: 10.3758/s13423-019-01632-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Murphy, J., Millgate, E., Geary, H., Ichijo, E., Coll, M.-P., Brewer, R., et al. (2018). Knowledge of resting heart rate mediates the relationship between intelligence and the heartbeat counting task. Biol. Psychol. 133, 1–3. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2018.01.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mussgay, L., Klinkenberg, N., and Rüddel, H. (1999). Heart beat perception in patients with depressive, somatoform, and personality disorders. J. Psychophysiol. 13, 27–36. doi: 10.1027//0269-8803.13.1.27

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Opdensteinen, K. D., Schaan, L., Pohl, A., Schulz, A., Domes, G., and Hechler, T. (2021). Interoception in preschoolers: new insights into its assessment and relations to emotion regulation and stress. Biol. Psychol. 165:108166. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2021.108166

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Parkin, L., Morgan, R., Rosselli, A., Howard, M., Sheppard, A., Evans, D., et al. (2014). Exploring the relationship between mindfulness and cardiac perception. Mindfulness 5, 298–313. doi: 10.1007/s12671-012-0181-7

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Peira, N., Fredrikson, M., and Pourtois, G. (2014). Controlling the emotional heart: heart rate biofeedback improves cardiac control during emotional reactions. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 91, 225–231. doi: 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2013.12.008

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Peira, N., Pourtois, G., and Fredrikson, M. (2013). Learned cardiac control with heart rate biofeedback transfers to emotional reactions. PLoS One 8:e70004. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0070004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Petzschner, F. H., Garfinkel, S. N., Paulus, M. P., Koch, C., and Khalsa, S. S. (2021). Computational models of interoception and body regulation. Trends Neurosci. 44, 63–76. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2020.09.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Petzschner, F. H., Weber, L. A., Wellstein, K. V., Paolini, G., Do, C. T., and Stephan, K. E. (2019). Focus of attention modulates the heartbeat evoked potential. Neuroimage 186, 595–606. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2018.11.037

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Plans, D., Ponzo, S., Morelli, D., Cairo, M., Ring, C., Keating, C. T., et al. (2021). Measuring interoception: the phase adjustment task. Biol. Psychol. 165:108171. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2021.108171

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pollatos, O., Herbert, B. M., Berberich, G., Zaudig, M., Krauseneck, T., and Tsakiris, M. (2016). Atypical self-focus effect on interoceptive accuracy in anorexia nervosa. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 10:484. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2016.00484

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pollatos, O., Kirsch, W., and Schandry, R. (2005a). Brain structures involved in interoceptive awareness and cardioafferent signal processing: a dipole source localization study. Hum. Brain Mapp. 26, 54–64. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20121

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pollatos, O., Kirsch, W., and Schandry, R. (2005b). On the relationship between interoceptive awareness, emotional experience, and brain processes. Cognit. Brain Res. 25, 948–962. doi: 10.1016/j.cogbrainres.2005.09.019

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pollatos, O., Kurz, A.-L., Albrecht, J., Schreder, T., Kleemann, A. M., Schöpf, V., et al. (2008). Reduced perception of bodily signals in anorexia nervosa. Eat. Behav. 9, 381–388. doi: 10.1016/j.eatbeh.2008.02.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pollatos, O., and Schandry, R. (2004). Accuracy of heartbeat perception is reflected in the amplitude of the heartbeat-evoked brain potential. Psychophysiology 41, 476–482. doi: 10.111/1469-8986.2004.00170.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pollatos, O., Traut-Mattausch, E., and Schandry, R. (2009). Differential effects of anxiety and depression on interoceptive accuracy. Depress. Anxiety 26, 167–173. doi: 10.1002/da.20504

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Porges, S.W. (1993, 2015). Body Perception Questionnaire (BPQ) Manual. Available online at: https://static1.squarespace.com/static/5c1d025fb27e390a78569537/t/5ccd9de46e9a7f37d527bbc9/1556979173189/BPQ_Information_and_Scoring_v2_091518.pdf [accessed on November 02, 2019].

Google Scholar

Rights, J. D., and Sterba, S. K. (2019). Quantifying explained variance in multilevel models: an integrative framework for defining R-squared measures. Psychol. Methods 24, 309–338. doi: 10.1037/met0000184

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ring, C., and Brener, J. (2018). Heartbeat counting is unrelated to heartbeat detection: a comparison of methods to quantify interoception. Psychophysiology 55:e13084. doi: 10.1111/psyp.13084

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ring, C., Brener, J., Knapp, K., and Mailloux, J. (2015). Effects of heartbeat feedback on beliefs about heart rate and heartbeat counting: a cautionary tale about interoceptive awareness. Biol. Psychol. 104, 193–198. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2014.12.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rominger, C., Graßmann, T. M., Weber, B., and Schwerdtfeger, A. R. (2021). Does contingent biofeedback improve cardiac interoception? A preregistered replication of Meyerholz, Irzinger, Withöft, Gerlach, and Pohl (2019) using the heartbeat discrimination task in a randomised control trial. PLoS One 16:e0248246. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0248246

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schaefer, M., Egloff, B., Gerlach, A. L., and Witthöft, M. (2014). Improving heartbeat perception in patients with medically unexplained symptoms reduces symptom distress. Biol. Psychol. 101, 69–76. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2014.05.012

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schandry, R. (1981). Heart beat perception and emotional experience. Psychophysiology 18, 483–488. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.1981.tb02486.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schandry, R., and Weitkunat, R. (1990). Enhancement of heartbeat-related brain potentials through cardiac awareness training. Int. J. Neurosci. 53, 243–253. doi: 10.3109/00207459008986611

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schenk, L., Fischbach, J. T. M., Müller, R., Vögele, C., Witthöft, M., van Diest, I., et al. (2020). High blood pressure responders show largest increase in heartbeat perception accuracy after post-learning stress following a cardiac interoceptive learning task. Biol. Psychol. 154:107919. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2020.107919

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schillings, C., Schultchen, D., and Pollatos, O. (2021). Effects of a single yoga session on cardiac interoceptive accuracy and emotional experience. Brain Sci. 11:1572. doi: 10.3390/brainsci11121572

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Scholz, O., Ott, R., and Sarnoch, H. (2001). Proprioception in somatoform disorders. Behav. Res. Ther. 39, 1429–1438. doi: 10.1016/S0005-7967(00)00108-X

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schultchen, D., Bayer, J., Kühnel, J., Melchers, K. G., and Pollatos, O. (2019a). Interoceptive accuracy is related to long-term stress via self-regulation. Psychophysiology 56:e13429. doi: 10.1111/psyp.13429

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schultchen, D., Zaudig, M., Krauseneck, T., Berberich, G., and Pollatos, O. (2019b). Interoceptive deficits in patients with obsessive-compulsive disorder in the time course of cognitive-behavioral therapy. PLoS One 14:e0217237. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0217237

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schultchen, D., Schneider, C., Berberich, G., Zaudig, M., Erle, T. M., and Pollatos, O. (2021). On the relationship of interoceptive accuracy and attention: a controlled study with depressed inpatients and a healthy cohort. Front. Psychol. 11:597488. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.597488

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schulz, A., Lass-Hennemann, J., Sütterlin, S., Schächinger, H., and Vögele, C. (2013). Cold pressor stress induces opposite effects on cardioceptive accuracy dependent on assessment paradigm. Biol. Psychol. 93, 167–174. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2013.01.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schulz, A., Schultchen, D., and Vögele, C. (2020). Interoception, stress, and physical symptoms in stress-associated diseases. Eur. J. Health Psychol. 27, 132–153. doi: 10.1027/2512-8442/a000063

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schulz, A., and Vögele, C. (2015). Interoception and stress. Front. Psychol. 6:993. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00993

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Seth, A. K., Suzuki, K., and Critchley, H. D. (2012). An interoceptive predictive coding model of conscious presence. Front. Psychol. 2:395. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00395

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stevens, S., Gerlach, A. L., Cludius, B., Silkens, A., Craske, M. G., and Hermann, C. (2011). Heartbeat perception in social anxiety before and during speech anticipation. Behav. Res. Ther. 49, 138–143. doi: 10.1016/j.brat.2010.11.009

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tsakiris, M., Tajadura-Jiménez, A., and Costantini, M. (2011). Just a heartbeat away from one’s body: interoceptive sensitivity predicts malleability of body-representations. Proc. Biol. Sci. 278, 2470–2476. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2010.2547

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

van den Bergh, O., Witthöft, M., Petersen, S., and Brown, R. J. (2017). Symptoms and the body: taking the inferential leap. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 74, 185–203. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2017.01.015

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

van den Bergh, O., Zacharioudakis, N., and Petersen, S. (2019). “Interoception, categorization and symptom perception,” in The Interoceptive Mind: From Homeostasis to Awareness, eds M. Tsakiris and H. de Preester (Oxford: Oxford University Press), 212–226.

Google Scholar

Weineck, F., Messner, M., Hauke, G., and Pollatos, O. (2019). Improving interoceptive ability through the practice of power posing: a pilot study. PLoS One 14:e0211453. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0211453

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Weineck, F., Schultchen, D., Hauke, G., Messner, M., and Pollatos, O. (2020b). Using bodily postures to reduce anxiety and improve interoception: a comparison between powerful and neutral poses. PLoS One 15:e0242578. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0242578

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Weineck, F., Messner, M., Hauke, G., and Pollatos, O. (2019). Improving interoceptive ability through the practice of power posing: a pilot study. PLoS One 14:e0211453. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0211453

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Weiss, S., Sack, M., Henningsen, P., and Pollatos, O. (2014). On the interaction of self-regulation, interoception and pain perception. Psychopathology 47, 377–382. doi: 10.1159/000365107

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Weisz, J., Bálazs, L., and Ádám, G. (1988). The influence of self-focused attention on heartbeat perception. Psychophysiology 25, 193–199. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8986.1988.tb00987.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wheat, A. L., and Larkin, K. T. (2010). Biofeedback of heart rate variability and related physiology: a critical review. Appl. Psychophysiol. Biofeedback 35, 229–242. doi: 10.1007/s10484-010-9133-y

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Whitehead, W. E., Drescher, V. M., Heiman, P., and Blackwell, B. (1977). Relation of heart rate control to heartbeat perception. Biofeedback Self Regul. 2, 371–392. doi: 10.1007/BF00998623

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zamariola, G., Frost, N., van Oost, A., Corneille, O., and Luminet, O. (2019a). Relationship between interoception and emotion regulation: new evidence from mixed methods. J. Affect. Disord. 246, 480–485. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2018.12.101

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zamariola, G., Luminet, O., Mierop, A., and Corneille, O. (2019b). Does it help to feel your body? Evidence is inconclusive that interoceptive accuracy and sensibility help cope with negative experiences. Cogn. Emot. 33, 1627–1638. doi: 10.1080/02699931.2019.1591345

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zamariola, G., Maurage, P., Luminet, O., and Corneille, O. (2018). Interoceptive accuracy scores from the heartbeat counting task are problematic: evidence from simple bivariate correlations. Biol. Psychol. 137, 12–17. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2018.06.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zimprich, D., Nusser, L., and Pollatos, O. (2020). Are interoceptive accuracy scores from the heartbeat counting task problematic? A comment on Zamariola etal. (2018). Biol. Psychol. 152:107868. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2020.107868

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: heartbeat perception training, interoception, cardiac interoceptive accuracy, interoceptive sensibility, interoceptive training

Citation: Schillings C, Karanassios G, Schulte N, Schultchen D and Pollatos O (2022) The Effects of a 3-Week Heartbeat Perception Training on Interoceptive Abilities. Front. Neurosci. 16:838055. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2022.838055

Received: 17 December 2021; Accepted: 07 April 2022;
Published: 09 May 2022.

Edited by:

Daisuke Ueno, Kyoto Prefectural University of Medicine, Japan

Reviewed by:

Anna Pohl, University of Cologne, Germany
Andy Schumann, University Hospital Jena, Germany

Copyright © 2022 Schillings, Karanassios, Schulte, Schultchen and Pollatos. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Christine Schillings, christine.schillings@uni-ulm.de

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.