Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychol., 01 September 2015
Sec. Psychology for Clinical Settings
This article is part of the Research Topic Parenthood from Biology to Relation. Prevention, Assessment and Interventions for Developmental and Clinical Issues View all 32 articles

Mother-preterm infant interactions at 3 months of corrected age: influence of maternal depression, anxiety and neonatal birth weight

\r\nErica Neri*Erica Neri1*Francesca AgostiniFrancesca Agostini1Paola SalvatoriPaola Salvatori1Augusto BiasiniAugusto Biasini2Fiorella MontiFiorella Monti1
  • 1Department of Psychology, University of Bologna, Bologna, Italy
  • 2Paediatric and Neonatal Intensive Care Unit, Bufalini Hospital, Cesena, Italy

Maternal depression and anxiety represent risk factors for the quality of early mother-preterm infant interactions, especially in the case of preterm birth. Despite the presence of many studies on this topic, the comorbidity of depressive and anxious symptoms has not been sufficiently investigated, as well as their relationship with the severity of prematurity and the quality of early interactions. The Aim of this study was to evaluate the quality of early mother-infant interactions and the prevalence of maternal depression and anxiety comparing dyads of extremely low birth weight (ELBW) and very low birth weight (VLBW) preterm infants with full-term ones. Seventy seven preterm infants (32 ELBW; 45 VLBW) and 120 full term (FT) infants and their mothers were recruited. At 3 months of corrected age, 5 min of mother-infant interactions were recorded and later coded through the Global Ratings Scales. Mothers completed the Edinburgh Postnatal Depression Scale and Penn State Worry Questionnaire. Infant levels of development were assessed through the Griffiths Mental Development Scales. A relation emerged among the severity of prematurity, depression, anxiety, and the quality of interactions. When compared with the FT group, the ELBW interactions were characterized by high maternal intrusiveness and low remoteness, while the VLBW dyads showed high levels of maternal sensitivity and infant communication. Depression was related to maternal remoteness and negative affective state, anxiety to low sensitivity, while infant interactive behaviors were impaired only in case of comorbidity. ELBW’s mothers showed the highest prevalence of depressive and anxious symptoms; moreover, only in FT dyads, low maternal sensitivity, negative affective state and minor infant communication were associated to the presence of anxious symptoms. The results confirmed the impact of prematurity on mother–infant interactions and on maternal affective state. Early diagnosis can help to plan supportive interventions.

Introduction

Maternal Depression and Anxiety in the Postnatal Period

Postnatal depression has been widely recognized as a significant risk factor for woman’s health, baby’s development and the quality of mother–infant interactions (O’Hara and Swain, 1996; Martins and Gaffan, 2000; Guedeney and Jeammet, 2001; Righetti-Veltema et al., 2002; Beebe et al., 2012; Piteo et al., 2012). Recent studies have underlined that postnatal depression is associated to infants’ level of oxytocin, imparing their response to stress (Apter-Levy et al., 2013; Feldman, 2015; Pratt et al., 2015). Over the last decade, literature has highlighted that anxiety is also common and often comorbid with depression during the perinatal period (Josefsson et al., 2001; Austin et al., 2010; Field et al., 2010; Beebe et al., 2011; Figueiredo and Conde, 2011).

The prevalence of maternal depression and anxiety in the postpartum period is about 15–20% (O’Hara and Swain, 1996; Guedeney and Jeammet, 2001; Reck et al., 2008; Seymour et al., 2015) and can increase in women that experienced miscarriage, still birth or preterm birth (Davis et al., 2003; Voegtline et al., 2010; Padovani et al., 2011).

When undiagnosed, postnatal depression and anxiety might have long-term effects both on the mother and on the infant (Kersting et al., 2004; Murray et al., 2011) and literature shows how both depression and anxiety impact on the quality of mother–infant interactions, which appear as less synchronous and coordinated than those of non-depressed or non-anxious mother-infant dyads (Mertesacker et al., 2004; Nicol-Harper et al., 2007; Zelkowitz et al., 2009). Depressed mothers appear to be less sensitive and responsive than non-depressed ones; indeed, they are more remote or intrusive during the interaction with their babies, who, in return, tend to react to maternal behavior with passivity or distress (Field, 1984, 1995; Diego et al., 2006; Feldman and Eidelman, 2007). Anxiety has a similar effect on the quality of mother–child interactions, even though the interactive behavior of these dyads has been less explored so far. In particular, recent literature focused on the cognitive components of anxiety and on the specific worries linked to the perinatal period (Murray et al., 2007; Stein et al., 2012).

Maternal Depression and Anxiety after Preterm Birth

Prematurity is defined as the condition of all babies born alive before the 37th week of pregnancy has been completed (World Health Organization [WHO], 2012). It has been estimated that more than 1 out of 10 babies around the world are born preterm and prematurity is one of the leading causes of infant mortality, pediatric morbidity and long-term disability (Aarnoudse-Moens et al., 2009; McCormick et al., 2011). Along with the objective risk for the baby’s health, preterm birth is an unexpected and stressful event for the parents, which may leave them disoriented and frightened (Lasiuk et al., 2013). Parents might experience feelings of guilt, grief and recurrent concerns about their baby’s survival and health (Mendelsohn, 2005; Korja et al., 2009, 2010; Shah et al., 2011; Lasiuk et al., 2013). The persistent worries and the stress experienced can be so intense as to satisfy the criteria of post-traumatic stress disorder (DeMier et al., 2000; Pierrehumbert et al., 2003; Kersting et al., 2004).

The risk of developing anxiety and depression increases when the baby’s prematurity is more severe (Vigod et al., 2010). The prevalence of depression in mothers of very low birth weight (VLBW) infants ranges from 12 to 30% in the first 4 months post-partum (Padovani et al., 2004; Miles et al., 2007; Mehler et al., 2011; Gray et al., 2012). As for anxiety, literature showed that the rate of maternal anxiety ranges from 35 to 43% during the baby’s hospitalization in the NICU (Singer et al., 1999; Padovani et al., 2004, 2011) and from 12 to 26% after its discharge (Padovani et al., 2004; Rogers et al., 2013). However, to our knowledge, there is a lack of studies exploring the cognitive components of maternal anxiety in case of preterm birth. Similarly, the comorbidity between anxiety and depression has been poorly investigated in the maternal context.

Mother–Infant Interactions and Preterm Birth

Recently, an increasing number of studies have focused on the impact of prematurity on early- interactions between mother and baby, finding that preterm dyads experience poorer and less synchronous interactions than full-term ones (Poehlmann and Fiese, 2001; Holditch-Davis et al., 2007; Korja et al., 2012). On the one hand, preterm babies appear as less active and responsive during the interaction with their mothers than full-term infants; this is due to their biological immaturity (Bozzette, 2007; Feldman and Eidelman, 2007). On the other hand, preterm infants’ mothers are generally reported as less sensitive and more intrusive than full term (FT) infants’ ones (Feldman, 2007a; Forcada-Guex et al., 2011).

However, literature on mother–child interactions in preterm dyads shows some inconsistencies (Bozzette, 2007; Korja et al., 2012). There is a lack of data exploring the relationship among the quality of early interactions, maternal depression, anxiety, and the severity of prematurity. To our knowledge, only Agostini et al. (2014) have partially investigated this relationship, finding that the quality of mother-preterm infant interactions could be impaired in specific ways both by the presence of maternal depression and by the severity of premature birth.

Aim of the Study

The objective of the following study was to fulfill the gap existing in previous literature.

The general aim was to explore how the severity of prematurity, depression, and anxiety might impact on the quality of early interactions, therefore to conduct an explorative study. Specifically, we aimed at evaluating if the severity of birth weight was significantly associated to the quality of mother–infant interactions. Secondly, we investigated the influence of maternal depression/anxiety, considered both separately as well as their interaction, on interactive patterns (maternal and infant ones). With reference to anxiety, we specifically focused on detecting maternal worries, which are the cognitive components of anxiety and the key symptoms of generalized anxiety disorder (GAD; American Psychiatric Association [APA], 2000).

To this end, mother–infant interactions in preterm dyads were observed and compared to mother–infant interactions in FT dyads at 3 months post-partum. This time of the assessment was chosen, based on the evidence that the third month postpartum is a significant step for the co- construction of dyadic interactive patterns (Feldman, 2007b; Tronick, 2007) and for the detection of postnatal depression (Cox et al., 1987a; Cramer, 2000).

Materials and Methods

Participants

During the period March 2010-February 2013, all the mothers of preterm infants with birth weight under 1500 g and a gestational age <32 weeks, who had been hospitalized at the NICU of the Bufalini Hospital (Cesena, Italy), were asked to take part in the study. Only five mothers refused to participate. A total of 77 mother–infant dyads were recruited. The severity of their prematurity was evaluated according to their birth weight: 32 infants were extremely low birth weight (ELBW; under 1000 g) and 45 were VLBW (birth weight between 1000 and 1500 g).

During the period March 2011–August 2012, a psychologist met potential subjects for the control group in 36 antenatal classes held in Cesena. Each antenatal class was attended by 10–12 pregnant women, at the third trimester of pregnancy; ~30% in each group accepted to participate in the study voluntarily. All women were included in the sample as none of them had severe complications at delivery and gave birth to a healthy full-term baby. A total of 120 women were recruited (FT group).

Exclusion criteria for both groups were: infant chromosomal abnormalities, cerebral palsy, malformations and fetopathy, previous or present parents’ psychiatric illness and lack of fluency in Italian.

Procedure

This study is part of a longitudinal research that followed mother- preterm infant dyads from 3 to 18 months of corrected age. The study protocol was approved by the Ethic Committee of the Department of Psychology (University of Bologna).

Mother–infant dyads were assessed at infant’s 3 months of age (corrected age for preterm infants). Mothers and their infants were met by a psychologist at the Laboratory of Psychodynamic of Development, Department of Psychology, University of Bologna, Italy.

All mothers completed a written consent form and a socio-demographic questionnaire. Perinatal data were collected for all dyads.

Measures

A general quotient (GQ) of the infants’ development was assessed by means of the Griffiths Mental Development Scales-Revised version (GMDS-R for 0–2 years), a well-recognized measure for infant mental and psychomotor development (Griffiths, 1996). The GQ represents the mean score of 5 areas of development (Locomotor, Personal-Social, Hearing and Language, Eye and Hand Co-ordination, Performance). The GMDS-R has been largely used in Italian samples of preterm infants (Giannì et al., 2007; Monti et al., 2013; Agostini et al., 2014; Biasini et al., 2015b).

The presence of depressive symptoms in the postnatal period was investigated through the Edinburgh Postnatal Depression Scale (EPDS; Cox et al., 1987b), a self-report questionnaire. Mothers were asked to describe their mood during the previous 7 days by means of 10 items, each scoring between 0 to 3, with higher total scores indicating increasing distress. The Italian version (Benvenuti et al., 1999) showed good internal consistency (0.78). In the present study, EPDS was used both as a continuous and a categorical variable (depressed vs. non-depressed), with a cut-off value of 12/13 to screen for major depressive symptomatology, according to a previous Italian study (Agostini et al., 2015).

As to maternal anxiety, all mothers completed the Penn State Worry Questionnaire (PSWQ; Meyer et al., 1990), a self-report questionnaire designed to assess generalized pathological worries, considering the degree of excessiveness and of uncontrollability. It was developed to evaluate an individual’s disposition to worry, as well as the frequency of the condition, its excess or intensity, as well as the tendency for the person to worry generally and not in one or a small number of situations. PSWQ is composed of 16 items, rated on a Likert scale between 1 (Not at all typical of me) to 5 (Very typical of me). Eleven items are positively worded (e.g., “Once I start to worry, I can’t stop”) and five items are negatively worded (e.g., “I never worry about anything”). All negatively worded items are reverse scored and the sum of all the item scores gives a total that ranges from 16 to 80, where the higher the value, the higher the levels of pathological worry. PSWQ was previously used to detect the presence of anxious symptomatology in the perinatal period (Murray et al., 2007; Swanson et al., 2011; O’Connor et al., 2013). The Italian version of PSWQ suggests a cut-off score of 57 to discriminate anxious and non-anxious subjects and showed good internal consistency (0.85) (Morani et al., 1999). As for EPDS, in the present study we used both continuous and categorical scores (anxious vs. non-anxious mothers).

Infant and maternal interactive behaviors were coded by means of the Global Rating Scales (GRS) for Mother–Infant interaction (Murray et al., 1996a,b). Similarly to other rating systems (e.g., Cohn et al., 1986; Field et al., 1990), GRS are video-based assessments of the quality of mother-infant engagement in the 2–4 month postnatal period and have originally been developed to distinguish depressed and non-depressed mother–infant interactions for research purposes. Previous Authors often used GRS to discriminate a wide range of infant and maternal populations (e.g., clinical groups with schizophrenia, social adversity), showing good reliability (Riordan et al., 1999; Gunning et al., 2004; Grant et al., 2010; Costa and Figueiredo, 2011; Montirosso et al., 2012; Agostini and Murray, 2014) and validity in predicting the subsequent child’s performance (Murray et al., 1996a,b). As to the procedure, the mother was asked to sit opposite her baby, and to freely interact for 5 min without toys, as she usually would do at home. Video recordings of the episode were rated by a trained and expert rater (blind to maternal mood) on four maternal behavioral dimensions (Sensitivity, Intrusiveness, Remoteness, Signs of depression) and on three infant’s ones (Communicative, Inert, Distressed). All the dimensions are scored on a 5-point Likert scale, where 1 always corresponds to “poor” interactive maternal or infant behavior and 5 to most “optimal” behavior. A second rater coded ten videos randomly selected: the intra-class correlations showed acceptable reliability (mean = 0.75, range 0.68–0.88).

Results

The infant and maternal socio-demographic characteristics are shown in Table 1.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

TABLE 1. Infant and maternal characteristics.

The three groups showed significant differences regarding the following infant characteristics: gestational age, birth weight, birth length, length of hospitalization, and type of delivery (Table 1); since these variables are strictly linked to preterm birth, these results were expected. No differences emerged regarding the infants’ gender.

As to maternal variables, the three groups were homogeneous with relation to most of the variables, except for parity and level of education (Table 1): FT infants’ mothers, compared to ELBW and VLBW ones, were primiparous in a higher percentage [χ2(2) = 11.495; p = 0.003] and had a higher level of education [F(1,196) = 12.023; p < 0.0005; Bonferroni post hoc: p < 0.0005 and p = 0.003, respectively].

Moreover, the level of infant development, as measured by the GQ (GMDS-R) was significantly different among the three groups [F(2,196) = 9.69, p < 0.0005; Table 1]: Bonferroni’s post hoc analyses showed that ELBW infants had significantly lower scores than both VLBW and FT groups (p = 0.004 and p < 0.0005, respectively).

Specific analyses were run to control the effect of maternal parity, years of education and GQ on EPDS, PSWQ and GRS scores: parity did not show any significant influence, while education and level of development were significantly associated with dependent variables. Therefore, the years of education and GQ were always included as covariates in consecutive analyses in order to control their influence.

Mother–Infant Interactions

The Univariate ANOVA was run for each GRS scale in order to analyze the main effects of birth weight, maternal depression and anxiety variables on interactive behaviors, also considering their possible interaction; GQ score and maternal education were always included as covariates (Table 2).

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

TABLE 2. Mother–infant interactive behaviors (GRS): differences among groups.

Birth Weight

The three groups showed significant differences on all the scales about maternal behaviors: Sensitivity [F(2,196) = 3.147; p = 0.045], Intrusiveness [F(2,196) = 4.993; p = 0.008], Remoteness [F(2,196) = 2.985; p = 0.050] and Signs of Depression [F(2,196) = 5.720; p = 0.004; Table 2].

In the case of Sensitivity dimension, VLBW mothers obtained higher scores than those of FT infants (Bonferroni post hoc, p = 0.031). When maternal Intrusiveness and Remoteness dimensions were considered, ELBW mothers showed more intrusive and less remote behaviors than the mothers of FT ones (Bonferroni post hoc, p = 0.006; p = 0.012). Finally, Bonferroni post hoc showed higher scores on Signs of Depression dimension in the mothers of ELBW and VLBW infants than those of FT infants (p = 0.028; p = 0.022, respectively; Figure 1).

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 1. Mother–infant interactive behaviors mean scores according to Birth Weight.

When infant’s scales were considered, a significant effect of birth weight emerged on Communicative dimension [F(2,196) = 6.436; p = 0.002]: VLBW infants showed higher scores than those of FT infants (Bonferroni post hoc, p = 0.016). No significant effect on any other infant dimensions emerged (Figure 1).

Maternal Symptomatology: Depression and Anxiety

When we considered the categorical score of EPDS (depressed vs. non-depressed), 10.2% women (n = 20) of the total sample resulted in the “depressed group.”

As to mother–infant interactions, maternal depression showed a significant effect on the mean scores of Remoteness dimension [F(1,196) = 7.389; p = 0.007]: depressed mothers were more remote than non-depressed ones. Besides, depressed mothers showed a lower mean score on the Signs of depression dimension [F(1,149) = 5.373; p = 0.022; Table 2], meaning that their interactive behavior was significantly affected by their negative affective state in terms of low energy, sad facial expressions and tendency to self-absorption. No differences emerged between depressed and non-depressed groups on mothers’ Sensitivity and Intrusiveness dimensions and on any of the infants’ dimensions considered (Table 2).

Considering the categorical score of PSWQ (anxious vs. non-anxious), the “anxious group” was composed of 7.6% of the women (n = 15%) of the total sample.

With regard to mother–infant interaction, the analyses showed no differences between the two groups on the dimensions of Intrusiveness, Remoteness, and Sign of Depression (Table 2). Indeed, anxious mothers obtained lower mean scores than non-anxious ones as to Sensitive dimension [F(1,196) = 4.355; p = 0.038]: this means that they showed less ability to detect and understand the infant’s signals and to respond with adequate levels of acceptance, affection and warmth. No differences emerged between anxious and non-anxious groups on any dimensions of the infants’ interactive behavior (Table 2).

A significant interaction effect emerged between maternal depressive and anxiety symptomatology on the mean scores of Signs of Depression [F(1,196) = 3.726; p = 0.050], Communicative [F(1,196) = 6.041; p = 0.015], and Inert dimension [F(1,196) = 3.777; p = 0.050]. In all cases, the comorbidity of depression and anxiety was associated to lower levels of affective maternal behaviors and less attention, communication and engagement in the infant (Figure 2).

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 2. Communicative and Inert mean scores in relation to depressive × anxious symptomatology.

Birth Weight and Maternal Symptomatology

The three birth weight groups showed significant differences on EPDS mean scores [F(2,196) = 11.345; p = 0.001]: the mothers of ELBW infants showed higher scores compared to those of VLBW and FT ones (10.06, 6.62, 5.39, respectively; Bonferroni post hoc test p = 0.015 and p < 0.0005, respectively). The Pearson chi square test showed a significantly higher frequency of depressed women in the ELBW group compared to those of the FT group (25.0, 4.2%, respectively; χ2(2) = 13.888, p = 0.001); even if 15.6% of VLBW infants’ mothers were depressed, their prevalence did not significantly differ from the other two groups.

The interaction between birth weight and maternal depression showed no significant effect on any interactive dimensions mean scores.

When maternal anxiety was considered, the mean scores of PSWQ resulted significantly different among the three categories of birth weight [F(2,196) = 4.31; p = 0.026]: Bonferroni post hoc analysis showed that ELBW infants’ mothers got higher scores compared to FT infants’ ones (44.22, 37.85, respectively; p = 0.026). The Pearson chi square test showed a significantly higher prevalence of anxious women in the ELBW group compared to those in the VLBW and FT ones [21.9, 6.7, 4.2%, respectively; χ2(2) = 11.336, p = 0.003].

Differently from maternal depression, a significant interaction between birth weight and anxiety emerged on the means scores of Sensitivity, Signs of Depression and Communicative dimensions [F(2,196) = 5.513; p = 0.007; F(2,196) = 3.445; p = 0.034; F(2,196) = 5.768; p = 0.004, respectively; Table 2]. Simple effect analyses showed that, only in FT mothers, lower scores were strongly associated to the presence of anxious symptoms, while for ELBW and VLBW mothers the quality of interaction was similar, independently from the exhibition of anxiety symptoms (Figure 3).

FIGURE 3
www.frontiersin.org

FIGURE 3. Sensitivity and Signs of Depressions mean scores in relation to birth weight × anxious symptomatology.

Discussion

Many studies have investigated the effect of maternal affective state on the quality of mother–infant interactions in the first 3 months of life, especially in the context of preterm birth. However, to our knowledge, there is a lack of studies exploring both the direct and the combined effect of maternal affective state (depressive and anxious symptoms) and the severity of preterm birth on early interactions. Therefore, we aimed at investigating how mother–infant interactions were influenced by maternal depression, anxiety and the severity of preterm birth.

A first interesting result is related to the effect of preterm birth on early interactions. Even if the development of mother–infant relationship in case of preterm birth has been deeply analyzed in literature, the effect of birth weight as a risk factor is still understudied. Our study has the strength of considering two groups of preterm infants characterized by a different level of severity of prematurity (very/extremely low birth weight) and a control group of full-term dyads.

In the present study, birth weight did not seem to compromise the quality of infant interactive patterns: preterm babies showed similar interactive scores compared to full-term infants; indeed, differently from what emerged from previous studies (Crawford, 1982; Singer et al., 2003; Korja et al., 2012; De Schuymer et al., 2012), they did not show high levels of passivity, fretful and disengaged behaviors. Moreover, VLBW infants appeared very communicative, with higher mean scores when compared to full-term ones.

When maternal interactive patterns are considered, many differences emerged as to birth weight. However, the results seem to indicate that birth weight has a different and specific effect on the single dimensions of maternal interactive behavior. Globally, all mothers showed good scores on Sensitivity dimensions, with higher mean scores in VLBW mothers compared to FT mothers. This result was somehow unexpected, because many authors previously described preterm babies’ mothers as non-sensitive (Zarling et al., 1988; Muller-Nix et al., 2004; Forcada-Guex et al., 2006, 2011). Nevertheless, other studies failed to find significant differences between maternal interactive behaviors when comparing preterm to FT mothers (Greenberg and Crnic, 1988; Schermann-Eizirik et al., 1997; Korja et al., 2008a; Montirosso et al., 2010), showing how prematurity was associated to high levels of caretaking (Crawford, 1982; Jean and Stack, 2012). The attention paid to different categories of prematurity may show specific interactive patterns, which did not emerge in previous studies where prematurity was considered as a global and homogenous category. The same explanation may be useful to understand the differences between VLBW and FT infants that emerged on infant communication dimensions. It is also important to consider the characteristics of the instrument chosen to evaluate interactive behaviors; GRS scales describe Sensitivity as the mother’s ability to detect and understand her infant’s signals and to appropriately respond, with adequate levels of acceptance, affection and warmth (Murray et al., 1996a). Therefore, our study may stress the differences related to the warmth and affection showed by VLBW mothers.

When the maternal intrusive and remote behaviors were considered, the results were consistent with previous studies (Crawford, 1982; Muller-Nix et al., 2004; Forcada-Guex et al., 2006; Korja et al., 2012) that described preterm mothers as very active and overstimulated. However, this pattern was detected only in the ELBW infants’ mothers. In the case of VLBW, the higher level of infant communication may possibly endorse maternal behaviors, reducing their intrusiveness. This result may add some information to the previous literature (Schmücker et al., 2005; Forcada-Guex et al., 2011) about the role of birth weight as a possible moderator between preterm birth and maternal intrusiveness, thus widening the areas for future investigation.

Another interesting result is related to the mean scores obtained by both groups of preterm infants’ mothers on the Signs of Depression dimension. This data may appear inconsistent with the prevalence of depression found by means of EPDS, which was significantly higher in ELBW mothers than in FT ones. As to the Sensitivity dimension, some considerations must be taken into account. The prevalence of depression detected with the EPDS concerns the subjective perception of maternal affective state as to sense of guilt, lack of pleasure, and dissatisfaction. To this end, the levels of depression found in ELBW infants’ mothers confirm and enrich the existing literature (Padovani et al., 2004; Miles et al., 2007; Vigod et al., 2010; Brandon et al., 2011; Gray et al., 2012), showing how the higher is the severity of prematurity the higher is the risk of maternal depression. On the contrary, the GRS Signs of Depression dimension focuses on detecting depressive symptoms emerged during the interaction with their baby in terms of low energy, self-absorption and poor engagement with the infant (Murray et al., 1996a). It should be noted that literature on prematurity shows preterm infants’ mothers to be engaged with their babies also in case of depression (Agostini et al., 2014): this pattern was observed in both groups of preterm dyads (ELBW and VLBW) and it might represent a specific interactive behavior of their mothers, while depression detected by EPDS was high only in the case of ELBW infants’ mothers. As a result, the difference found between the scores of the EPDS and the GRS Sign of Depression dimension might be explained as the different aspects of depression investigated by the two instruments. Moreover, it should be considered that while the EPDS is a self-report questionnaire, the GRS is a measure of the quality of interaction evaluated by a blinded rater: therefore he can observe also adequate interactive behaviors independently by the level of maternal sense of guilt, lack of pleasure, and dissatisfaction. This aspect has clinical implications: when interventions are planned to support parenting skills, the GRS may enrich maternal self-representation, showing positive aspects that could be missed by the mother due to her depressive mood/state.

Another objective of the study was to evaluate the effect of maternal symptomatology (depression and anxiety) on the quality of mother–infant interactions. The results seem to show that, when their interaction is considered, maternal depression was associated with Remoteness and Sign of Depression dimensions, while maternal anxiety significantly affected the level of Sensitivity.

The results seem to underline that both kind of symptoms impaired the relationship between mothers and their infants, but with a specific effect on maternal behaviors: depression has a negative influence on the maternal ability to stay close to her infant, on her level of energy and engagement during the interaction; conversely, anxiety has a major effect on the skills of perceiving and responding to infant cues. This result is very relevant and widens the literature on the topic, since previously depression and anxiety have been investigated separately.

Another peculiarity of the present study was the use of PSWQ to detect the symptoms of maternal anxiety. While in previous studies about perinatality, anxiety was often evaluated through generic instruments as STAI (Padovani et al., 2004, 2008; Correia and Linhares, 2007; Zelkowitz et al., 2007, 2009), we chose to focus on worries, a specific component of anxiety. It could be hypothesized that the tendency to worry or ruminate might alter the mother’s attentional focus, reducing her ability to adequately respond to her infant’s cues (Stein et al., 2012).

Interestingly, the study did not detect a direct effect of anxiety on any infant dimensions. However, when the interaction between maternal depression and anxiety was considered, we found a significant effect on infant Communicative and Inert dimensions: in both cases, the infant interactive behaviors were negatively influenced by the presence of maternal depression and anxiety. Besides, a significant interaction effect emerged on the Signs of Depression dimension, underlining how the level of maternal affective state, as measured by GRS scales, was affected by the presence of depression and worsened in case of comorbidity with anxiety – a cumulative effect.

Globally, the results seem to indicate a direct effect of maternal symptomatology (depression or anxiety) on maternal behaviors, whereas infant interactive patters seem to be more influenced by the co-occurrence of maternal depression and anxiety. Considering that the comorbidity of depression and anxiety is high in the first postpartum period (18–34%; Reck et al., 2008), it is very important that future studies pay particular attention to the effects of co-occurrence of both maternal depression and anxiety.

The study confirmed how the first postpartum months can be a very sensitive period with an elevate risk of onset of maternal depression and anxiety, especially in case of preterm birth. However, it is important to note that, when preterm birth was considered, a high prevalence of both depressive and anxious symptoms only in ELBW mothers were found, while in case of VLBW group the frequency was similar to those of full-term mothers.

As to depressive symptomatology, the review by Vigod et al. (2010) showed how a severe preterm birth may be associated, during the first postnatal year, with higher levels of depressive symptoms in LBW and VLBW infants’ mothers. This study deepened Vigod’s results adding the evaluation on ELBW infants’ mothers.

To our knowledge, there are no specific studies that investigate the prevalence of maternal anxiety according to preterm birth weight. In this research, the use of PSWQ showed a high tendency of worries or rumination only in ELBW infants’ mothers. The extreme severity of prematurity may possibly represent a very frightful event for mothers with concerns and worries which last for many months after the discharge of the baby (Singer et al., 1999; Wijnroks, 1999; Kersting et al., 2004; Feeley et al., 2007).

Considering both depression and anxiety, the results give a contribution to literature and it emphasizes the need for future research to distinguish among different preterm conditions. This element could deepen existing literature and identify those parents who, after a preterm birth, are forced to face stress with less adaptive coping skills.

However, it should be noted that, in full-term mothers, the rate of maternal depressive and anxiety symptoms was slightly lower compared to the prevalence reported in other studies (depression: 4.2 vs. 10%; Murray et al., 1996a; Gavin et al., 2005; anxiety: 4.2 vs. 27.9%, Ross et al., 2003; Britton, 2005). This result could be partially influenced by the method of recruitment and explained by the sample characteristics; therefore, it is suggested to confirm these data on wider samples in the future.

According to the objectives of this study, an interactive significant effect emerged between the birth weight and maternal symptomatology only in case of maternal anxiety. As previously reported, the use of PSWQ could be particularly adequate to detect how the tendency to worry may impair the maternal ability to capture and reply to their infant’s needs (Stein et al., 2012). Interestingly, this effect is particularly evident in the mothers of the control group. In absence of birth risk factor, this group of mothers tends to interact with the infant in a sensitive way. The presence of anxious symptomatology can worsen the maternal ability to interact with the infants, while maternal anxiety may represent a very common condition when premature birth occurs. The influences of anxiety on mother–infant interactions could be then screened by the preterm birth, while anxiety may function as an adaptive response in this traumatic condition to help mothers to maintain the focus on the infant and their relationship.

Differently, depression have the same negative effects on preterm and FT dyads, confirming existing literature (Murray et al., 1996a; Korja et al., 2008b; Poehlmann et al., 2011).

Some considerations must be done regarding the NICU where preterm infants were recruited. According to NIDCAP guidelines (Als and Gibes, 1986; Ohlsson and Jacobs, 2013), all the staff is careful to protect and enhance the infant’s and parents’ quality of life. Since the first moments of life, the relationship between preterm infant and their parents is guaranteed by 24-h free access to the unit. In fact, after preterm birth mothers could feel inadequate to care to such a fragile infant, a number of specialized professional figures (e.g., physiotherapists, clinical psychologists) are present to help them feel useful and involved in their infant’s care. To this end, many methods are used, such as kangaroo care, infant massage, both useful to teach to touch the infant in a comfortable way, and reading a little fairy-tale, which allow the baby to listen to maternal voice as it used to do during pregnancy (Biasini et al., 2015a). Literature states that these interventions are very important to reduce the level of psychological distress in the NICU and after discharge (Melnyk et al., 2006; Trombini et al., 2008), with positive effects on the quality of the dyadic interaction. These elements could explain the high levels of maternal sensitivity and infant communicative interactive patterns found in this study. These protective factors could be particularly relevant for the VLBW group, which being less at-risk may better benefit from the interventions. Globally, when a NICU functions as a supportive environment, it can promote positive mother–infant interactions during hospitalization, inducing mechanistic changes in the brain structure and function, maximizing positive neurodevelopmental outcomes and reducing neurologic deficits (Weber et al., 2012).

Some limits of the study might be acknowledged. Firstly, all results need replication, based on studies with wider samples. Secondly, in the present study the role of the infant’s father was not considered. Because the father plays a relevant role in moderating the effects of maternal depression on dyadic interactions and can represent a strong source of support for his partner (Robertson et al., 2004; Fletcher, 2009), it is advisable that future studies should deepen this. Moreover, further investigation could better understand the possible influences of maternal characteristics, i.e., parity or level of education. Finally, it will be clinically relevant to understand how these mother–infant interactions will evolve in a longitudinal way.

Conclusion

Globally, the results suggested that mothers of both preterm groups’ appear involved and close to their babies. Nevertheless, the ELBW preterm group was characterized by a higher risk of maternal symptomatology and by a more “intrusive and controlling” mothering. These results, therefore, seem to suggest that, within preterm populations, the ELBW dyads could represent a sample with peculiar characteristics, specific needs and difficulties. So, during ELBW babies’ hospitalization and at the moment of their discharge, hospital staff should pay special attention to both the infant’s development and the parental affective state, in order to prevent the onset of depression or anxiety and to give a prompt intervention. Specifically, ad hoc interventions should be promoted to assess the risk of depressive or anxiety symptoms with adequate tools to give special support and treatment for symptomatology and to enhance parental functioning.

The longitudinal evaluation of mother–infant interaction in these dyads will help give a more comprehensive description of the long-term effects of depressive and anxious symptoms on interactive patterns, and if birth weight could act as a moderator in the relationship between maternal and infant interactive patterns.

Conflict of Interest Statement

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

References

Aarnoudse-Moens, C. S., Weisglas-Kuperus, N., van Goudoever, J. B., and Oosterlaan, J. (2009). Meta-analysis of neurobehavioral outcomes in very preterm and/or very low birth weight children. Pediatrics 124, 717–728. doi: 10.1542/peds.2008-2816

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Agostini, F., and Murray, L. (2014). “Metodi osservativi dell’interazione adulto-bambino. I. Global rating scales of mother-infant interaction,” in La Funzione Genitoriale. Sviluppo e Psicopatologia, ed. A. Simonelli (Milan: Cortina Editore), 147–161.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Agostini, F., Neri, E., Dellabartola, S., Biasini, A., and Monti, F. (2014). Early interactive behaviours in preterm infants and their mothers: influences of maternal depressive symptomatology and neonatal birth weight. Infant Behav. Dev. 37, 86–93. doi: 10.1016/j.infbeh.2013.12.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Agostini, F., Neri, E., Salvatori, P., Dellabartola, S., Bozicevic, L., and Monti, F. (2015). Antenatal depressive symptoms associated with specific life events and sources of social support among italian women. Matern. Child Health J. 19, 1131–1141. doi: 10.1007/s10995-014-1613-x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Als, H., and Gibes, R. (1986). Newborn Individualized Developmental Care and Assessment Program (Nidcap). Training guide. Boston, MA: Children’s Hospital.

Google Scholar

American Psychiatric Association [APA]. (2000). DSM-IV-TR. Manuale Diagnostico e Statistico e Statico dei Disturbi Mentali [DSM-IV-TR. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders]. Milano: Masson.

Google Scholar

Apter-Levy, Y., Feldman, M., Vakart, A., Ebstein, R. P., and Feldman, R. (2013). Impact of maternal depression across the first 6 years of life on the child’s mental health, social engagement, and empathy: the moderating role of oxytocin. Am. J. Psychiatry 170, 1161–1168. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2013.12121597

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Austin, M. P., Hadzi-Pavlovic, D., Priest, S. R., Reilly, N., Wilhelm, K., Saint, K., et al. (2010). Depressive and anxiety disorders in the postpartum period: how prevalent are they and can we improve their detection? Arch. Womens Ment. Health 13, 395–401. doi: 10.1007/s00737-010-0153-7

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Beebe, B., Lachmann, F., Jaffe, J., Markese, S., Buck, K. A., Chen, H., et al. (2012). Maternal postpartum depressive symptoms and 4-month mother–infant interaction. Psychoanal. Psychol. 29, 383–407. doi: 10.1037/a0029387

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Beebe, B., Steele, M., Jaffe, J., Buck, K. A., Chen, H., Cohen, P., et al. (2011). Maternal anxiety symptoms and mother–infant self- and interactive contingency. Infant Ment. Health J. 32, 174–206. doi: 10.1002/imhj.20274

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Benvenuti, P., Ferrara, M., Niccolai, C., Valoriani, V., and Cox, J. L. (1999). The edinburgh postnatal depression scale: validation for an italian sample. J. Affect. Disord. 53, 137–141. doi: 10.1016/S0165-0327(98)00102-5

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Biasini, A., Neri, E., Gianstefani, I., Bertozzi, L., Agostini, F., and Monti, F. (2015a). Griffiths mental development scales as a tool for the screening of motor disability in premature infants: is it worth it? J. Clin. Neonatol. 4, 22–25. doi: 10.4103/2249-4847.151162

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Biasini, A., Neri, E., Fiuzzi, F., Stella, M., and Monti, F. (2015b). Leggere storie ai neonati pretermine in terapia intensiva è una marcia in più nel prendersi cura di loro: risultati dopo due anni dall’inizio dell’intervento. Quad. ACP 1, 13–16.

Google Scholar

Bozzette, M. A. (2007). Review of research on premature infant-mother interaction. Newborn Infant Nurs. Rev. 7, 49–55. doi: 10.1053/j.nainr.2006.12.002

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Brandon, D. H., Tully, K. P., Silvia, S. G., Malcom, W. F., Murtha, A. P., Turner, B. S., et al. (2011). Emotional responses of mothers of late-preterm and term infants. J. Obstet. Gynecol. Neonatal Nurs. 40, 716–731. doi: 10.1111/j.1552-6909.2011.01290.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Britton, J. R. (2005). Pre-discharge anxiety among mothers of well newborns: prevalence and correlates. Acta Paediatr. 94, 1771–1776. doi: 10.1111/j.1651-2227.2005.tb01852.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cohn, J. F., Matias, R., Tronick, E. Z., Connell, D., and Lyons-Ruth, K. (1986). Face-to-face interactions of depressed mothers and their infants. New Dir. Child Adolesc. Dev. 34, 31–45. doi: 10.1002/cd.23219863405

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Correia, L. L., and Linhares, M. B. M. (2007). Maternal anxiety in the pre- and postnatal period: a literature review. Rev. Lat. Am. Enfermagem 15, 677–683. doi: 10.1590/S0104-11692007000400024

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Costa, R., and Figueiredo, B. (2011). Infant’s psychophysiological profile and temperament at 3 and 12 months. Infant Behav. Dev. 34, 270–279. doi: 10.1016/j.infbeh.2011.01.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cox, A. D., Puckering, C., Pound, A., and Mills, M. (1987a). The impact of maternal depression in young children. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 28, 917–928. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.1987.tb00679.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cox, J., Holden, J. M., and Sagovsky, R. (1987b). Detection of postnatal depression, development of the 10-item edinburgh postnatal depression scale. Br. J. Psychiatry 150, 782–786. doi: 10.1192/bjp.150.6.782

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Cramer, B. (2000). Cosa Diventeranno i Nostri Bambini? London: Raffaello Cortina Editore.

Google Scholar

Crawford, J. W. (1982). Mother-infant interaction in premature and full-term infants. Child Dev. 53, 957–962. doi: 10.2307/1129134

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Davis, L., Edwards, H., Mohay, H., and Wollin, J. (2003). The impact of very premature birth on the psychological health of mothers. Early Hum. Dev. 73, 61–70. doi: 10.1016/S0378-3782(03)00073-2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

De Schuymer, L., De Groote, I., Desoete, A., and Roeyers, H. (2012). Gaze aversion during social interaction in preterm infants: a function of attention skills? Infant Behav. Dev. 35, 129–139. doi: 10.1016/j.infbeh.2011.08.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

DeMier, R. L., Hynan, M. T., Hatfield, R. F., Varner, M. W., Harris, H. B., and Manniello, R. L. (2000). A measurement model of perinatal stressors: identifying risk for postnatal emotional distress in mothers of high-risk infants. J. Clin. Psychol. 56, 89–100.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Diego, M. A., Field, F., Jones, N. A., and Hernandez-Reif, M. (2006). Withdrawn and intrusive maternal interaction style and infant frontal EEG assimetry shifts in infants of depressed and non-depressed mothers. Infant Behav. Dev. 29, 220–229. doi: 10.1016/j.infbeh.2005.12.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Feeley, N., Gottlieb, L., and Zelkowitz, P. (2007). Mothers and fathers of very low-birthweight infants: similarities and differences in the first year after birth. J. Obstet. Gynecol. Neonatal Nurs. 36, 558–567. doi: 10.1111/j.1552-6909.2007.00186.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Feldman, R. (2007a). Maternal versus child risk and the development of parent-child and family relationships in five high-risk populations. Dev. Psychopathol. 19, 293–312. doi: 10.1017/S0954579407070150

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Feldman, R. (2007b). Parent-infant synchrony and the construction of shared timing; physiological precursors, developmental outcomes, and risk conditions. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 48, 329–354. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2006.01701.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Feldman, R. (2015). Sensitive periods in human social development: new insights from research on oxytocin, synchrony, and high-risk parenting. Dev. Psychopathol. 27, 369–395. doi: 10.1017/S0954579415000048

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Feldman, R., and Eidelman, A. I. (2007). Maternal postpartum behavior and the emergence of infant-mother and infant-father synchrony in preterm and full-term infants: the role of neonatal vagal tone. Dev. Psychobiol. 49, 290–302. doi: 10.1002/dev.20220

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Field, T. (1984). Early interactions between infants and their postpartum depressed mothers. Infant Behav. Dev. 7, 527–532. doi: 10.1016/S0163-6383(84)80179-4

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Field, T. (1995). Infants of depressed mothers. Infant Behav. Dev. 18, 1–13. doi: 10.1016/0163-6383(95)90003-9

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Field, T., Diego, M., Hernandez-Reif, M., Figueiredo, B., Deeds, O., Ascencio, A., et al. (2010). Comorbid depression and anxiety effects on pregnancy and neonatal outcome. Infant Behav. Dev. 33, 23–29. doi: 10.1016/j.infbeh.2009.10.004

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Field, T., Healy, B. T., Goldstein, S., and Guthertz, M. (1990). Behavior-state matching and synchrony in mother–infant interactions of non-depressed versus depressed dyads. Dev. Psychol. 26, 7–14. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.26.1.7

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Figueiredo, B., and Conde, A. (2011). Anxiety and depression symptoms in women and men from early pregnancy to 3-months postpartum: parity differences and effects. J. Affect. Disord. 132, 146–157. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2011.02.007

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fletcher, R. (2009). Promoting infant well-being in the context of maternal depression by supporting the father. Inf. Mental Health. J. 30, 95–102. doi: 10.1002/imhj.20205

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Forcada-Guex, M., Borghini, A., Pierrehumbert, B., Ansermet, F., and Muller-Nix, C. (2011). Prematurity, maternal posttraumatic stress and consequences on the mother-infant relationship. Early Hum. Dev. 87, 21–26. doi: 10.1016/j.earlhumdev.2010.09.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Forcada-Guex, M., Pierrehumbert, B., Borghini, A., Moessinger, A., and Muller-Nix, C. (2006). Early dyadic patterns of mother-infant interactions and outcomes of prematurity at 18 months. Pediatrics 118, E107–E114. doi: 10.1542/peds.2005-1145

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gavin, N. I., Gaynes, B. N., Lohr, K. N., Meltzer-Brody, S., Gartlehner, G., and Swinson, T. (2005). Perinatal depression: a systematic review of prevalence and incidence. Obstet. Gynecol. 106, 1071–1083. doi: 10.1097/01.AOG.0000183597.31630.db

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Giannì, M. L., Picciolini, O., Vegni, C., Gardon, L., Fumagalli, M., and Mosca, F. (2007). Twelve-month neurofunctional assessment and cognitive performance at 36 months of age in extremely low birth weight infants. Pediatrics 120, 1012–1019. doi: 10.1542/peds.2006-3364

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Grant, K. A., McMahon, C., Reilly, N., and Austin, M. P. (2010). Maternal sensitivity moderates the impact of prenatal anxiety disorder on infant responses to the still-face procedure. Infant Behav. Dev. 33, 453–462. doi: 10.1016/j.infbeh.2010.05.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gray, P. H., Edwards, D. M., O’Callaghan, M. J., and Cuskelly, M. (2012). Parenting stress in mothers of preterm infants during early infancy. Early Hum. Dev. 88, 45–49. doi: 10.1016/j.earlhumdev.2011.06.014

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Greenberg, M. T., and Crnic, K. A. (1988). Longitudinal predictors of developmental status and social interaction in premature and full-term infants at age two. Child Dev. 59, 554–570. doi: 10.2307/1130557

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Griffiths, R. (1996). The Griffiths Mental Development Scales. Henley: Association for Research in Infant and Child Development, Test Agency.

Google Scholar

Guedeney, N., and Jeammet, P. (2001). Dépressions postnatales (DPN) et décisions d’orientation thérapeutique. Devenir 3, 51–64. doi: 10.3917/dev.013.0051

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gunning, M., Conroy, S., Valoriani, V., Figueiredo, B., Kammerer, M. H., Muzik, M., et al. (2004). Measurement of mother–infant interactions and the home environment in a European setting: preliminary results from a cross-cultural study. Br. J. Psychiatry 184, s38–s44. doi: 10.1192/bjp.184.46.s38

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Holditch-Davis, D., Schwartz, T., Black, B., and Scher, M. (2007). Correlates of mother-premature infant interactions. Res. Nurs. Health 30, 333–346. doi: 10.1002/nur.20190

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jean, A. D., and Stack, D. M. (2012). Full-term and very-low-birth-weight preterm infants’ self-regulating behaviors during a Still-Face interaction: influences of maternal touch. Infant Behav. Dev. 35, 779–791. doi: 10.1016/j.infbeh.2012.07.023

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Josefsson, A., Berg, G., Nordin, C., and Sydsjo, G. (2001). Prevalence of depressive symptoms in late pregnancy and postpartum. Acta Obstet. Gynecol. Scand. 80, 251–255. doi: 10.1034/j.1600-0412.2001.080003251.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Kersting, A., Dorsch, M., Wesselmann, U., Lüdorff, K., Witthaut, J., Ohrmann, P., et al. (2004). Maternal posttraumatic stress response after the birth of a very low-birth-weight infant. J. Psychosom. Res. 57, 473–476. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychores.2004.03.011

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Korja, R., Ahlqvist-Bjorkroth, S., Savonlahti, E., Stolt, S., Haataja, L., Lapinleimu, H., et al. (2010). Relations between maternal attachment representations and the quality of mother-infant interaction in preterm and full-term infants. Infant Behav. Dev. 33, 330–336. doi: 10.1016/j.infbeh.2010.03.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Korja, R., Latva, R., and Lehtonen, L. (2012). The effects of preterm birth on mater-infant interaction and attachment during the first two years. Acta Obstet. Gynecol. Scand. 91, 164–173. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-0412.2011.01304.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Korja, R., Maunu, J., Kirjavainen, J., Savonlahti, E., Haataja, L., Lapinleimu, H., et al. (2008a). Mother-infant interaction is influenced by the amount of holding in preterm infants. Early Hum. Dev. 84, 257–267. doi: 10.1016/j.earlhumdev.2007.06.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Korja, R., Savonlahti, E., Ahlqvist-Björkroth, S., Stolt, S., Haataja, L., Lapinleimu, H., et al. (2008b). Maternal depression is associated with mother–infant interaction in preterm infants. Acta Paediatr. 97, 724–730. doi: 10.1111/j.1651-2227.2008.00733.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Korja, R., Savonlahti, E., Haataja, L., Lapinleimu, H., Manninen, H., Piha, J., et al. (2009). Attachment representations in mothers of preterm infants. Infant Behav. Dev. 32, 305–311. doi: 10.1016/j.infbeh.2009.04.003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lasiuk, G. C., Comeau, T., and Newburn-Cook, C. (2013). Unexpected: an interpretative description of parental traumas’ associated with preterm birth. BMC Pregnancy Childbirth 13:S13. doi: 10.1186/1471-2393-13-S1-S13

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Martins, C., and Gaffan, E. A. (2000). Effects of early maternal depression on patterns of infant-mother attachment: a meta-analytic investigation. J. Child Psychol. Psychiarty 41, 737–746. doi: 10.1111/1469-7610.00661

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

McCormick, M. C., Litt, J. S., Smith, V. C., and Zupancic, J. A. F. (2011). Prematurity: an overview and public health implications. Annu. Rev. Public Health 32, 367–379. doi: 10.1146/annurev-publhealth-090810-182459

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mehler, K., Wendrich, D., Kissgen, R., Roth, B., Oberthuer, A., Pillekamp, F., et al. (2011). Mothers seeing their VLBW infants within 3 h after birth are more likely to establish a secure attachment behavior: evidence of a sensitive period with preterm infants? J. Perinatol. 31, 404–410. doi: 10.1038/jp.2010.139

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Melnyk, B. M., Feinstein, N. F., Alpert-Gillis, L., Fairbanks, E., Crean, H. F., Sinkin, R. A., et al. (2006). Reducing premature infants’ length of stay and improving parents’ mental health outcomes with the Creating Opportunities for Parent Empowerment (COPE) neonatal intensive care unit program: a randomized, controlled trial. Pediatrics 118, e1414–e1427. doi: 10.1542/peds2005-2580

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mendelsohn, A. (2005). Recovering reverie: using infant observation in interventions with traumatised mothers and their premature babies. Infant Obs. 8, 195–208. doi: 10.1080/13698030500375693

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Mertesacker, B., Bade, U., Haverkock, A., and Pauli-Pott, U. (2004). Predicting maternal reactivity/sensitivity: the role of infant emotionality, maternal depressiveness/anxiety, and social support. Infant Mental Health J. 25, 47–61. doi: 10.1002/imhj.10085

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Meyer, T. J., Miller, M. L., Metzger, R. L., and Borkovec, T. D. (1990). Development and validation of the penn state worry questionnaire. Behav. Res. Ther. 28, 487–495. doi: 10.1016/0005-7967(90)90135-6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Miles, M. S., Holditch-Davis, D., Schwartz, T. A., and Scher, M. (2007). Depressive symptoms in mothers of prematurely born infants. J. Dev. Behav. Pediatr. 28, 36–44. doi: 10.1097/01.DBP.0000257517.52459.7a

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Monti, F., Neri, E., Trombini, E., Aureliano, F., Biasini, A., and Agostini, F. (2013). Prematurity: parental stress, temperament and child development. Euro. J. Child Dev. Educ. Psychopathol. 1, 141–155.

Google Scholar

Montirosso, R., Borgatti, R., Trojan, S., Zanini, R., and Tronick, E. (2010). A comparison of dyadic interactions and coping with still-face in healthy pre-term and full-term infants. Br. J. Dev. Psychol. 28, 347–368. doi: 10.1348/026151009X416429

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Montirosso, R., Fedeli, C., Murray, L., Morandi, F., Brusati, R., Perego, G. G., et al. (2012). The role of negative maternal affective states and infant temperamentin early interactions between infants with cleft lip and their mothers. J. Pediatr. Psychol. 37, 241–250. doi: 10.1093/jpepsy/jsr089

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Morani, S., Pricci, D., and Sanavio, E. (1999). Penn state worry questionnaire e worry domains questionnaire. presentazione delle versioni italiane ed analisi della fedeltà. Psicoter. Cogn. Comportament. 5, 195–209.

Google Scholar

Muller-Nix, C., Forcada-Guez, M., Pierrehumbert, B., Jaunin, L., Borghini, A., and Ansermet, F. (2004). Prematurity, maternal stress and mother-child interactions. Early Hum. Dev. 79, 145–158. doi: 10.1016/j.earlhumdev.2004.05.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Murray, L., Arteche, A., Fearon, P., Halligan, S., Goodyer, I., and Cooper, P. (2011). Maternal postnatal depression and the development of depression in offspring up to 16 years of age. J. Am. Acad. Child Psychiatry 50, 460–470. doi: 10.1016/j.jaac.2011.02.001

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Murray, L., Cooper, P. J., Creswell, C., Schofield, E., and Sack, C. (2007). The effects of maternal social phobia on mother–infant interactions and infant social responsiveness. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 48, 45–52. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2006.01657.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Murray, L., Fiori-Cowley, A., Hooper, R., and Cooper, P. (1996a). The impact of postnatal depression and associated adversity on early mother–infant interactions and later infant outcome. Child Dev. 67, 2512–2526. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.1996.tb01871.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Murray, L., Stanley, C., Hooper, R., King, F., and Fiori-Cowley, A. (1996b). The role of infant factors in postnatal depression and mother–infant interactions. Dev. Med. Child Neurol. 38, 109–119. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-8749.1996.tb12082.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Nicol-Harper, R., Harvey, A. G., and Stein, A. (2007). Interactions between mothers and infants: impact of maternal anxiety. Infant Behav. Dev. 30, 161–167. doi: 10.1016/j.infbeh.2006.08.005

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

O’Connor, T. G., Winter, M. A., Hunn, J., Carnahan, J., Pressman, E. K., Glover, V., et al. (2013). Prenatal maternal anxiety predicts reduced adaptive immunity in infants. Brain Behav. Immun. 32, 21–28. doi: 10.1016/j.bbi.2013.02.002

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

O’Hara, M. W., and Swain, A. M. (1996). Rates and risk of postpartum depression—a meta-analysis. Int. Rev. Psychiatry 8, 37–54. doi: 10.3109/09540269609037816

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ohlsson, A., and Jacobs, S. E. (2013). NIDCAP: a systematic review and meta-analyses of randomized controlled trials. Pediatrics 131, e881–e893. doi: 10.1542/peds.2012-2121

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Padovani, F. H., Duarte, G., Martinez, F. E., and Linhares, M. B. (2011). Perceptions of breastfeeding in mothers of babies born preterm in comparison to mothers of full-term babies. Span. J. Psychol. 14, 884–898. doi: 10.5209/rev_SJOP.2011.v14.n2.35

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Padovani, F. H., Linhares, M. B., Carvalho, A. E., Duarte, G., and Martinez, F. E. (2004). Anxiety and depression symptoms assessment in pre-term neonates’ mothers during and after hospitalization in neonatal intensive care unit. Rev. Bras. Psiquiatr. 26, 251–254.

Google Scholar

Padovani, F. H., Linhares, M. B., Pinto, I. D., Duarte, G., and Martinez, F. E. (2008). Maternal concepts and expectations regarding a preterm infant. Span. J. Psychol. 11, 581–592.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

Pierrehumbert, B., Nicole, A., Muller-Nix, C., Forcada-Guex, M., and Ansermet, F. (2003). Parental post-traumatic reactions after premature birth: implications for sleeping and eating problems in the infant. Arch. Dis. Child Fetal Neonatal Ed. 88, F400–F404. doi: 10.1136/fn.88.5.F400

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Piteo, A. M., Yelland, L. N., and Makrides, M. (2012). Does maternal depression predict developmental outcome in 18 month old infants? Early Hum. Dev. 88, 651–655. doi: 10.1016/j.earlhumdev.2012.01.013

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Poehlmann, J., and Fiese, B. H. (2001). Parent-infant interaction as a mediator of the relation between neonatal risk status and 12-month cognitive development. Infant Behav. Dev. 24, 171–188. doi: 10.1016/S0163-6383(01)00073-X

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Poehlmann, J., Schwichtenberg, A. J., Shlafer, R. J., Hahn, E., Bianchi, J. P., and Warner, R. (2011). Emerging self-regulation in toddlers born preterm or low birth weight: differential susceptibility to parenting? Dev. Psychopathol. 23, 177–193. doi: 10.1017/S0954579410000726

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pratt, M., Apter-Levi, Y., Vakart, A., Feldman, M., Fishman, R., Feldman, T., et al. (2015). Maternal depression and child oxytocin response; moderation by maternal oxytocin and relational behavior. Depress Anxiety doi: 10.1002/da.22392 [Epub ahead of print]

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Reck, C., Struben, K., Backenstrass, M., Stefenelli, U., Reinig, K., Fuchs, T., et al. (2008). Prevalence, onset and comorbidity of postpartum anxiety and depressive disorders. Acta Psychiatr. Scand. 118, 459–468. doi: 10.1111/j.1600-0447.2008.01264.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Righetti-Veltema, M., Conne-Perreard, E., Bousquet, A., and Manzano, J. (2002). Postpartum depression and mother-infant relationship at 3 months old. J. Affect. Disord. 70, 291–306. doi: 10.1016/S0165-0327(01)00367-6

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Riordan, D., Appleby, L., and Faragher, B. (1999). Mother–infant interaction in post-partum women with schizophrenia and affective disorders. Psychol. Med. 29, 991–995. doi: 10.1017/S0033291798007727

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Robertson, E., Grace, S., Wallington, T., and Stewart, D. E. (2004). Antenatal risk factors for postpartum depression: a synthesis of recent literature. Gen. Hosp. Psychiatry 26, 289–295. doi: 10.1016/j.genhosppsych.2004.02.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rogers, C. E., Kidokoro, H., Wallendorf, M., and Inder, T. E. (2013). Identifying mothers of very preterm infants at-risk for postpartum depression and anxiety before discharge. J. Perinatol. 33, 171–176. doi: 10.1038/jp.2012.75

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ross, L., Gilbert Evans, S., Sellers, E., and Romach, M. (2003). Measurement issues in post-partum depression part 1: anxiety as a feature of post-partum depression. Arch. Womens Ment. Health 6, 51–57. doi: 10.1007/s00737-002-0155-1

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schermann-Eizirik, L., Hagekull, B., Bohlin, G., Persson, K., and Sedin, G. (1997). Interaction between mothers and infants born at risk during the first six months of corrected age. Acta Paediatr. 86, 864–872. doi: 10.1111/j.1651-2227.1997.tb08613.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schmücker, G., Brisch, K. H., Köhntop, B., Betzler, S., Österle, M., Pohlandt, F., et al. (2005). The influence of prematurity, maternal anxiety, and infants’ neurobiological risk on mother–infant interactions. Infant Mental Health J. 26, 423–441. doi: 10.1002/imhj.20066

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Seymour, M., Giallo, R., Cooklin, A., and Dunning, M. (2015). Maternal anxiety, risk factors and parenting in the first post-natal year. Child Care Health Dev. 41, 314–323. doi: 10.1111/cch.12178

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shah, P. E., Clements, M., and Poehlmann, J. (2011). Maternal resolution of grief after preterm birth: implications for infant attachment security. Pediatrics 127, 284–292. doi: 10.1542/peds.2010-1080

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Singer, L. T., Fulton, S., Davillier, M., Koshy, D., Salvator, A., and Baley, J. E. (2003). Effects of infant risk status and maternal psychological distress on maternal-infant interactions during the first year of life. J. Dev. Behav. Pediatr. 24, 233–241. doi: 10.1097/00004703-200308000-00003

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Singer, L. T., Salvator, A., Guo, S., Collin, M., Lilien, L., and Baley, J. (1999). Maternal psychological distress and parenting stress after the birth of a very low-birth-weight infant. JAMA 281, 799–805. doi: 10.1001/jama.281.9.799

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Stein, A., Craske, M. G., Lehtonen, A., Harvey, A., Savage-McGlynn, E., Davies, B., et al. (2012). Maternal cognitions and mother-infant interaction in postnatal depression and generalized anxiety disorder. J. Abnorm Psychol. 121, 795–809. doi: 10.1037/a0026847

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Swanson, L. M., Pickett, S. M., Flynn, H., and Armitage, R. (2011). Relationships among depression, anxiety, and insomnia symptoms in perinatal women seeking mental health treatment. J. Womens Health (Larchmt). 20, 553–558. doi: 10.1089/jwh.2010.2371

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Trombini, E., Surcinelli, P., Piccioni, A., Alessandroni, R., and Faldella, G. (2008). Environmental factors associated with stress in mothers of preterm newborns. Acta Paediatr. 97, 894–898. doi: 10.1111/j.1651-2227.2008.00849.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tronick, E. Z. (2007). The Neurobehavioral and Social-Emotional Development of Infants and Children. New York: W.W. Norton and Co.

Google Scholar

Vigod, S. N., Villegas, L., Dennis, C. L., and Ross, L. E. (2010). Prevalence and risk factors for postpartum depression among women with preterm and low-birth-weight infants: a systematic review. BJOG 117, 540–550. doi: 10.1111/j.1471-528.2009.02493.x

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Voegtline, K. M., Stifter, C. A., and The Family Project Investigators. (2010). Late-preterm birth, maternal symptomatology, and infant negativity. Infant Behav. Dev. 33, 545–554. doi: 10.1016/j.infbeh.2010.07.006

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Weber, A. M., Harrison, T. M., and Steward, D. K. (2012). Schore’s regulation theory: maternal–infant interaction in the NICU as a mechanism for reducing the effects of allostatic load on neurodevelopment in premature infants. Biol. Res. Nurs. 14, 375–386. doi: 10.1177/1099800412453760

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wijnroks, L. (1999). Maternal recollected anxiety and mother–infant interaction in preterm infants. Infant Ment. Health J. 20, 393–409.

PubMed Abstract | Google Scholar

World Health Organization [WHO]. (2012). Born Too Soon: The Global Action Report on Preterm Birth. Geneva: World Health Organization.

Google Scholar

Zarling, C. L., Hirsch, B. J., and Landry, S. (1988). Maternal social networks and mother-infant interactions in full-term and very low birthweight, preterm infants. Child Dev. 59, 178–185. doi: 10.2307/1130399

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zelkowitz, P., Bardin, C., and Papageorgiou, A. (2007). Anxiety affects the relationship between parents and their very low birth weight infants. Infant Ment. Health J. 28, 296–313. doi: 10.1002/imhj.20137

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zelkowitz, P., Papageorgiou, A., Bardin, C., and Wang, T. (2009). Persistent maternal anxiety affects the interaction between mothers and their very low birthweight children at 24 months. Early Hum. Dev. 85, 51–58. doi: 10.1016/j.earlhumdev.2008.06.010

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: maternal depression, maternal anxiety, birth weight, mother–infant interaction, preterm birth

Citation: Neri E, Agostini F, Salvatori P, Biasini A and Monti F (2015) Mother-preterm infant interactions at 3 months of corrected age: influence of maternal depression, anxiety and neonatal birth weight. Front. Psychol. 6:1234. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01234

Received: 25 May 2015; Accepted: 03 August 2015;
Published: 01 September 2015.

Edited by:

Silvia Salcuni, Università degli Studi di Padova, Italy

Reviewed by:

Chiara Nosarti, King’s College London, UK
Udita Iyengar, Baylor College of Medicine, USA

Copyright © 2015 Neri, Agostini, Salvatori, Biasini and Monti. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) or licensor are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Erica Neri, Department of Psychology, University of Bologna, Viale Europa 115, 47521 Cesena (FC), Bologna, Italy, erica.neri4@unibo.it

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.