Skip to main content

ORIGINAL RESEARCH article

Front. Psychol., 23 February 2022
Sec. Educational Psychology
This article is part of the Research Topic Cognitive and Affective Factors in Relation to Learning View all 14 articles

Relations Between Class Competition and Primary School Students’ Academic Achievement: Learning Anxiety and Learning Engagement as Mediators

  • Jing Hengyi School of Education, Hangzhou Normal University, Hangzhou, China

This study aimed to analyze the relations between class competition and primary school students’ academic achievement, considering the possible mediating roles of learning anxiety and learning engagement. Participants were 1,479 primary school students from four primary schools in Zhejiang, China. We analyzed participants’ scores for class competition, learning anxiety, and learning engagement and their last two final exam scores. Class competition did not directly predict academic achievement, but indirectly affected academic achievement through learning anxiety and learning engagement. There were three effect paths: (1) class competition negatively predicted academic achievement by increasing learning anxiety; (2) class competition positively predicted academic achievement by promoting learning engagement; and (3) class competition affected academic achievement through multiple mediating effects of learning anxiety and learning engagement. This study highlights the important roles of learning anxiety and learning engagement in class competition and academic achievement, which have theoretical and practical significance.

Introduction

Competition is a ubiquitous and age-old behavior pattern (Sun et al., 2015), and has become a prevalent problem across countries, cultures, and ethnic groups (Wong et al., 2006). For decades, psychological research has valued the study of competition (Sun et al., 2015). In the field of education, due to the limited high-quality educational resources, plus the driving force of students’ life goals, competition has also become a reality that students have to face (Posselt and Lipson, 2016). The primary school stage is the beginning of a person’s academic career, and it is also the basis of subsequent learning and future development. Parents often pay much attention to students’ academic achievement since children commence primary school life. This is especially reflected in students from some Asian countries such as China (Deng and Zou, 2015). As a result, primary school students may be unknowingly faced with academic competition. Children spend most of their time at school, which, after family, is the most important environment affecting child development (Volk et al., 2015). Because primary school students spend most of their school time in classes, academic competition among students is mainly reflected in class competition (Hu, 2018).

Academic achievement is a key indicator of students’ learning conditions, and provides an important basis for determining whether students can enter higher education. In addition, academic achievement is commonly used as a standard to judge the quality of school teaching. Therefore, students’ academic achievement receives substantial attention from students, parents and society. Various researchers have proposed that class competition is a critical factor in the education process (John et al., 2003; Ogbuehi and Fraser, 2007), and can have a negative impact on students’ academic achievement (Johnson and Johnson, 1994; Ames and Archer, 1988). The pressure perceived by students increases as class competition increases, and many children experience doubt regarding their learning abilities (Sommet et al., 2013). In addition, competitive instruction has been observed to stress the acquisition of low-level information rather than high-level ideas (Sullivan, 1980), which can have a negative impact on children’s academic achievement. However, previous studies have reported conflicting results. In some studies, individual competition was reported to have a positive impact on students’ academic achievement (Fisher, 1976; Tauer and Harackiewicz, 2004). He et al. (2011) reported a significant positive correlation between class competition and academic achievement. Competition can be divided into benign competition and vicious competition (Horney, 1937; Ryckman et al., 1990), and some researchers (Burguillo, 2010; Olitsky, 2011) have analyzed class competition from a dialectical perspective, and reported that benign competition stimulated students’ motivation, promoted individuals to learn from each other, and caused individuals to perform better, but vicious competition resulted in inaccurate cognition, children’s loss of correct judgment of their own value, and limit collaboration among students, hinder their progress, and ultimately have negative effects on their academic achievement. However, other researchers have reported that competition has no clear effect on students’ academic achievement (Pesout and Nietfeld, 2021) and that class competition cannot predict academic achievement in students (Pang, 2009). Thus, the relation between class competition and academic achievement is complex. Consequently, the effect of class competition on students’ academic achievement remains controversial, and its internal mechanisms require further investigation.

Mediating Role of Learning Anxiety

Learning anxiety may be an important factor in the negative relation between class competition and academic achievement. From a psychological perspective, class competition reflects students’ and teachers’ perceptions regarding the competitive atmosphere in the class or classroom environment (Fraser, 1986). Therefore, class competition is the type of psychological environment that students perceive. Learning anxiety refers to uneasy or unpleasant psychological reflection caused by internal conflict and is a specific state of tension in a student group (Woodman and Hardy, 2001). Learning anxiety is therefore a negative emotion produced by students in the process of learning (Pekrun, 2005).

Relative deprivation theory suggests that students tend to evaluate their academic level based on comparison with other students in the same class (Davis, 1966). Because of differences in learning ability among students, it is inevitable that some students win while other students fail in a competitive situation. Anxiety is triggered when students anticipate that they may fail (Pekrun, 2006). When the level of class competition is low, the pressure caused by students comparing themselves with each other is low, and students learn easily and happily. However, when class competition exceeds a certain level, students’ learning pressure increases and they may begin to doubt themselves, which results in negative emotions such as high anxiety (Johnson and Johnson, 1994). Therefore, it follows that class competition may increase students’ learning anxiety (Epstein and Harackiewicz, 1992; Quach et al., 2015).

Previous studies confirmed that learning anxiety can lead to poor academic achievement among students (Sariem et al., 2014; Chang and Beilock, 2016; D’Agostino et al., 2021). Emotions have been found to affect a wide range of cognitive processes, including attention, memory storage and retrieval, social judgment, decision making and cognitive problem solving (Clore and Huntsinger, 2009). Students in a state of learning anxiety will exhibit task-irrelevant thinking and a reduction in the cognitive resources available for task purposes, as well as becoming distracted during learning, causing low learning efficiency and a decline in academic achievement (Pekrun et al., 2017). Some researchers have examined the relation between learning anxiety and academic achievement in specific subjects, particularly mathematics. Previous studies reported that mathematics anxiety negatively predicted mathematics achievement (Foley et al., 2017; Wang et al., 2019; Tomasetto et al., 2021). Mathematics anxiety may impair performance by diverting cognitive resources from task-relevant purposes (i.e., a mathematics task) to task-irrelevant aspects (e.g., worry) (Ching, 2017; Ching et al., 2020). Further research revealed that, compared with boys, girls have less fun and more anxiety in mathematics learning (Pekrun and Stephens, 2010; Dowker et al., 2019).

Mediating Role of Learning Engagement

A possible reason for the positive correlation between class competition and academic achievement is the mediating role of learning engagement. Learning engagement is considered to be a positive, fulfilling, and work-related state of mind that is characterized by vigor, dedication, and absorption (Schaufeli et al., 2002b). Vigor is characterized by high levels of energy and mental resilience while working and by the willingness and ability to invest effort in one’s work. Dedication is characterized by a sense of significance, enthusiasm, inspiration, pride, and challenge. Absorption is characterized by being fully concentrated and happily engrossed in one’s work (Schaufeli et al., 2002a).

Class competition may promote learning engagement (Tauer and Harackiewicz, 2004; Reyes et al., 2012) and enhance students’ pressure and motivation in learning. Students will consciously invest in learning to ensure or improve their status among their classmates (Roth et al., 2009). Social learning theory (Bandura, 1978) posits that when many students in a class work hard, they serve as role models with which other students will compare themselves and imitate, promoting their engagement in learning activities (Hasan and Bagde, 2013). Learning engagement is positively correlated with academic achievement (Schaufeli et al., 2002a; Chang and Beilock, 2016). When the level of learning engagement is high, students will actively invest more time and energy in learning activities, and they will also have positive emotional experiences in learning (Schaufeli and Bakker, 2004; Liu et al., 2020), which plays an important role in promoting students’ academic achievement (Pietarinen et al., 2014; Bayoumy and Alsayed, 2021).

Relation Between Learning Anxiety and Learning Engagement

Learning anxiety, as a negative emotion in an academic context, may have an adverse effect on students’ learning engagement. Studies have shown that students’ emotions are critical to their willingness to learn and their volitional control over the learning process (Pekrun, 2005). Positive emotions can stimulate learners’ motivation and promote high engagement in learning (Frenzel et al., 2007; Wara et al., 2018), whereas negative emotions (e.g., anxiety) reduce learning efficiency and may even cause serious learning delays (Macher et al., 2012). In addition, learning anxiety means students focus more on threats and failures, which restricts their cognition and activity maps (Fredrickson, 2001) and occupies their limited cognitive resources (Pekrun and Stephens, 2010). In turn, this affects students’ processing of current learning tasks (Owens et al., 2012). When students fail to meet the cognitive requirements of academic activities, they gradually develop low learning engagement (Zhen et al., 2017).

Present Study and Hypotheses

Previous studies reported inconsistent findings about the relation between class competition and student academic achievement. We speculated that this may be related to the dual effects of class competition on students. On the one hand, class competition may have a negative effect on students’ academic achievement by increasing their learning anxiety; on the other hand, class competition may have a positive effect on their academic achievement by promoting learning engagement. However, few studies have explored this inconsistency and its underlying mechanisms, particularly in class competition among primary school students. Primary school students begin to face competition in the early stages of their learning careers, and investigating the underlying mechanisms is important for understanding the mode of action of class competition, promoting primary school students’ physical and mental health, and improving teaching outcomes. Therefore, the current study focused on primary school students to investigate the relation between class competition and academic achievement, as well as the mediating role of learning anxiety and learning engagement. Based on available research results, this study proposed the following hypotheses and a conceptual model (Figure 1).

Hypothesis 1 (H1): Class competition will have a direct effect on students’ academic achievement.

Hypothesis 2 (H2): Learning anxiety will play a mediating role between class competition and students’ academic achievement.

Hypothesis 3 (H3): Learning engagement will play a mediating role between class competition and students’ academic achievement.

Hypothesis 4 (H4): Learning anxiety and learning engagement will play multiple mediating roles between class competition and students’ academic achievement.

FIGURE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 1. Hypothesis model of the relation between class competition and academic achievement.

Materials and Methods

Participants

This survey was conducted in Hangzhou, Zhejiang Province, in Eastern China. We chose four primary schools (two from the city and two from rural areas) to reflect the overall situation of local schools. Considering the limitations of students’ psychological level and text understanding ability in grades one and two of primary school, we surveyed 40 classes from grade three to grade six in the four selected schools (10 classes for each grade). In total, 1,645 questionnaires were collected. After summarizing the questionnaire data and students’ exam results, and excluding omissions, overfilling, and missing results, data for 1,479 students were included in the analysis (effective rate 89.91%). Participating students were aged 8–13 years (M = 10.592 years; standard deviation = 1.204 years), 46.1% of them were girls, and 53.9% were boys.

Measuring Instruments

Class Competition

Class competition is often regarded as a dimension of class environment. The most influential class environmental measurement scale has mainly been used to examine classes in schools in western countries (Fraser et al., 1982; Moos and Trickett, 1987). In the Chinese cultural context, classes in Chinese schools are very different to those in western countries. To ensure cultural applicability, this study chose the “My Class” scale compiled by Chinese researchers (Jiang, 2004). Previous studies have confirmed that this scale has good reliability and validity among Chinese students (Zhang et al., 2006). The scale consists of five dimensions, including teacher-student relationships, classmate relationships, order and discipline, class competition, and learning burden, with a total of 38 items. Among these dimensions, the class competition dimension consists of seven items (e.g., “Everyone is afraid of falling behind in learning” and “In order not to be surpassed by others, no one dares to relax in learning”). Responses are on a 5-point Likert-type scale from 1 (completely disagree) to 5 (completely agree), and the average score of the seven items was used as a student’s perceived class competition score. Higher scores indicate greater competition. The Cronbach’s alpha value for the class competition scale in this study was 0.682.

Learning Anxiety

The learning anxiety scale used in this study was derived from the Mental Health Diagnostic Test compiled by Zhou (1991), which has been widely used in thousands of primary and secondary schools in more than 20 provinces and cities in China, with high reliability and validity (Ding and Li, 2017; Hu et al., 2019). The learning anxiety subscale of the test scale has a total of 15 items (e.g., “When the teacher asks questions to the class, I feel uneasy about asking myself,” “I have dreamed about being reprimanded by parents or teachers because of my poor grades”). Responses were provided on a 5-point Likert-type scale from 1 (completely disagree) to 5 (completely agree), and the average score of the items was used as a student’s perceived learning anxiety score. Higher scores indicate greater learning anxiety. The Cronbach’s alpha value for the learning anxiety scale in this study was 0.887.

Learning Engagement

The Chinese version of the learning engagement scale in this study was translated and revised by Fang et al. (2008). The original scale was developed by Schaufeli et al. (2002a,b). The Chinese version of the scale has been reported to have good reliability and validity (Fang et al., 2008; Shi et al., 2013). The scale includes three dimensions and has a total of 17 items. Items 1–6 belong to the vitality dimension, items 7–11 to the dedication dimension, and items 12–17 to the concentration dimension. Examples of items are: “Even if my study is not smooth, I am not discouraged and can persevere” and “When I study, I forget everything around me.” Responses are provided on a 5-point Likert-type scale from 1 (completely disagree) to 5 (completely agree), and the average score of the 17 items was used as a student’s perceived learning engagement score. Higher scores indicate greater learning engagement. In this study, the three dimensions and the total scale had good reliability (vitality: Cronbach’s α = 0.849; dedication: Cronbach’s α = 0.861; concentration: Cronbach’s α = 0.881; total scale: Cronbach’s α = 0.938). The structural validity indicators were: χ2 (111) = 466.467, comparative fit index (CFI) = 0.975, Tucker–Lewis index (TLI) = 0.970, root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA) with 90% confidence interval (CI) = 0.047 (0.042–0.051), and standardized root mean squared residual (SRMR) = 0.024.

Academic Achievement

The indicators of academic achievement used in this study were based on four subjects: Chinese, mathematics, English, and science (full score for each course: 100 points). These four subjects are recognized as the main subjects in primary school in China and usually serve as an important reference to reflect the academic achievement of primary school students. In the current study, class competition refers to the overall feelings of students about the competitive atmosphere of the class, and not specifically to the feelings of competition in a certain subject. Therefore, the academic achievement of the students in this study also refers to the overall learning situation of the students, not that of a certain subject, so students’ academic achievement is calculated based on the average scores of the four subjects of Chinese, mathematics, English, and science. To ensure that the indicators of academic achievement were robust, we adopted the results of the last two final exams.

Procedures

This study was first approved by the Ethics Committee of Hangzhou Normal University. We then applied to the administrative department for each selected school and obtained permission to conduct the study. Written informed consent was obtained from students’ parents before the investigation. After receiving consent, paper-and-pencil questionnaires were administered to students in class by trained graduate students who were majoring in psychology. It took approximately 20 min to complete the questionnaires. Students’ academic achievement data were provided by teachers at each school.

Data Analysis

After the survey data were recovered, SPSS version 24.0 was used to test the valid sample data for missing completely at random (MCAR) data. The MCAR test results were: χ2 = 1254.895, df = 1206, p = 0.160 > 0.05, and the missing value was completely random. The Harman’s single-factor test was used to control the common method biases, and the first factor without rotation explained 24.44% of the variance, which was less than the critical value of 40%. Therefore, there were no obvious common method biases in this study. Descriptive statistics and correlations were calculated using SPSS version 24.0. A structural equation model and bias-corrected bootstrap test were then performed using Mplus version 7.0 to verify the mediating role of learning anxiety and learning engagement between class competition and academic achievement as well as to analyze the effect path.

Results

Descriptive Statistics and Correlation Analysis

The descriptive statistics and Pearson’s correlations among the variables are presented in Table 1. The average class competition score for primary school students was 3.16 points (range 1–5), indicating that the class competition atmosphere of primary school students was in a moderate level. In terms of relevance, class competition was significantly positively correlated with learning anxiety and learning engagement, learning engagement was significantly positively correlated with academic achievement, and learning anxiety was significantly negatively related to learning engagement and academic achievement. There was no significant correlation between class competition and academic achievement.

TABLE 1
www.frontiersin.org

Table 1. Descriptive statistics and correlation analysis for each variable.

Further analysis revealed that in addition to learning engagement, the three variables of class competition, learning anxiety, and academic achievement exhibited significant differences by gender and urban–rural dimensions. Regarding gender, the average class competition score of boys was significantly higher than that of girls, and the average learning anxiety and academic achievement scores of boys were significantly lower than those of girls. Regarding the urban–rural dimension, the average class competition and learning anxiety scores of urban students were lower than those of rural students, but the average academic achievement score of urban students was significantly higher than that of rural students.

Mediating Effect Analysis of Learning Anxiety and Learning Engagement

As mentioned earlier, the results revealed no significant correlation between class competition and academic achievement. This finding raises the question of whether or not class competition has an effect on academic achievement. Some methodologists believe that whether independent variables and dependent variables are significantly correlated does not constitute a prerequisite for the existence of mediating effects (Collins et al., 1998; Shrout and Bolger, 2002; Preacher et al., 2007). A simulation study by Rucker et al. (2011) revealed that in nearly half of all simulation conditions, the relation between the independent variable and the dependent variable was not significant, but there was a significant mediating effect. Therefore, it is necessary to further analyze mediating effects despite the lack of a significant correlation between students’ perceived class competition and their academic achievement.

The starting theoretical model (Figure 1) showed a good fit to the empirical data: χ2(6) = 29.135, CFI = 0.992, TLI = 0.981, RMSEA (90% CI) = 0.051 (0.033–0.070), and SRMR = 0.014. The results indicated that the effect of class competition on academic achievement was not significant, but learning anxiety and learning engagement fully mediated the relation between class competition and academic achievement (Figure 2).

FIGURE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Figure 2. Mediation effect model of class competition on academic achievement. ***p < 0.001.

Finally, we used a bias-corrected bootstrap method to test whether the mediating effects described above were significant. In general, if the 95% CI does not include 0, the mediating effect is considered to be significant at the 0.05 level (Preacher and Hayes, 2008). The results revealed that the mediating effects of learning anxiety and learning engagement were significant, and class competition affected academic achievement through the three possible paths: class competition had a negative effect on academic achievement through increasing learning anxiety, a positive effect on academic achievement through promoting learning engagement, and a chain effect on academic achievement through learning anxiety via learning engagement (Table 2).

TABLE 2
www.frontiersin.org

Table 2. Bias-corrected bootstrap test on mediating effects.

Discussion

The Current Situation of Main Variables

The results in Table 1 show that the average class competition score of primary school students in the current study was 3.16 points (score range: 1–5), indicating that primary school classes had a clear competitive atmosphere. In China, many parents expect their children to be among the best in class rankings (Deng and Zou, 2015). Students with advantages in class competition are often favored by teachers and tend to have greater influence in the student group, while students with disadvantages in class competition have difficulty in becoming influential figures in the class (Warren et al., 2005). According to the relative deprivation theory (Davis, 1966), individuals’ sense of relative deprivation arises from comparison with others, and their sense of competition will be stimulated when perceiving inferior position. Given the combined effects of pressure from parents, teachers and other factors, primary school students are increasingly likely to experience Competition (Liu et al., 2020).

Regarding the gender differences in class competition, learning anxiety, and academic achievement among primary school students revealed in the current results, we believe that, in addition to boys being more sensitive to the competitive atmosphere (Volk et al., 2006), the high familial expectations in traditional Chinese culture concerning the academic achievement and future careers of male children in particular may also increase boys’ sense of competitive pressure. This is consistent with the conclusion of a previous study that boys’ perception of the class competition atmosphere is higher than that of girls (Li et al., 2019). Emotion-related characteristics of girls (Pekrun and Stephens, 2010) may explain why girls’ learning anxiety was higher than that of boys in the current results. Boys have been reported to exhibit significantly lower levels of school adaptation and lower learning consciousness compared with girls (Zhang et al., 2014; Liu et al., 2020), which may explain why the academic achievement of girls in primary school is higher than that of boys.

The differences between urban and rural students in class competition, learning anxiety and academic achievement may be related to the imbalance between urban and rural education in China. Compared with urban students, rural students have fewer employment channels, and changing their employment trajectory through studying hard is often the only option for rural children’s future development. As Pettigrew et al. (2008) pointed out, individuals’ socioeconomic status significantly affects their sense of relative deprivation, and those with lower social status and less political influence experience stronger sense of relative deprivation, and strive to change the status quo. Compared with urban students, rural students have a stronger sense of relative deprivation, which further intensifies competition in class and increases their learning anxiety. However, because the quality of teachers and teaching facilities in rural schools lags behind that in urban areas, the education received by rural students is significantly worse than that of urban students (Sang et al., 2009). This reality may be the main reason why rural students’ academic achievement is not as good as that of urban students.

Direct Effect of Class Competition on Academic Achievement

This study investigated the mechanism of the effect of class competition on academic achievement among primary school students by establishing a multiple mediating model for the first time. The results revealed that class competition had no significant direct effect on students’ academic achievement, which failed to support H1. A previous study reported that class competition had different effects on students with different levels of academic achievement, such that students with good grades felt the pressure brought by competition, leading them to study harder and achieve better grades, whereas students with poor grades lacked self-confidence, causing their learning enthusiasm and motivation to be further decreased by repeated failures in class competition, leading to worse grades (Zimmerman, 2003). The polarization of academic achievement caused by class competition counteracts the effect of class competition on academic achievement to some degree, which may explain the absence of a significant correlation between these two variables in the current study.

The results of this study were inconsistent with the findings of many previous studies (Ames and Archer, 1988; Sommet et al., 2013). We believe that these discrepancies may have arisen because most previous research focused on students from junior high schools, senior high schools, and universities, whereas we investigated primary school students. Students’ perception of the classroom psychological environment is reported to vary across education stages (Zhang et al., 2014), and their academic achievement is the result of interactions among many factors, including their family background and school environment (Feng et al., 2019). Therefore, it is understandable that the relation between class competition and academic achievement in this study differed from that reported in previous studies.

Mediating Roles of Learning Anxiety and Learning Engagement

The current findings indicate that class competition can be a “double-edged sword” for academic achievement through the intermediating variables of learning anxiety and learning engagement. These findings supported H2 and H3. Class competition negatively affected students’ academic achievement by increasing their learning anxiety, which was consistent with conclusions drawn by Sariem et al. (2014) and D’Agostino et al. (2021). According to Lewin’s field theory (Lewin, 1936), individuals’ mental activity and behaviors are closely related with the environment they live. For students who daily live in a competitive atmosphere, the pressure of comparison and potential failures leads them to feel uneasy and anxious about learning outcomes. Anxiety is experienced particularly strongly when control over the outcome of a competition seems impossible (Pekrun, 2006). The conclusion that class competition can negatively affect students’ academic achievement by increasing their learning anxiety may serve as a reminder for teachers and parents to pay attention to the learning-related emotions of primary school students. Reducing class competition and alleviating students’ learning anxiety via home–school cooperation may enable students to study in a relaxed and happy state, thereby improving their learning outcomes.

Conversely, the current results also revealed that class competition positively affected students’ academic achievement by promoting their learning engagement, which was consistent with the results of several previous studies (Roth et al., 2009; Reyes et al., 2012; Bayoumy and Alsayed, 2021). The pressure of class competition means that students who want to satisfy their needs for academic achievement or class status have to increase their learning engagement by using certain strategies (e.g., listening carefully in class and reviewing spontaneously after class) to achieve better grades. One study found no significant correlation between learning engagement and academic achievement (Shernoff, 2010). We propose that this finding may have arisen because students with good grades master fast learning skills, thereby reducing their learning time. In contrast, students with poor grades do not have a good foundation for those skills, and it remains difficult for them to achieve good grades even if they struggle and invest more time (Phuntsho and Dhendup, 2020). This highlights the need for teachers to pay attention to students’ learning foundations, learning style, and learning habits, and to guide students to adopt a learning method that suits their specific characteristics. Furthermore, Schaufeli et al. (2002b) divided learning engagement into three dimensions (vitality, dedication, and concentration). In the current study, we did not perform comprehensive analyses of the specific roles of these three dimensions in the relation between class competition and academic achievement. This question should be addressed in future research.

The current study revealed that class competition negatively affected students’ academic achievement through multiple mediating effects of learning anxiety and learning engagement. This finding supported H4 and also revealed another possible path by which class competition indirectly affected academic achievement (i.e., class competition positively affected students’ learning anxiety, learning anxiety negatively affected learning engagement, and learning engagement positively predicted academic achievement). Therefore, the effects of class competition on academic achievement may be determined by learning anxiety and learning engagement. This conclusion further demonstrated the close relation between learning anxiety and learning engagement. As Kindermann (1993) noted, learning engagement is essentially students’ emotional engagement in learning activities. The control value theory proposed by Pekrun (2006) also details this process: emotions can affect students’ academic achievement by influencing their motivation and effort, their use of learning strategies and self-regulation, and the availability of cognitive resources needed for learning and performance. Some researchers in the field of psychology verified the impact of learning anxiety on personal values and individual behavioral input through empirical research and follow-up surveys (Gao et al., 2021). Class competition can affect academic achievement through multiple mediating effects of learning anxiety and learning engagement, which reflects the chain relation among the four variables and demonstrates another way in which learning anxiety and learning engagement play a mediating role between class competition and academic achievement.

Limitations and Contributions

Several limitations of the current study should be acknowledged. First, the sample was selected from four primary schools in Hangzhou, Zhejiang Province, China. However, the educational and cultural contexts in China differ from those in other countries, and further research should examine a wider range of samples to include students in different cultural contexts. Second, this study mainly focused on individual students’ perceptions of the competitive atmosphere in class. However, classes with different levels of competition may have different effects on students. Therefore, in future studies, multilevel modeling should be adopted to model class-level effects. Third, this study focused on the relation between students’ overall feelings about class competition and students’ learning anxiety, learning engagement and academic achievement, which are not specific to individual subjects. However, the relations between the four variables may vary between different subjects. Thus, relevant research for specific disciplines should be carried out in future research. Fourth, although the reliability of the class competition scale in this study was acceptable, it is still low, and the survey process should be further improved. In addition, because this was a cross-sectional study, the causal direction of the hypothesized effects should be confirmed in longitudinal research.

Despite these limitations, the current study has important theoretical implications and practical value. From a theoretical perspective, this study expanded the research on competition among primary school students and also provides a theoretical explanation for the inconsistent effects of class competition on academic achievement. From a practical perspective, the existence of class competition is inevitable. The results of this study can guide teachers in taking effective measures to enable students to strengthen their learning engagement levels actively in an atmosphere of benign competition. At the same time, our findings suggest that teachers should pay close attention to students’ psychological states and create cooperative classroom learning environments at an appropriate stage to give students appropriate encouragement, thereby reducing the negative effects of class competition and avoiding learning anxiety (Okebukola, 1986). In addition, teachers should guide students to improve their learning methods and form good learning habits, thereby improving learning engagement and promoting a high level of academic achievement.

Conclusion

The current study focused on primary school students to investigate the relation between class competition and academic achievement, as well as the mediating roles of learning anxiety and learning engagement. The following three findings were highlighted. First, the class competition atmosphere of primary school students we investigated was in a moderate level. Except learning engagement, class competition, learning anxiety, and academic achievement exhibited significant differences in gender and urban–rural residence. Second, class competition did not have a direct relation with academic achievement. Third, class competition was significantly negatively correlated with academic achievement via learning anxiety, whereas was positively correlated with academic achievement via learning engagement. Besides, class competition was negatively associated with academic achievement via the multiple mediating roles of learning anxiety and learning engagement.

Data Availability Statement

The raw data supporting the conclusions of this article will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation.

Ethics Statement

The studies involving human participants were reviewed and approved by the Academic Ethics Committee of Jing Hengyi School of Education, Hangzhou Normal University. Written informed consent to participate in this study was provided by the participants’ legal guardian/next of kin.

Author Contributions

GL and RZ developed the research project, with the contribution of XW and ZL. ZL prepared the data set. XW carried out the data analysis. ZL reviewed the literature. GL and RZ reviewed and edited the manuscript. All authors contributed to the article and approved the submitted version.

Funding

Cultivation Project for Provincial Advantageous and Characteristic Discipline of Hangzhou Normal University (20JYXK011) and Research Project for 2019 Degree and Postgraduate Education Reform of Hangzhou Normal University (YJG201914).

Conflict of Interest

The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.

Publisher’s Note

All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.

Acknowledgments

We thank the students and teachers who devoted time and support to this study.

References

Ames, C., and Archer, J. (1988). Achievement goals in the classroom: students’ learning strategies and motivation processes. J. Educ. Psychol. 80, 260–267. doi: 10.1037/0022-0663.80.3.260

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bandura, A. (1978). The self system in reciprocal determinism. Am. Psychol. 33, 344–358. doi: 10.1037/0003-066X.33.4.344

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Bayoumy, H., and Alsayed, S. (2021). Investigating relationship of perceived learning engagement, motivation, and academic performance among nursing students: a multisite study. Adv. Med. Educ. Pract. 12, 351–369. doi: 10.2147/AMEP.S272745

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Burguillo, J. C. (2010). Using game-theory and competition-based learning to stimulate student motivation and performance. Comput. Educ. 55, 566–575. doi: 10.1016/j.compedu.2010.02.018

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Chang, H., and Beilock, S. L. (2016). The math anxiety-math performance link and its relation to individual and environmental factors: a review of current behavioral and psychophysiological research. Curr. Opin. Behav. Sci. 10, 33–38. doi: 10.1016/j.cobeha.2016.04.011

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ching, B. H. (2017). Mathematics anxiety and working memory: longitudinal associations with mathematical performance in Chinese children. Contemp. Educ. Psychol. 51, 99–113. doi: 10.1016/j.cedpsych.2017.06.006

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ching, B. H., Kong, K. H. C., Wu, H. X., and Chen, T. T. (2020). Examining the reciprocal relations of mathematics anxiety to quantitative reasoning and number knowledge in Chinese children. Contemp. Educ. Psychol. 63:101919. doi: 10.1016/j.cedpsych.2020.101919

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Clore, G. L., and Huntsinger, J. R. (2009). How the object of affect guides its impact. Emot. Rev. 1, 39–54. doi: 10.1177/1754073908097185

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Collins, L. M., Graham, J. W., and Flaherty, B. P. (1998). An alternative framework for defining mediation. Multivar. Behav. Res. 33, 295–312. doi: 10.1207/s15327906mbr3302_5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

D’Agostino, A., Spagnolo, F. S., and Salvati, N. (2021). Studying the relationship between anxiety and school achievement: evidence from PISA data. Stat. Methods Appl. doi: 10.1007/s10260-021-00563-9

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Davis, J. A. (1966). The campus as a frog pond: an application of the theory of relative deprivation to career decisions of college men. Am. J. Sociol. 72, 17–31. doi: 10.2307/2775756

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Deng, Y. C., and Zou, J. Q. (2015). From hierarchy and hierarchical consciousness to achievement anxiety and academic competition. J. Hubei Eng. Univ. 35, 76–79.

Google Scholar

Ding, Y., and Li, X. P. (2017). Mental health of high-grade pupils in poverty-stricken areas of Shanxi Province, China. J. Health Psychol. 25, 140–144. doi: 10.13342/j.cnki.cjhp.2017.01.033

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Dowker, A., Cheriton, O., Horton, R., and Mark, W. (2019). Relationships between attitudes and performance in young children’s mathematics. Educ. Stud. Math. 100, 211–230. doi: 10.1007/s10649-019-9880-5

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Epstein, J. A., and Harackiewicz, J. M. (1992). Winning is not enough: the effects of competition and achievement orientation on intrinsic interest. Pers. Soc. Psychol. Bull. 18, 128–138. doi: 10.1177/0146167292182003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fang, L. T., Shi, K., and Zhang, F. H. (2008). Research on reliability and validity of Utrecht work engagement scale-student. Chin. J. Clin. Psychol. 16, 618–620. doi: 10.16128/j.cnki.1005-3611.2008.06.023

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Feng, X., Xie, K., Gong, S., Gao, L., and Cao, Y. (2019). Effects of parental autonomy support and teacher support on middle school students’ homework effort: homework autonomous motivation as mediator. Front. Psychol. 10:612. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.00612

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fisher, J. E. (1976). Competition and gaming: an experimental study. Simulat. Gaming 7, 321–328. doi: 10.1177/003755007673007

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Foley, A. E., Herts, J. B., Borgonovi, F., Guerriero, S., Levine, S. C., and Beilock, S. L. (2017). The math anxiety-performance link: a global phenomenon. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 26, 52–58. doi: 10.1177/0963721416672463

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Fraser, B. J. (1986). Classroom Environment. London: Croom Helm Ltd.

Google Scholar

Fraser, B. J., Anderson, G. J., and Walberg, H. J. (1982). Assessment of Learning Environments: Manual for Learning Environment Inventory (LEI) and My Class Inventory(MCI). Perth, WA: Western Australia Institute of Technology.

Google Scholar

Fredrickson, B. L. (2001). The role of positive emotions in positive psychology: the broaden-and-build theory of positive emotions. Am. Psychol. 56, 218–226. doi: 10.1037/0003-066x.56.3.218

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Frenzel, A. C., Pekrun, R., and Goetz, T. (2007). Perceived learning environment and students’ emotional experiences: a multilevel analysis of mathematics classrooms. Learn. Instr. 17, 478–493. doi: 10.1016/j.learninstruc.2007.09.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Gao, L. J., Huang, F. L., Wu, X. X., Liao, F. Q., and Chen, X. B. (2021). Characteristics and relationship of attachment relationships and coping styles of adolescents with emotional behavior problems. Psychol. Mon. 16, 12–13+8. doi: 10.19738/j.cnki.psy.2021.16.005

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hasan, S., and Bagde, S. (2013). The mechanics of social capital and academic performance in an Indian college. Am. Sociol. Rev. 78, 1009–1032. doi: 10.1177/0003122413505198

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

He, L. X., Zhang, Q., and Wang, C. K. (2011). The relationship between competition-cooperation and academic achievement: a mediator role of academic self-concept. Psychol. Explor. 31, 172–175.

Google Scholar

Horney, K. (1937). The Neurotic Personality of Our Time. New York, NY: W.W. Norton & Co.

Google Scholar

Hu, Q. J., Liang, W. M., Zhang, Y. L., Zhang, X. X., Chen, M., Xing, L., et al. (2019). Mental health condition of primary school students in Dongguan, China. J. Health Psychol. 27, 923–928. doi: 10.13342/j.cnki.cjhp.2019.06.014

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Hu, Z. P. (2018). Kernel collaborative representation classification based on adaptive dictionary learning. Int. J. Intell. Inform. Syst. 7:15. doi: 10.11648/j.ijiis.20180702.11

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Jiang, G. R. (2004). Class environment in the Chinese school system: structure and measurement. Psychol. Sci. 27, 839–843. doi: 10.16719/j.cnki.1671-6981.2004.04.018

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

John, P., Buerck, T. M., and Elliott, P. (2003). Learning environment and learning styles: nontraditional student environment and success in an internet-based versus a lecture-based computer science course. Learn. Environ. Res. 6, 137–155. doi: 10.1023/A:1024939002433

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Johnson, D. W., and Johnson, R. T. (1994). Learning Together and Alone. Cooperative, Competitive, and Individualistic Learning, 4th Edn. Boston, MA: Allyn and Bacon.

Google Scholar

Kindermann, T. A. (1993). Natural peer groups as contexts for individual development: the case of children’s motivation in school. Dev. Psychol. 29, 970–977. doi: 10.1037/0012-1649.29.6.970

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Lewin, K. (1936). Principles of Topological Psychology. New York, NY: Mc Graw-Hill.

Google Scholar

Li, B. L., Gao, T., Deng, L. Y., Chen, J. J., and Ni, H. (2019). The mediation effect of core self-evaluation between class environment and academic stress among middle school students —the moderation effect of gender. Psycholol. Dev. Educ. 35, 192–202. doi: 10.16187/j.cnki.issn1001-4918.2019.02.08

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Liu, H. R., Yao, M. L., and Li, J. (2020). Chinese adolescents’ achievement goal profiles and their relation to academic burnout, learning engagement, and test anxiety. Learn. Indiv. Differ. 83–84:101945. doi: 10.1016/j.lindif.2020.101945

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Macher, D., Paechter, M., Papousek, I., and Ruggeri, K. (2012). Statistics anxiety, trait anxiety, learning behavior, and academic performance. Eur. J. Psychol. Educ. 27, 483–498. doi: 10.1007/s10212-011-0090-5

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Moos, R. H., and Trickett, E. J. (1987). Classroom Environment Scale Manual, 2nd Edn. Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press.

Google Scholar

Ogbuehi, P. I., and Fraser, B. J. (2007). Learning environment, attitudes and conceptual development associated with innovative strategies in middle-school mathematics. Learn. Environ. Res. 10, 101–114. doi: 10.1007/s10984-007-9026-z

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Okebukola, P. A. (1986). Reducing anxiety in science classes: an experiment involving some models of class interaction. Educ. Res. 28, 146–149. doi: 10.1080/0013188860280211

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Olitsky, S. (2011). The role of fictive kinship relationships in mediating classroom competition and supporting reciprocal mentoring. Cult. Stud. Sci. Educ. 6, 883–894. doi: 10.1007/s11422-011-9363-1

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Owens, M., Stevenson, J., Hadwin, J. A., and Norgate, R. (2012). Anxiety and depression in academic performance: an exploration of the mediating factors of worry and working memory. Sch. Psychol. Int. 33, 433–449. doi: 10.1177/0143034311427433

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pang, H. B. (2009). An investigation on the correlation between junior middle school students’ class environment and academic achievement. Psychol. Sci. 32, 742–744. doi: 10.16719/j.cnki.1671-6981.2009.03.065

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pekrun, R. (2005). Progress and open problems in educational emotion research. Learn. Instr. 15, 497–506. doi: 10.1016/j.learninstruc.2005.07.014

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pekrun, R. (2006). The control-value theory of achievement emotions: assumptions, corollaries, and implications for educational research and practice. Educ. Psychol. Rev. 18, 315–341. doi: 10.1007/s10648-006-9029-9

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pekrun, R., Lichtenfeld, S., Marsh, H. W., Murayama, K., and Goetz, T. (2017). Achievement emotions and academic performance: longitudinal models of reciprocal effects. Child Dev. 88, 1653–1670. doi: 10.1111/cdev.12704

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pekrun, R., and Stephens, E. J. (2010). Achievement emotions: a control-value approach. Soc. Pers. Psychol. Compass 4, 238–255. doi: 10.1111/j.1751-9004.2010.00259.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pesout, O., and Nietfeld, J. (2021). The impact of cooperation and competition on metacognitive monitoring in classroom context. J. Exp. Educ. 89, 237–258. doi: 10.1080/00220973.2020.1751577

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pettigrew, T. F., Christ, O., Wagner, U., Meertens, R. W., van Dick, R., and Zick, A. (2008). Relative deprivation and intergroup prejudice. J. Soc. Issues 64, 385–401. doi: 10.1111/j.1540-4560.2008.00567.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Phuntsho, U., and Dhendup, R. (2020). The relationship between school climate, student engagement, and academic achievement in higher secondary school. Bhutan J. Res. Dev. Autumn 9, 98–131. doi: 10.13140/RG.2.2.28798.00327/2

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Pietarinen, J., Soini, T., and Pyhältö, K. (2014). Students’ emotional and cognitive engagement as the determinants of well-being and achievement in school. Int. J. Educ. Res. 67, 40–51. doi: 10.1016/j.ijer.2014.05.001

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Posselt, J. R., and Lipson, S. K. (2016). Competition, anxiety, and depression in the college classroom: variations by student identity and field of study. J. Coll. Stud. Dev. 57, 973–989. doi: 10.1353/csd.2016.0094

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Preacher, K. J., and Hayes, A. F. (2008). Asymptotic and resampling strategies for assessing and comparing indirect effects in multiple mediator models. Behav. Res. Methods 40, 879–891. doi: 10.3758/BRM.40.3.879

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Preacher, K. J., Rucker, D. D., and Hayes, A. F. (2007). Addressing moderated mediation hypotheses: theory, methods, and prescriptions. Multivar. Behav. Res. 42, 185–227. doi: 10.1080/00273170701341316

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Quach, A. S., Epstein, N. B., Riley, P. J., Falconier, M. K., and Fang, X. (2015). Effects of parental warmth and academic pressure on anxiety and depression symptoms in Chinese adolescents. J. Child Fam. Stud. 24, 106–116. doi: 10.1007/s10826-013-9818-y

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Reyes, M. R., Bracket, M. A., River, S. E., White, M., and Salovey, P. (2012). Classroom emotional climate, student engagement, and academic achievement. J. Educ. Psychol. 104, 700–712. doi: 10.1037/a0027268

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Roth, G., Assor, A., Niemiec, C. P., Ryan, R. M., and Deci, E. L. (2009). The emotional and academic consequences of parental conditional regard: comparing conditional positive regard, conditional negative regard, and autonomy support as parenting practices. Dev. Psychol. 45, 1119–1142. doi: 10.1037/a0015272

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Rucker, D. D., Preacher, K. J., Tormala, Z. L., and Petty, R. E. (2011). Mediation analysis in social psychology: current practices and new recommendations. Soc. Pers. Psychol. Compass 5, 359–371. doi: 10.1111/j.1751-9004.2011.00355.x

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Ryckman, R. M., Hammer, M., Kaczor, L. M., and Gold, J. A. (1990). Construction of a hypercompetitive attitude scale. J. Pers. Assess. 55, 630–639. doi: 10.1080/00223891.1990.9674097

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sang, G. Y., Valcke, M., van Braak, J., and Tondeur, J. (2009). Investigating teachers’ educational beliefs in Chinese primary schools: socioeconomic and geographical perspectives. Asia Pac. J. Teach. Educ. 37, 363–377. doi: 10.1080/13598660903250399

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sariem, C. N., Fwangshak, F. D., Shalkur, D., and Adeniyi, M. A. (2014). Factors affecting academic performance of pharmacy students in the University of Jos, Nigeria. J. Pharm. Bioresour. 11, 85–92. doi: 10.4314/jpb.v11i2.8

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schaufeli, W. B., and Bakker, A. B. (2004). Job demands, job resources, and their relationship with burnout and engagement: a multi-sample study. J. Organ. Behav. 25, 293–315. doi: 10.1002/job.248

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schaufeli, W. B., Salanova, M., González-Romá, V., and Bakker, A. B. (2002b). The measurement of engagement and burnout: a two sample confirmatory factor analytic approach. J. Happiness Stud. 3, 71–92. doi: 10.1023/A:1015630930326

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Schaufeli, W. B., Martinez, I. M., Pinto, A. M., Salanova, M., and Bakker, A. B. (2002a). Burnout and engagement in university students a cross-national study. J. Cross Cult. Psychol. 33, 464–481. doi: 10.1177/0022022102033005003

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shernoff, D. J. (2010). Engagement in after-school programs as a predictor of social competence and academic performance. Am. J. Community Psychol. 45, 325–337. doi: 10.1007/s10464-010-9314-0

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shi, L. S., Chen, Y. M., Hou, X., and Gao, F. Q. (2013). Social economic status and study engagement: the mediating effects of academic self-efficacy among junior high school students. Psychol. Dev. Educ. 29, 71–78. doi: 10.16187/j.cnki.issn1001-4918.2013.01.006

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Shrout, P. E., and Bolger, N. (2002). Mediation in experimental and nonexperimental studies: new procedures and recommendations. Psychol. Methods 7, 422–445. doi: 10.1037//1082-989X.7.4.422

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sommet, N., Pulfrey, C., Butera, F., and Ozakinci, G. (2013). Did my M.D. really go to university to learn? Detrimental effects of numerus clausus on self-efficacy, mastery goals and learning. PLoS One 8:84178. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0084178

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Sullivan, E. (1980). “Can values be taught?,” in Moral Development and Socialization, eds M. Windmiller, N. Lamber, and E. Turiel (Boston, MA: Allyn and Bacon).

Google Scholar

Sun, Z., Bai, T., Yu, W., Zhou, J., Zhang, M., and Shen, M. (2015). Attentional bias in competitive situations: winner does not take all. Front. Psychol. 6:1469. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01469

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tauer, J. M., and Harackiewicz, J. M. (2004). The effects of cooperation and competition on intrinsic motivation and performance. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol. 86, 849–861. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.86.6.849

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Tomasetto, C., Morsanyi, K., Guardabassi, V., and O’Connor, P. A. (2021). Math anxiety interferes with learning novel mathematics contents in early elementary school. J. Educ. Psychol. 13, 315–329. doi: 10.1037/edu0000602

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Volk, A., Craig, W., Boyce, W., and King, M. (2006). Adolescent risk correlates of bullying and different types of victimization. Int. J. Adolesc. Med. Health 18, 575–586. doi: 10.1515/ijamh.2006.18.4.575

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Volk, A. A., Cioppa, V. D., Earle, M., and Farrell, A. H. (2015). “Social competition and bullying: an adaptive socioecological perspective,” in Evolutionary Perspectives on Social Psychology. Evolutionary Psychology, eds V. Zeigler-Hill, L. Welling, and T. Shackelford (Cham: Springer).

Google Scholar

Wang, Y. H., Yang, Z. X., Zhang, Y. B., Wang, F. M., Liu, T., and Xin, T. (2019). The effect of social-emotional competency on child development in western China. Front. Psychol. 10:1282. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.01282

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wara, E., Aloka, P. J., and Odongo, B. C. (2018). Relationship between emotional engagement and academic achievement among Kenyan secondary school students. Acad. J. Interdiscipl. Stud. 7, 107–118. doi: 10.2478/ajis-2018-0011

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Warren, K., Schoppelrey, S., Moberg, D. P., and McDonald, M. (2005). A model of contagion through competition in the aggressive behaviors of elementary school students. J. Abnorm. Child Psychol. 33, 283–292. doi: 10.1007/s10802-005-3565-5

PubMed Abstract | CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Wong, P. T., Wong, L. C., and Scott, C. (2006). “Beyond stress and coping: the positive psychology of transformation,” in Handbook of Multicultural Perspectives on Stress and Coping, eds P. T. P. Wong and L. C. J. Wong (Boston, MA: Springer).

Google Scholar

Woodman, T., and Hardy, L. (2001). “Stress and anxiety,” in Handbook of Research on Sport Psychology, eds R. Singer, H. A. Hausenblas, and C. M. Janelle (New York, NY: Wiley), 290–318.

Google Scholar

Zhang, C. M., Zou, H., and Hou, K. (2006). The relations among respecting behaviors, personalities and class environment of primary school children. Psychol. Dev. Educ. 3, 16–22. doi: 10.16187/j.cnki.issn1001-4918.2006.03.004

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhang, G. Z., Liang, Z. B., Deng, H. H., and Lu, Z. H. (2014). Relations between perceptions of school climate and school adjustment of adolescents: a longitudinal study. Psychol. Dev. Educ. 30, 371–379. doi: 10.16187/j.cnki.issn1001-4918.2014.04.019

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhen, R., Liu, R., Ding, Y., Wang, J., Liu, Y., and Xu, L. (2017). The mediating roles of academic self-efficacy and academic emotions in the relation between basic psychological needs satisfaction and learning engagement among Chinese adolescent students. Learn. Individ. Differ. 54, 210–216. doi: 10.1016/j.lindif.2017.01.017

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Zhou, B. C. (1991). Mental Health Diagnostic Test (MHT). Shanghai: East China Normal University Press.

Google Scholar

Zimmerman, D. J. (2003). Peer effects in academic outcomes: evidence from a natural experiment. Rev. Econ. Stat. 85, 9–23. doi: 10.2307/3211619

CrossRef Full Text | Google Scholar

Keywords: primary school students, class competition, academic achievement, learning anxiety, learning engagement, mediating role

Citation: Li G, Li Z, Wu X and Zhen R (2022) Relations Between Class Competition and Primary School Students’ Academic Achievement: Learning Anxiety and Learning Engagement as Mediators. Front. Psychol. 13:775213. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2022.775213

Received: 13 September 2021; Accepted: 31 January 2022;
Published: 23 February 2022.

Edited by:

Riikka Mononen, University of Oulu, Finland

Reviewed by:

Ann Dowker, University of Oxford, United Kingdom
Virginia Clinton-Lisell, University of North Dakota, United States

Copyright © 2022 Li, Li, Wu and Zhen. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.

*Correspondence: Rui Zhen, zhenrui1206@126.com

Disclaimer: All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article or claim that may be made by its manufacturer is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.