Next Article in Journal
The Prevalence of Different Types of Headache in Patients with Subjective Tinnitus and Its Influence on Tinnitus Parameters: A Prospective Clinical Study
Next Article in Special Issue
Effortful Control and Prefrontal Cortex Functioning in Children with Autism Spectrum Disorder: An fNIRS Study
Previous Article in Journal
Studying Individual Differences in Spatial Cognition Through Differential Item Functioning Analysis
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Relations between Sensory Responsiveness and Features of Autism in Children

by
Jacob I. Feldman
1,*,
Margaret Cassidy
2,3,
Yupeng Liu
2,4,
Anne V. Kirby
5,
Mark T. Wallace
1,6,7,8,9,10,11 and
Tiffany G. Woynaroski
1,6,7,8
1
Department of Hearing & Speech Sciences, Vanderbilt University Medical Center, Nashville, TN 37232, USA
2
Neuroscience Undergraduate Program, Vanderbilt University, Nashville, TN 37232, USA
3
National Institutes of Health, Bethesda, MD 20814, USA
4
Washington University School of Medicine, Washington University in St. Louis, St. Louis, MO 63110, USA
5
Department of Occupational and Recreational Therapies, University of Utah, Salt Lake City, UT 84112, USA
6
Vanderbilt Kennedy Center, Vanderbilt University Medical Center, Nashville, TN 37232, USA
7
Vanderbilt Brain Institute, Vanderbilt University, Nashville, TN 37232, USA
8
Frist Center for Autism & Innovation, Vanderbilt University, Nashville, TN 37232, USA
9
Department of Psychology, Vanderbilt University, Nashville, TN 37232, USA
10
Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, Vanderbilt University Medical Center, Nashville, TN 37232, USA
11
Department of Pharmacology, Vanderbilt University, Nashville, TN 37232, USA
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Brain Sci. 2020, 10(11), 775; https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci10110775
Submission received: 29 September 2020 / Revised: 18 October 2020 / Accepted: 20 October 2020 / Published: 24 October 2020
(This article belongs to the Special Issue Sensory, Motor and Cognitive Alterations in Autism)

Abstract

:
Autism is a neurodevelopmental condition defined by differences in social communication and by the presence of restricted and repetitive patterns of behavior, interests, and activities (RRBs). Individuals with autism also commonly present with atypical patterns of sensory responsiveness (i.e., hyporesponsiveness, hyperresponsiveness, and sensory seeking), which are theorized to produce cascading effects across other domains of development. The purpose of this study was to examine differences in sensory responsiveness in children with and without autism (ages 8–18 years), as well as relations between patterns of sensory responsiveness and core and related features of autism. Participants were 50 children with autism and 50 non-autistic peers matched on age and sex. A comprehensive clinical battery included multiple measures of sensory responsiveness, core features of autism, adaptive behavior, internalizing behaviors, cognitive ability, and language ability. Groups significantly differed on all three patterns of sensory responsiveness. Some indices of core and related autism features were robustly associated with all three patterns of sensory responsiveness (e.g., RRBs), while others were more strongly associated with discrete patterns of sensory responsiveness (i.e., internalizing problem behaviors and hyperresponsiveness, language and sensory seeking). This study extends prior work to show that differences in sensory responsiveness that are linked with core and related features of autism persist in older children and adolescents on the spectrum.

1. Introduction

Autism is a neurodevelopmental condition that has conventionally been defined by differences in social communication and by the presence of restrictive and repetitive patterns of behaviors, interests, and activities (RRBs) [1]. The most recent diagnostic criteria, however, additionally recognize differences in sensory functioning as a diagnostically relevant feature of autism [2], and a growing body of literature shows that individuals with autism (see Supplementary Materials File S1 for information on our flexible use of terminology) present with altered patterns of sensory responsiveness compared to non-autistic peers [3,4]. Three patterns of sensory responsiveness are commonly described in the literature: hyperresponsiveness, or exaggerated responding to sensory stimuli; hyporesponsiveness, or reduced responding to sensory stimuli; and sensory seeking, or craving of certain sensory stimuli [4,5,6,7]. Although these classifications may be distinct, they are not mutually exclusive or restricted to one sensory modality; individuals can and do present with varied combinations of alterations across patterns of sensory responsiveness and sensory modalities, e.g., [3,8].
It has been proposed that differences in sensory responsiveness, particularly early in life, may produce cascading effects on development across domains, potentially causing or contributing to characteristics associated with autism [9,10]. Findings from empirical research lend some support to this theory. Atypical patterns of sensory responsiveness, for example, have been linked to the core features of RRBs [6,11,12,13,14,15] and social communication in persons on the autism spectrum [12,16,17,18,19,20,21].
In addition to the aforementioned ‘core’ characteristics, individuals with autism may present with a variety of related (i.e., often co-occurring) features that impact their quality of life and long-term outcomes. For example, language abilities in children with autism are highly variable, and early language proficiency is associated with social, academic, and vocational success later in life in this population, e.g., [22,23]. There is evidence to suggest that sensory differences are associated with language abilities, at least in preschoolers with autism [21,24,25]. Other related differences commonly observed in autism, including decreased adaptive behaviors and increased prevalence of anxiety and related behavioral concerns, have also been associated with patterns of sensory responsiveness, e.g., [8,20,25,26,27,28,29,30,31].
The aforementioned findings for associations between patterns of sensory responsiveness and core and related characteristics of autism provide some empirical support for the theory of ‘cascading effects’. There are, however, several limitations of the extant literature. First, the majority of studies to date were conducted with young children, though the literature suggests that differences in sensory responsiveness may persist into adolescence and even adulthood in individuals with autism e.g., [32,33,34,35]. Furthermore, the vast majority of past reports have focused on only one aspect of sensory functioning, e.g., [12,20,26], or have explored relations with only one domain of core or related features of autism, e.g., [6,14,20]. Finally, no study to our knowledge has investigated whether any of the three patterns of sensory responsiveness is a value-added predictor of core and related autism characteristics (i.e., whether one pattern of sensory responsiveness accounts for unique variance in autism features of interest after controlling for the other two patterns of sensory responsiveness; [36]).
The current study thus seeks to comprehensively examine sensory responsiveness in an older sample of school-aged children and adolescents with and without autism and to test relations between all three sensory constructs of interest (i.e., hyporesponsiveness, hyperresponisveness, and sensory seeking) with a broad range of core and related features of autism. Specifically, the research questions to be examined are:
Are between-group differences in sensory responsiveness present in children and adolescents with autism and non-autistic peers matched on chronological age and biological sex?
Are there significant associations between patterns of sensory responsiveness and core and related features of autism?
What proportion of the variance in core and related features of autism is uniquely accounted for by each of the three patterns of sensory responsiveness?
Do any of the three patterns of sensory responsiveness account for a significant amount of unique variance in core and related characteristics of autism when controlling for the other two patterns?

2. Materials and Methods

All experimental procedures were carried out in compliance with the Vanderbilt University Medical Center IRB. The project was initially approved on 15 September 2010 (project no. 101194).

2.1. Participants

Participants were 50 children and adolescents with autism (Mage = 13.1 years; 37 male; 13 female) and 50 children and adolescents without autism (Mage = 13.0 years; 37 male; 13 female) matched at the group level on both chronological age and biological sex. Participants were drawn from a larger study of sensory functioning in children with and without autism e.g., [37,38,39]. Nonverbal cognitive ability (NVIQ) was measured by either the Leiter International Performance Scale–Third edition (Leiter–3) or the Test of Nonverbal Intelligence–Fourth edition (TONI–4) [40,41]. NVIQ means for the autism and non-autism groups were 108.32 and 118.13, respectively (see Table 1).
Inclusion criteria for this study were: (a) chronological age between 8;0 and 18;11 years; (b) normal or corrected-to-normal vision and normal hearing, as confirmed by screening at entry to the study; (c) no history of seizure disorders; and (d) no parent report of diagnosed genetic disorders, such as Fragile X or tuberous sclerosis. Additional inclusion criterion for children with autism was a diagnosis of autism according to DSM-5 criteria [2], as confirmed by a research-reliable administration of the Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule-Second edition (ADOS-2) [42] and the judgement of a licensed clinician on the research team. Additional inclusion criteria for children without autism were (a) scores below the screening threshold for autism risk on the Social Communication Scale (SCQ) [43]; (b) no immediate family members with a diagnoses of autism; (c) NVIQ above 85; and (d) no prior history or present indicators of psychiatric conditions or learning disorders.

2.2. Materials

See Table 2 for a summary of measures and variables derived from those measures for use in planned analyses.

2.2.1. Measures of Sensory Responsiveness

Parents of participants completed the Sensory Experience Questionnaire-Version 2.1 (SEQ) and the Sensory Profile (SP) [5,44]. The summary scores indexing hyporesponsiveness, hyperresponsiveness, and sensory seeking from the SEQ and SP (i.e., SP poor registration and SEQ hyporesponsiveness; SP sensory sensitivity, SP sensation avoiding, SEQ hyperresponsiveness; SP and SEQ sensory seeking) were converted to z-scores. All variables purported to measure the same construct were sufficiently intercorrelated to warrant aggregation (|r| = 0.48–0.71; note that scores from the SP were reflected such that higher scores indicated higher presence of these features, in accord with SEQ scoring). The z-scores purported to tap the same construct were then averaged to create aggregate scores for hyporesponsiveness, hyperresponsiveness, and sensory seeking [32].

2.2.2. Measures of Core Features of Autism

The SCQ, a screening measure used to characterize parents’ concerns related to autism symptomatology, was used to calculate a summary score for the presence of core features of autism. Parents also completed the Social Responsiveness Scale (SRS), a survey instrument that yields indices of RRBs, social communication differences, and total autistic features [45]. As the SCQ scores were highly intercorrelated with the SRS total T scores (r = 0.89), an aggregate of autistic features was generated by averaging z-scores for the aforementioned component variables.

2.2.3. Measures of Related Features of Autism

Parents completed the Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scales-Second edition (VABS), a norm referenced clinical instrument used to measure adaptive behavior across the lifespan in domains including communication, daily living skills, and socialization [46]. Standard scores for each domain were used in analyses.
Participants also completed the Behavior Assessment System for Children-Third edition (BASC), a norm referenced parent/caregiver report measure that assesses child behavioral and emotional functionality [47]. From this assessment, T-scores were extracted in the following domains: anxiety, depression, and somatization, which comprise the internalizing problem behaviors composite, and the functional impairment probability index.

2.2.4. Measures of Language Ability

Participants completed a battery of standardized, norm-referenced language assessments, including the Receptive One Word Picture Vocabulary Test-Fourth edition (ROWPVT) and the Expressive One Word Picture Vocabulary Test-Fourth edition (EOWPVT) [48,49]. Standard scores from these assessments were used in analyses.
Additionally, participants completed the Clinical Evaluation of Language Fundamentals-Fourth edition (CELF) [50]. From this measure, standard scores for core language, receptive language, and expressive language were utilized in analyses.

2.3. Procedures

Participants traveled to Vanderbilt University Medical Center for each appointment; all measures were collected over the course of one to three visits. Parent surveys were provided to the parent or caregiver to complete during the first appointment. Language assessments were administered by either a certified speech-language pathologist or a student/clinical fellow under the supervision of a certified speech language pathologist.

2.4. Analytic Plan

Prior to conducting analyses, missing data (ranging from 0–11% across variables derived for use in analyses) were imputed using the missForest package in R [51,52]. The first research question examined differences in patterns of sensory responsiveness between the autism and non-autism groups. Independent samples t-tests were performed on the aggregates of sensory responsiveness.
The second research question examined correlations between aggregate scores for patterns of sensory responsiveness and the various indices of core and related features of autism. Bivariate correlational analyses, corrected for multiple comparisons using a Benjamini–Yekutieli false discovery rate correction, were utilized to evaluate the magnitude and direction of all associations of interest [53,54]. Steiger’s z tests were utilized to test the difference between bivariate correlations with dependent variables (i.e., indices of core and related autism features) in common using the cocor package in R [55,56]. Moderation analyses were conducted to test whether any associations of interest varied according to diagnostic group [57]. Cook’s D was utilized to monitor for undue influence throughout regression analyses; discrete data points with D values ≥ 1 were removed [58].
The third research question examined the proportion of variance in each core and related feature of autism jointly and uniquely attributable to each of the three patterns of sensory responsiveness. To answer this question, a series of multiple regression analyses were conducted. The aggregate variables for each pattern of sensory responsiveness were regressed onto each feature of autism in step-wise models utilizing systematically varied orders (see [59] for an example of this approach). The change in the proportion of variance accounted for in the final step of the model (ΔR2; e.g., the change in the variance accounted for when adding hyporesponsiveness into a model that already included sensory seeking and hyperresponsiveness as predictors) was considered the amount of unique variance attributable to each pattern of sensory responsiveness. To answer the fourth research question, each model containing two patterns of sensory responsiveness was then compared to the full model using the anova function in R to determine whether adding the third pattern of sensory responsiveness as a predictor resulted in a significant increase in model fit.

3. Results

3.1. Group Differences

Results indicate that groups differed significantly in all three patterns of sensory responsiveness. On average, children with autism were rated as significantly higher in hyporesponsiveness (t = 13.12, d = 2.6, p < 0.001), hyperresponsiveness (t = 12.62, d = 2.5, p < 0.001), and sensory seeking (t = 7.77, d = 1.6, p < 0.001) compared to their peers without autism (see Figure 1). All effects were large in magnitude. These group differences are robust to controlling for nonverbal IQ (all p values for the effect of group remain <0.001).

3.2. Correlational Analyses

See Table 3 for a summary of findings for associations of interest for patterns of sensory responsiveness with core and related features of autism.
Hyporesponsiveness and hyperresponsiveness were not significantly associated with nonverbal IQ or age (r values = −0.10–−0.25). Increased sensory seeking, however, was negatively associated with age (r = −0.31, p = 0.009) and nonverbal IQ (r = −0.33, p = 0.004). Additionally, all three patterns of sensory responsiveness were strongly intercorrelated, r values = 0.57–0.79.
All three patterns of sensory responsiveness were strongly correlated with the variables quantifying the core features of autism, the social communication and RRB subscales of the SRS and the core autism feature aggregate, with large magnitudes (r values = 0.67–0.78; see Figure 2A–C for an example). Steiger’s z tests indicated that the correlations between the social communication index of the SRS and the total autism feature aggregate with both hyperresponsiveness and hyporesponsiveness were stronger than those with sensory seeking (z values ≥ 3.07; p values ≤ 0.001). The remaining correlations for patterns of sensory responsiveness with core autism features were not significantly different.
In regards to adaptive behaviors, all three patterns of sensory responsiveness negatively correlated with the indices of the VABS with moderate to large magnitudes (r values = −0.44–−0.80), such that increased sensory behaviors were associated with decreased adaptive behaviors. The correlations for the VABS variables with hyporesponsiveness were consistently stronger than those with sensory seeking (z values ≥ 2.45; p values ≤ 0.014). The correlations for the socialization and daily living skills indices with hyperresponsiveness were also stronger than those with sensory seeking (z values = 2.94 and 2.57, respectively; p values = 0.003 and 0.010). The remaining correlations were not significantly different.
All of the indices from the BASC were moderately to strongly associated with hyperresponsiveness (r values = 0.45–0.81), such that increased hyperresponsiveness was associated with more parent report of internalizing behaviors and functional impairment; all of these correlations were stronger than those with sensory seeking (r values = 0.19–0.65; z values ≥ 2.69). Hyporesponsiveness was also moderately to strongly correlated with BASC scores (r values = 0.36–0.80); however, several of these correlations (i.e., those with anxiety and internalizing problem behaviors) were weaker than those with hyperresponsiveness (z values = 3.27 and 2.63, respectively; p values = 0.001 and 0.009).
In regards to language, all of the correlations with hyporesponsiveness (r values = −0.31–−0.41) and sensory seeking (r values = −0.32–−0.48) were moderate in magnitude, such that increased hyporesponsiveness and sensory seeking behavior was associated with decreased language (see Figure 2D,E for an example). The correlations between indices of language and hyperresponsiveness were small to moderate (r values = 0.27–0.37). None of the correlations between aggregate scores for patterns of sensory responsiveness and language significantly differed. None of the associations of interest in the study were moderated by group.
Given that groups differed on nonverbal IQ, post-hoc analyses were conducted in R to control for this factor [60]. Correcting for multiple comparisons, the majority of significant correlations between patterns of sensory responsiveness and the core and related features of autism were robust to controlling for nonverbal IQ (see Table S1 in supplementary material). When controlling for nonverbal IQ, however, selected relations between indices of sensory responsiveness and language were attenuated and no longer surpassed the threshold for statistical significance. Associations between patterns of sensory responsiveness and broad language ability as measured by the CELF, as well as the association between sensory seeking and receptive vocabulary as measured by the ROWPVT remained statistically significant when controlling for IQ (refer to Table S1 for further detail).

3.3. Unique Variance Explained by Patterns of Sensory Responsivness

See Table 4 for information regarding the amount of unique variance in each core and related feature of interest attributable to each pattern of sensory responsiveness and the total amount of variance accounted for by all three patterns in the final model.
In regards to the core features of autism, hyperresponsiveness and hyporesponsiveness both accounted for a significant amount of unique variance when added to the multiple regression model for all three dependent variables, while sensory seeking accounted for a significant amount of unique variance when added to the models for RRBs and core autism features.
For indices derived from the VABS, hyporresponsiveness accounted for a significant amount of unique variance for all three domains, sensory seeking accounted for significant unique variance in communication, and hyperresponsiveness accounted for significant unique variance in daily living skills and socialization. Hyperresponsiveness was the only pattern of sensory responsiveness that significantly accounted for unique variance in the anxiety, depression, somatization, and internalizing problem behavior indices of the BASC. All three patterns of sensory responsiveness, however, significantly accounted for unique variance in the functional impairment probability index.
Sensory seeking significantly accounted for unique variance in the expressive vocabulary (i.e., EOWPVT) standard scores and all three indices from the CELF. None of the other patterns of sensory responsiveness accounted for unique variance in language scores.

4. Discussion

The present study sought to examine sensory responsiveness in older children and adolescents and its relation to the core and related features of autism. The older children and adolescents with autism in the present study exhibited significantly altered patterns of sensory responsiveness relative to a control group of peers without autism matched on chronological age and sex. Effect sizes for between-group differences across hyporesponsiveness, hyperresponsiveness, and sensory seeking were all large in magnitude. This finding extends the extant literature on sensory differences in individuals with autism by examining these patterns in a relatively understudied age-range using multiple previously developed and validated measures of sensory responsiveness with a large sample size. The robust differences between groups that we observed suggest that sensory differences observed early in life in individuals with autism do not resolve within this age range and are robust to controlling for nonverbal IQ.
Our study is the first to comprehensively evaluate correlations between patterns of sensory responsiveness and the wide range of core and related symptoms of autism within a single sample. Even after correcting for multiple comparisons, nearly all of the correlations between patterns of sensory responsiveness and overall presence of core autistic features, adaptive behavior skills, internalizing problem behaviors, and language abilities were significant, with moderate to large effect sizes (the sole exception being the relation between somatization and sensory seeking, which was small and non-significant). The majority of these associations were also robust to controlling for nonverbal IQ (see Table S1).
Sensory hyporesponsiveness and hyperresponsiveness were consistently and robustly correlated with the core features of autism, social communication differences in particular, and adaptive behavior. Of the three patterns of sensory responsiveness, hyporesponsiveness has been reported to be most prevalent in, and specific to, autism [4,24]. It has been proposed that hyporesponsiveness may be particularly disruptive to development, as children with high hyporesponsiveness often fail to orient towards, and may subsequently fail to engage with and learn from, sensory information in their environment [17,32]. Although hyperresponsiveness is also commonly observed in other neurodevelopmental conditions (e.g., attention deficit hyperactivity disorder [61,62]), hyperresponsiveness is also very frequently observed in autism [4] and has been proposed to be a causal factor in autism [63]. Notably, all three patterns of sensory responsiveness were strongly correlated with and accounted for unique variance in RRBs. Longitudinal work is much needed to determine whether alterations in patterns of sensory responsiveness precede broader differences in young children with or at increased likelihood for autism.
Interestingly, sensory hyperresponsiveness was most strongly associated with internalizing problem behaviors in this sample. This is, perhaps, not surprising, given that hyperresponsiveness has previously been linked with anxious behaviors [8,26]. It may be important for occupational therapists to intervene with children demonstrating hyperresponsiveness in order to reduce distress and curtail downstream consequences of internalizing problems (e.g., suicide, which may be increased in autism [64,65]) in this population [66].
The current study is the first to demonstrate robust links between language and sensory responsiveness in older children and adolescents with autism. All three patterns of sensory responsiveness displayed significant associations with our five variables indexing language ability. Of the three patterns of sensory responsiveness, sensory seeking tended to account for the greatest amount of unique variance in language-related constructs. This finding is consistent with the previous literature on young children and preschoolers with autism, which has consistently linked sensory seeking to language and communication abilities [16,17,20,21,25]. Future research should examine the emergence of sensory seeking behaviors as they relate to different aspects of language development (e.g., semantics, syntax) early in life. Additionally, given that language abilities are critical to literacy, e.g., [59], future research should examine the degree to which sensory responsiveness relates to reading proficiency in persons on the spectrum.
There are several strengths to the present study. First, we comprehensively examined between- group differences and associations of interest in a large sample of children with autism and a control group matched on several factors, including biological sex and chronological age. Diagnostic status of children within the autism group was confirmed via gold-standard measures, and all participants were well characterized. Multiple measures of sensory responsiveness were employed, increasing the stability, and thus the potential construct validity, of our indices of hyporesponsiveness, hyperresponsiveness, and sensory seeking [67]. Additionally, this study investigated correlations between sensory responsiveness and a broad range of core and related features of autism as evaluated via a very comprehensive battery of assessments.
There are, however, limitations to this study. First, our sample was limited to older children and adolescents on the autism spectrum. Additional work is therefore necessary to ascertain the degree to which the present findings generalize to younger children and/or adults with autism. Additionally, we did not exclude participants from either group who had certain co-occurring conditions common to autism, such as depression, anxiety, and attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). Given that other populations are known to exhibit these altered patterns of sensory responsiveness (e.g., individuals with ADHD [61,62], Down syndrome [68,69], specific language impairment [70]), additional work is warranted to determine whether the relations observed and reported here are specific to autism, or apply to neurodevelopmental conditions more broadly. Finally, we did not track the medications used by participants in the present study. Some medications that are prescribed to children with autism (e.g., psychotropic medications [71]) lead to altered brain function in this population e.g., [72]. Future studies should thus (a) track medication use and (b) evaluate whether there is an effect of psychotropic medication on sensory functioning in this population.

5. Conclusions

This study evaluated sensory responsiveness and its relation to clinical features in older children and adolescents with autism. We found that children with autism presented with significantly altered sensory responsiveness compared to matched peers without autism. Across both groups, all three patterns of sensory responsiveness were associated with core and related features of autism. Hyporesponsiveness and hyperresponsiveness were most robustly correlated with the core features of autism, in particular social communication differences, and adaptive behavior; all three patterns of sensory responsiveness were highly correlated with RRBs; hyperresponsiveness was most strongly associated with internalizing problem behaviors; and sensory seeking was most strongly associated with measures of language. Future research should investigate these relations longitudinally in young children with or at increased likelihood for autism and cross-sectionally in adults with autism and children with other neurodevelopmental conditions, such as Down syndrome and specific language impairment.

Supplementary Materials

The following are available online at https://www.mdpi.com/2076-3425/10/11/775/s1, File S1: Information on flexible use of terminology; Table S1: Partial correlations between patterns of sensory responsiveness and core and related features of autism controlling for nonverbal IQ.

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, T.G.W., M.T.W., and J.I.F.; Methodology, T.G.W. and J.I.F.; Validation, J.I.F., M.C., and Y.L.; Formal analysis, J.I.F.; Resources, M.T.W.; Data curation, T.G.W., J.I.F., M.C., and Y.L.; Writing—original draft preparation, J.I.F. and M.C.; Writing—review and editing, all authors; Visualization, J.I.F. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This research was funded by NIH U54 HD083211 (PI: Neul), NIH/NCATS KL2TR000446 (PI: Woynaroski), NIH/NIDCD 1R21DC016144 (PI: Woynaroski), and NSF NRT grant DGE 19-22697 (PI: Wallace). T.G.W. is additionally funded in part by the Nancy Lurie Marks Family Foundation (PI: Schaaf).

Acknowledgments

We would like to thank Wayne Kuang, Julie Conrad, and Alexander Tu for their assistance in organizing the data. We also thank all of the families for their participation.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

  1. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders-IV-TR; APA: Washington, DC, USA, 2000. [Google Scholar]
  2. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders-5; APA: Washington, DC, USA, 2013. [Google Scholar]
  3. Ben-Sasson, A.; Hen, L.; Fluss, R.; Cermak, S.A.; Engel-Yeger, B.; Gal, E. A meta-analysis of sensory modulation symptoms in individuals with autism spectrum disorders. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 2009, 39, 1–11. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  4. Ben-Sasson, A.; Gal, E.; Fluss, R.; Katz-Zetler, N.; Cermak, S.A. Update of a meta-analysis of sensory symptoms in ASD: A new decade of research. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 2019, 49, 4974–4996. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  5. Baranek, G.T.; David, F.J.; Poe, M.D.; Stone, W.L.; Watson, L.R. Sensory Experiences Questionnaire: Discriminating sensory features in young children with autism, developmental delays, and typical development. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 2006, 47, 591–601. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  6. Boyd, B.A.; Baranek, G.T.; Sideris, J.; Poe, M.D.; Watson, L.R.; Patten, E.; Miller, H. Sensory features and repetitive behaviors in children with autism and developmental delays. Autism Res. 2010, 3, 78–87. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  7. Liss, M.; Saulnier, C.; Fein, D.; Kinsbourne, M. Sensory and attention abnormalities in autistic spectrum disorders. Autism 2006, 10, 155–172. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  8. Lane, S.J.; Reynolds, S.; Dumenci, L. Sensory overresponsivity and anxiety in typically developing children and children with autism and attention deficit hyperactivity disorder: Cause or coexistence? Am. J. Occup. Ther. 2012, 66, 595–603. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  9. Cascio, C.J.; Woynaroski, T.; Baranek, G.T.; Wallace, M.T. Toward an interdisciplinary approach to understanding sensory function in autism spectrum disorder. Autism Res. 2016, 9, 920–925. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Robertson, C.E.; Baron-Cohen, S. Sensory perception in autism. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2017, 18, 671–684. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Boyd, B.A.; McBee, M.; Holtzclaw, T.; Baranek, G.T.; Bodfish, J.W. Relationships among repetitive behaviors, sensory features, and executive functions in high functioning autism. Res. Autism Spectr. Disord. 2009, 3, 959–966. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  12. Foss-Feig, J.H.; Heacock, J.L.; Cascio, C.J. Tactile responsiveness patterns and their association with core features in autism spectrum disorders. Res. Autism Spectr. Disord. 2012, 6, 337–344. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  13. Gabriels, R.L.; Agnew, J.A.; Miller, L.J.; Gralla, J.; Pan, Z.; Goldson, E.; Ledbetter, J.C.; Dinkins, J.P.; Hooks, E. Is there a relationship between restricted, repetitive, stereotyped behaviors and interests and abnormal sensory response in children with autism spectrum disorders? Res. Autism Spectr. Disord. 2008, 2, 660–670. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  14. Schulz, S.E.; Stevenson, R.A. Sensory hypersensitivity predicts repetitive behaviours in autistic and typically-developing children. Autism 2019, 23, 1028–1041. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  15. Grzadzinski, R.; Donovan, K.; Truong, K.; Nowell, S.; Lee, H.; Sideris, J.; Turner-Brown, L.; Baranek, G.T.; Watson, L.R. Sensory reactivity at 1 and 2 years old is associated with ASD severity during the preschool years. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 2020. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  16. Baranek, G.T.; Woynaroski, T.G.; Nowell, S.; Turner-Brown, L.; DuBay, M.; Crais, E.R.; Watson, L.R. Cascading effects of attention disengagement and sensory seeking on social symptoms in a community sample of infants at-risk for a future diagnosis of autism spectrum disorder. Dev. Cogn. Neurosci. 2018, 29, 30–40. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Damiano-Goodwin, C.R.; Woynaroski, T.G.; Simon, D.M.; Ibañez, L.V.; Murias, M.; Kirby, A.; Newsom, C.R.; Wallace, M.T.; Stone, W.L.; Cascio, C.J. Developmental sequelae and neurophysiologic substrates of sensory seeking in infant siblings of children with autism spectrum disorder. Dev. Cogn. Neurosci. 2018, 29, 41–53. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  18. Horder, J.; Wilson, C.E.; Mendez, M.A.; Murphy, D.G. Autistic traits and abnormal sensory experiences in adults. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 2014, 44, 1461–1469. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  19. Lane, A.E.; Young, R.L.; Baker, A.E.Z.; Angley, M.T. Sensory processing subtypes in autism: Association with adaptive behavior. J. Autism Deve. Disord. 2010, 40, 112–122. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  20. Williams, K.L.; Kirby, A.V.; Watson, L.R.; Sideris, J.; Bulluck, J.; Baranek, G.T. Sensory features as predictors of adaptive behaviors: A comparative longitudinal study of children with autism spectrum disorder and other developmental disabilities. Res. Dev. Disabil. 2018, 81, 103–112. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Nowell, S.W.; Watson, L.R.; Crais, E.R.; Baranek, G.T.; Faldowski, R.A.; Turner-Brown, L. Joint attention and sensory-regulatory features at 13 and 22 months as predictors of preschool language and social-communication outcomes. J. Speech Lang. Hear. Res. 2020, 63, 3100–3116. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Billstedt, E.; Carina Gillberg, I.; Gillberg, C. Autism in adults: Symptom patterns and early childhood predictors. Use of the DISCO in a community sample followed from childhood. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 2007, 48, 1102–1110. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  23. Kobayashi, R.; Murata, T.; Yoshinaga, K. A follow-up study of 201 children with autism in Kyushu and Yamaguchi areas, Japan. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 1992, 22, 395–411. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  24. Baranek, G.T.; Watson, L.R.; Boyd, B.A.; Poe, M.D.; David, F.J.; McGuire, L. Hyporesponsiveness to social and nonsocial sensory stimuli in children with autism, children with developmental delays, and typically developing children. Dev. Psychopathol. 2013, 25, 307–320. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  25. Watson, L.R.; Patten, E.; Baranek, G.T.; Poe, M.; Boyd, B.A.; Freuler, A.; Lorenzi, J. Differential associations between sensory response patterns and language, social, and communication measures in children with autism or other developmental disabilities. J. Speech Lang. Hear. Res. 2011, 54, 1562–1576. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  26. Green, S.A.; Ben-Sasson, A.; Soto, T.W.; Carter, A.S. Anxiety and sensory over-responsivity in toddlers with autism spectrum disorders: Bidirectional effects across time. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 2012, 42, 1112–1119. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  27. Neil, L.; Olsson, N.C.; Pellicano, E. The relationship between intolerance of uncertainty, sensory sensitivities, and anxiety in autistic and typically developing children. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 2016, 46, 1962–1973. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  28. Lidstone, J.; Uljarević, M.; Sullivan, J.; Rodgers, J.; McConachie, H.; Freeston, M.; Le Couteur, A.; Prior, M.; Leekam, S. Relations among restricted and repetitive behaviors, anxiety and sensory features in children with autism spectrum disorders. Res. Autism Spectr. Disord. 2014, 8, 82–92. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  29. Pfeiffer, B.; Kinnealey, M.; Reed, C.; Herzberg, G. Sensory modulation and affective disorders in children and adolescents with Asperger’s disorder. Am. J. Occup. Ther. 2005, 59, 335–345. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  30. Rogers, S.J.; Hepburn, S.; Wehner, E. Parent reports of sensory symptoms in toddlers with autism and those with other developmental disorders. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 2003, 33, 631–642. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Wigham, S.; Rodgers, J.; South, M.; McConachie, H.; Freeston, M. The Interplay between sensory processing abnormalities, intolerance of uncertainty, anxiety and restricted and repetitive behaviours in autism spectrum disorder. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 2015, 45, 943–952. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Feldman, J.I.; Kuang, W.; Conrad, J.G.; Tu, A.; Santapuram, P.; Simon, D.M.; Foss-Feig, J.H.; Kwakye, L.D.; Stevenson, R.A.; Wallace, M.T.; et al. Brief report: Differences in multisensory integration covary with differences in sensory responsiveness in children with and without autism spectrum disorder. J. Autism Dev. Disabil. 2019, 49, 397–403. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  33. Tavassoli, T.; Hoekstra, R.A.; Baron-Cohen, S. The Sensory Perception Quotient (SPQ): Development and validation of a new sensory questionnaire for adults with and without autism. Mol. Autism 2014, 5, 1–10. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  34. Gonthier, C.; Longuépée, L.; Bouvard, M. Sensory processing in low-functioning adults with autism spectrum disorder: Distinct sensory profiles and their relationships with behavioral dysfunction. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 2016, 46, 3078–3089. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  35. Elwin, M.; Schröder, A.; Ek, L.; Wallsten, T.; Kjellin, L. Sensory clusters of adults with and without autism spectrum conditions. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 2017, 47, 579–589. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  36. Yoder, P.; Watson, L.R.; Lambert, W. Value-added predictors of expressive and receptive language growth in initially nonverbal preschoolers with autism spectrum disorders. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 2015, 45, 1254–1270. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  37. Dunham, K.; Feldman, J.I.; Liu, Y.; Cassidy, M.; Conrad, J.G.; Santapuram, P.; Suzman, E.; Tu, A.; Butera, I.M.; Simon, D.M.; et al. Stability of variables derived from measures of multisensory function in children with autism spectrum disorder. Am. J. Intellect. Dev. Disabil. 2020, 125, 287–303. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  38. Noel, J.P.; De Niear, M.; Lazzara, N.S.; Wallace, M.T. Uncoupling between multisensory temporal function and non-verbal turn-taking in autism spectrum disorder. IEEE Trans. Cogn. Dev. Syst. 2018, 10, 973–982. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  39. Feldman, J.I.; Dunham, K.; Conrad, J.G.; Simon, D.M.; Cassidy, M.; Liu, Y.; Tu, A.; Broderick, N.; Wallace, M.T.; Woynaroski, T.G. Perceptual training in children with autism spectrum disorder: A single-case treatment study. Res. Autism Spectr. Disord. 2020, 74, 1–13. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  40. Brown, L.; Sherbenou, R.J.; Johnsen, S.K. Test of Nonverbal Intelligence, 4th ed.; Pro-Ed: Austin, TX, USA, 2010. [Google Scholar]
  41. Roid, G.H.; Miller, L.J.; Pomplun, M.; Koch, C. Leiter International Performance Scale, 3rd ed.; Western Psychological Services: Torrance, CA, USA, 2013. [Google Scholar]
  42. Lord, C.; Rutter, M.; DiLavore, P.; Risi, S.; Gotham, K.; Bishop, S.L. Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule, Second Edition (ADOS-2) Manual (Part I): Modules 1–4; Western Psychological Services: Los Angeles, CA, USA, 2012. [Google Scholar]
  43. Rutter, M.; Bailey, A.; Lord, C. Social Communication Questionnaire (SCQ); Western Psychological Services: Los Angeles, CA, USA, 2003. [Google Scholar]
  44. Dunn, W. The Sensory Profile: User’s Manual; Psychological Corporation: San Antonio, TX, USA, 1999. [Google Scholar]
  45. Constantino, J.N.; Gruber, C.P. Social Responsiveness Scale, 2nd ed.; Western Psychological Services: Torrance, CA, USA, 2012. [Google Scholar]
  46. Sparrow, S.S.; Cicchetti, D.V.; Bella, D.A. Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scales, 2nd ed.; Pearson: Livonia, MN, USA, 2005. [Google Scholar]
  47. Reynolds, C.R.; Kamphaus, R.W. Behavior Assessment System for Children, 3rd ed.; Pearson: Livonia, MN, USA, 2015. [Google Scholar]
  48. Martin, N.T.; Brownell, R. Receptive One-Word Picture Vocabulary Test, 4th ed.; Academic Therapy Publications: Novato, CA, USA, 2010. [Google Scholar]
  49. Brownell, R. Expressive One-Word Picture Vocabulary Test, 4th ed.; Academic Therapy Publications: Novato, CA, USA, 2010. [Google Scholar]
  50. Semel, E.M.; Wiig, E.H.; Secord, W.A. Clinical Evaluation of Language Fundamentals, 4th ed.; The Psychological Corporation: San Antonio, TX, USA, 2004. [Google Scholar]
  51. Stekhoven, D.J.; Bühlmann, P. MissForest—Non-parametric missing value imputation for mixed-type data. Bioinformatics 2012, 28, 112–118. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  52. R Core Team. R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing, 4.0.2; R Foundation for Statistical Computing: Vienna, Austria, 2020. [Google Scholar]
  53. Benjamini, Y.; Yekutieli, D. The control of the false discovery rate in multiple testing under dependency. Ann. Stat. 2001, 29, 1165–1188. [Google Scholar]
  54. Vasilopoulos, T.; Morey, T.E.; Dhatariya, K.; Rice, M.J. Limitations of significance testing in clinical research: A review of multiple comparison corrections and effect size calculations with correlated measures. Anesth. Analg. 2016, 122, 825–830. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  55. Steiger, J.H. Tests for comparing elements of a correlation matrix. Psychol. Bull. 1980, 87, 245–251. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  56. Diedenhofen, B.; Musch, J. cocor: A comprehensive solution for the statistical comparison of correlations. PLoS ONE 2015, 10. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  57. Hayes, A.F. Introduction to Mediation, Moderation, and Conditional Process Analysis: A Regression-Based Approach; Guilford Press: New York, NY, USA, 2017. [Google Scholar]
  58. Cook, R.D. Detection of influential observation in linear regression. Technometrics 1977, 19, 15–18. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  59. Cutting, L.E.; Scarborough, H.S. Prediction of reading comprehension: Relative contributions of word recognition, language proficiency, and other cognitive skills can depend on how comprehension is measured. Sci. Stud. Read. 2006, 10, 277–299. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  60. Kim, S. ppcor: An R package for a fast calculation to semi-partial correlation coefficients. Commun. Stat. Appl. Methods 2015, 22, 665–674. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  61. Cheung, P.P.P.; Siu, A.M.H. A comparison of patterns of sensory processing in children with and without developmental disabilities. Res. Dev. Disabil. 2009, 30, 1468–1480. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  62. Little, L.M.; Dean, E.; Tomchek, S.; Dunn, W. Sensory processing patterns in autism, attention deficit hyperactivity disorder, and typical development. Phys. Occup. Ther. Pediatrics 2018, 38, 243–254. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  63. Markram, K.; Markram, H. The intense world theory—A unifying theory of the neurobiology of autism. Front. Hum. Neurosci 2010, 4. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  64. Kirby, A.V.; Bakian, A.V.; Zhang, Y.; Bilder, D.A.; Keeshin, B.R.; Coon, H. A 20-year study of suicide death in a statewide autism population. Autism Res. 2019, 12, 658–666. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  65. Cassidy, S.A.; Robertson, A.; Townsend, E.; O’Connor, R.C.; Rodgers, J. Advancing our understanding of self-harm, suicidal thoughts and behaviours in autism. J. Autism Dev. Disord. 2020, 50, 3445–3449. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  66. Kirby, A.V.; Terrill, A.L.; Schwartz, A.; Henderson, J.; Whitaker, B.N.; Kramer, J. Occupational therapy practitioners’ knowledge, comfort, and competence regarding youth suicide. OTJR Occup. Particip. Health 2020. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  67. Rushton, J.P.; Brainerd, C.J.; Pressley, M. Behavioral development and construct validity: The principle of aggregation. Psychol. Bull. 1983, 94, 18–38. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  68. Bruni, M.; Cameron, D.; Dua, S.; Noy, S. Reported sensory processing of children with Down syndrome. Phys. Occup. Ther. Pediatrics 2010, 30, 280–293. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  69. Wuang, Y.-P.; Su, C.-Y. Correlations of sensory processing and visual organization ability with participation in school-aged children with Down syndrome. Res. Dev. Disabil. 2011, 32, 2398–2407. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  70. Taal, M.N.; Rietman, A.B.; Meulen, S.V.D.; Schipper, M.; Dejonckere, P.H. Children with specific language impairment show difficulties in sensory modulation. Logop. Phoniatr. Vocol. 2013, 38, 70–78. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  71. Coury, D.L.; Anagnostou, E.; Manning-Courtney, P.; Reynolds, A.; Cole, L.; McCoy, R.; Whitaker, A.; Perrin, J.M. Use of psychotropic medication in children and adolescents with autism spectrum disorders. Pediatrics 2012, 130, S69–S76. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  72. Linke, A.C.; Olson, L.; Gao, Y.; Fishman, I.; Müller, R.-A. Psychotropic medication use in autism spectrum disorders may affect functional brain connectivity. Biol. Psychiatry Cogn. Neurosci. Neuroimaging 2017, 2, 518–527. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Figure 1. Group differences in sensory responsiveness. On average, children with autism (blue) were rated as significantly higher than the children without autism (yellow) in all three patterns of sensory responsiveness. Hypo = hyporesponsiveness; hyper = hyperresponsiveness; seeking = sensory seeking.
Figure 1. Group differences in sensory responsiveness. On average, children with autism (blue) were rated as significantly higher than the children without autism (yellow) in all three patterns of sensory responsiveness. Hypo = hyporesponsiveness; hyper = hyperresponsiveness; seeking = sensory seeking.
Brainsci 10 00775 g001
Figure 2. Scatterplots depicting selected associations between patterns of sensory responsiveness and features of autism. All three patterns of sensory responsiveness were positively correlated with the restricted, repetitive behaviors (RRB) index T scores from the Social Responsiveness Scale (SRS; (AC)) [45] and negatively correlated with core language standard scores (SS) from the Clinical Evaluation of Language Fundamentals-Fourth edition (CELF; (DF)) [50]. All depicted associations were statistically significant; effects of interest were observed across, and did not significantly vary according to, diagnostic group (autism group plotted in blue; non-autism group plotted in yellow).
Figure 2. Scatterplots depicting selected associations between patterns of sensory responsiveness and features of autism. All three patterns of sensory responsiveness were positively correlated with the restricted, repetitive behaviors (RRB) index T scores from the Social Responsiveness Scale (SRS; (AC)) [45] and negatively correlated with core language standard scores (SS) from the Clinical Evaluation of Language Fundamentals-Fourth edition (CELF; (DF)) [50]. All depicted associations were statistically significant; effects of interest were observed across, and did not significantly vary according to, diagnostic group (autism group plotted in blue; non-autism group plotted in yellow).
Brainsci 10 00775 g002
Table 1. Means and standard deviations of selected variables by group
Table 1. Means and standard deviations of selected variables by group
Autism (n = 50)
M (SD) or n (%)
Non-Autism (n = 50)
M (SD) or n (%)
Chronological Age (Years)13.1 (3.1)13.0 (2.7)
Nonverbal IQ *109.0 (16.2)118.0 (13.0)
Biological Sex
Male37 (74%)37 (74%)
Female13 (26%)13 (26%)
Race
Asian3 (6%)1 (2%)
Black or African American2 (4%)4 (8%)
White35 (70%)40 (80%)
Multiple Races7 (14%)5 (10%)
Not Reported3 (6%)0 (0%)
Ethnicity
Hispanic or Latino5 (10%)5 (10%)
Not Hispanic or Latino43 (86%)44 (88%)
Not Reported2 (4%)1 (2%)
Note: Nonverbal IQ = Nonverbal intelligence as measured by the Leiter International Performance Scale–Third edition or the Test of Nonverbal Intelligence–Fourth edition [40,41]. * Denotes groups significantly differed, p < 0.01.
Table 2. Summary of measures and variables derived for use in analyses
Table 2. Summary of measures and variables derived for use in analyses
MeasureTypeVariables Used in Analyses
Measures of Nonverbal Cognitive Ability
Leiter International Performance Scale–Third edition (Leiter–3) [41] or
Test of Nonverbal Intelligence–Fourth edition (TONI–4) [40]
Standardized AssessmentNonverbal IQ
Measures of Sensory Responsiveness
Sensory Experiences Questionnaire-Version 2.1 [5]Parent ReportSensory Seeking a, Hyperresponsiveness b, and Hyporesponsiveness c mean scores
Sensory Profile [44]Parent ReportLow Registration (Hypo) c, Sensation Seeking (Seeking) a, Sensory Sensitivity (Hyper) b, and Sensation Avoiding (Hyper) b scores
Measures of Core Features of Autism
Social Communication Questionnaire [43]Parent ReportTotal scored
Social Responsiveness Scale [45]Parent ReportRestricted Interests and Repetitive Behavior, Social Communication Impairment, and Overall Autistic Features d T-scores
Measures of Related Features of Autism
Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scales–Second edition [46]Parent ReportCommunication, Daily Living Skills, and Socialization Domain standard scores
Behavior Assessment System for Children–Third edition [47]Parent ReportAnxiety, Depression, and Somatization scale T-scores; Internalizing Problem Behaviors and Functional Impairment Probability Index T-scores
Measures of Language Ability
Receptive One Word Picture Vocabulary Test–Fourth edition [48]Standardized AssessmentReceptive Vocabulary standard score
Expressive One Word Picture Vocabulary Test–Fourth edition [49]Standardized AssessmentExpressive Vocabulary standard score
Clinical Evaluation of Language Fundamentals–Fourth edition [50]Standardized AssessmentCore Language, Receptive Language and Expressive Language Index standard scores
Note: Seeking = Sensory Seeking, Hyper = Hyperresponsiveness, Hypo = Hyporesponsiveness. Variables were aggregated to measure: a sensory seeking, b hyperresponsiveness, c hyporesponsiveness, d core autism features.
Table 3. Zero-order correlations between patterns of sensory responsiveness and core and related features of autism
Table 3. Zero-order correlations between patterns of sensory responsiveness and core and related features of autism
HypoHyperSeeking
Sample Characteristics
Age−0.10−0.12−0.31 **
Nonverbal IQ−0.23−0.25−0.33 **
Sensory Responsiveness
Hypo10.79 ***0.60 ***
Hyper0.79 ***10.57 ***
Seeking0.60 ***0.57 ***1
Core Features of Autism
SRS Social Communication T score0.85 ***0.83 ***0.62 ***
SRS RRB T-score0.78 ***0.80 ***0.72 ***
Core Autism Feature Aggregate0.87 ***0.83 ***0.67 ***
Related Features of Autism
VABS Communication SS−0.72 ***−0.65 ***−0.57 ***
VABS Daily Living Skills SS−0.66 ***−0.63 ***−0.44 ***
VABS Socialization SS−0.80 ***−0.75 ***−0.57 ***
BASC Anxiety T score0.44 ***0.61 ***0.29 *
BASC Depression T score0.63 ***0.70 ***0.52 ***
BASC Somatization T score0.36 **0.45 ***0.19
BASC Internalizing Problem Behaviors T score0.57 ***0.70 ***0.40 ***
BASC Functional Impairment Probability T score0.80 ***0.81 ***0.65 ***
Language
ROWPVT SS−0.32 **−0.28 *−0.40 ***
EOWPVT SS−0.31 **−0.27 *−0.32 **
CELF Receptive Language SS−0.38 **−0.35 **−0.44 ***
CELF Expressive Language SS−0.39 ***−0.35 **−0.45 ***
CELF Core Language SS−0.41 ***−0.37 **−0.48 ***
Note: Hypo = hyporesponsiveness aggregate score, Hyper = hyperresponsiveness aggregate score, Seeking = sensory seeking aggregate score, Nonverbal IQ = Nonverbal intelligence as measured by the Leiter International Performance Scale-Third edition [41] or the Test of Nonverbal Intelligence-Fourth edition [40], SRS = Social Responsiveness Scale [45], Core Autism Feature Aggregate = Aggregate of SRS total T score and Social Communication Questionnaire total score [43], SS = Standard score, VABS = Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scales-Second edition [46], BASC = Behavior Assessment System for Children-Third edition [47], ROWPVT = Receptive One Word Picture Vocabulary Test-Fourth edition [48], EOWPVT = Expressive One Word Picture Vocabulary Test-Fourth edition [49], CELF = Clinical Evaluation of Language Fundamentals-Fourth edition [50]. * p < 0.05, ** p < 0.01, *** p < 0.001 after applying a Benjamini–Yekutieli correction [53].
Table 4. Variance in each measure of core and related features of autism explained jointly and uniquely by each pattern of sensory responsiveness
Table 4. Variance in each measure of core and related features of autism explained jointly and uniquely by each pattern of sensory responsiveness
Unique VarianceTotal Variance Explained
HypoHyperSeeking
Core Features of Autism
SRS Social Communication T score8.57% ***4.82% ***0.64%79.91%
SRS RRB T score2.53% **5.28% ***7.06% ***76.65%
Core Autism Feature Aggregate8.60% ***3.93% ***1.86% **83.47%
Related Features of Autism
VABS Communication SS7.85% ***0.93%2.07% *55.73%
VABS Daily Living Skills SS5.99% **2.90% *0.04%47.16%
VABS Socialization SS8.88% ***3.07% **0.48%68.11%
BASC Anxiety T score0.41%19.23% ***0.20%38.52%
BASC Depression T score0.67%9.25% ***1.26%52.12%
BASC Somatization T score0.08%8.03% **0.80%20.88%
BASC Internalizing Problem Behaviors T score0.06%15.52% ***0.00%48.40%
BASC Functional Impairment Probability T score3.74% ***6.57% ***2.68% **75.25%
Language
ROWPVT SS1.01%0.01%2.69%12.58%
EOWPVT SS0.76%0.02%6.22% ***16.85%
CELF Receptive Language SS0.81%0.05%6.42% **21.43%
CELF Expressive Language SS1.21%0.00%6.50% **22.28%
CELF Core Language SS1.04%0.01%8.22% **25.69%
Note: Hypo = hyporesponsiveness aggregate score, Hyper = hyperresponsiveness aggregate score, Seeking = sensory seeking aggregate score, Nonverbal IQ = Nonverbal intelligence as measured by the Leiter International Performance Scale-Third edition [41] or the Test of Nonverbal Intelligence-Fourth edition [40], SRS = Social Responsiveness Scale [45], Core Autism Feature Aggregate = Aggregate of SRS total T score and Social Communication Scale total score [43], SS = Standard score, VABS = Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scales-Second edition [46], BASC = Behavior Assessment System for Children-Third edition [47], ROWPVT = Receptive One Word Picture Vocabulary Test-Fourth edition [48], EOWPVT = Expressive One Word Picture Vocabulary Test-Fourth edition [49], CELF = Clinical Evaluation of Language Fundamentals-Fourth edition [50]. * p < 0.05, ** p < 0.01, *** p < 0.001.
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Feldman, J.I.; Cassidy, M.; Liu, Y.; Kirby, A.V.; Wallace, M.T.; Woynaroski, T.G. Relations between Sensory Responsiveness and Features of Autism in Children. Brain Sci. 2020, 10, 775. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci10110775

AMA Style

Feldman JI, Cassidy M, Liu Y, Kirby AV, Wallace MT, Woynaroski TG. Relations between Sensory Responsiveness and Features of Autism in Children. Brain Sciences. 2020; 10(11):775. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci10110775

Chicago/Turabian Style

Feldman, Jacob I., Margaret Cassidy, Yupeng Liu, Anne V. Kirby, Mark T. Wallace, and Tiffany G. Woynaroski. 2020. "Relations between Sensory Responsiveness and Features of Autism in Children" Brain Sciences 10, no. 11: 775. https://doi.org/10.3390/brainsci10110775

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop