Next Article in Journal
Immune Checkpoint Inhibitor Related Rheumatological Complications: Cooperation between Rheumatologists and Oncologists
Next Article in Special Issue
Neuroticism and Stress in Older Adults: The Buffering Role of Self-Esteem
Previous Article in Journal
Needs and Attitudes of Older Chronic Back Pain Patients towards a Wearable for Ultrasound Biofeedback during Stabilization Exercises: A Qualitative Analysis
Previous Article in Special Issue
Anxiety in the Classroom: Only Girls’ Anxiety Is Related to Same-Sex Peers’ Anxiety
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Work-Related Psychosocial Factors and Global Cognitive Function: Are Telomere Length and Low-Grade Inflammation Potential Mediators of This Association?

by
Caroline S. Duchaine
1,2,3,4,5,
Chantal Brisson
1,2,3,4,
Caroline Diorio
2,3,
Denis Talbot
2,3,
Elizabeth Maunsell
2,3,
Pierre-Hugues Carmichael
1,3,
Yves Giguère
2,3,
Mahée Gilbert-Ouimet
3,6,
Xavier Trudel
1,2,3,4,
Ruth Ndjaboué
7,
Michel Vézina
8,
Alain Milot
1,2,
Benoît Mâsse
9,
Clermont E. Dionne
1,2,3,4,5 and
Danielle Laurin
1,3,4,5,10,*
1
Centre d’excellence sur le vieillissement de Québec (CEVQ), CIUSSS-Capitale Nationale, Québec, QC G1S 4L8, Canada
2
Faculty of Medicine, Université Laval, Québec, QC G1V 0A6, Canada
3
Centre de Recherche du CHU de Québec—Université Laval, Québec, QC G1S 4L8, Canada
4
VITAM, Centre de Recherche en santé Durable, Québec, QC G1S 4L8, Canada
5
Institut sur le Vieillissement et la Participation Sociale des Aînés, Université Laval, Québec, QC G1S 4L8, Canada
6
Canada Research Chair in Sex and Gender in Occupational Health, Université du Québec à Rimouski, Campus de Lévis, Lévis, QC G6V 0A6, Canada
7
School of Social Work, University of Sherbrooke, Sherbrooke, QC J1K 2R1, Canada
8
Institut National de Santé Publique du Québec (INSPQ), Québec, QC G1V 5B3, Canada
9
École de Santé Publique de l’Université de Montréal, Montréal, QC H3N 1X9, Canada
10
Faculty of Pharmacy, Université Laval, Québec, QC G1V 0A6, Canada
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2023, 20(6), 4929; https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph20064929
Submission received: 18 January 2023 / Revised: 5 March 2023 / Accepted: 8 March 2023 / Published: 10 March 2023
(This article belongs to the Special Issue Stress Across the Lifespan in Canada: From Risk to Management)

Abstract

:
The identification of modifiable factors that could maintain cognitive function is a public health priority. It is thought that some work-related psychosocial factors help developing cognitive reserve through high intellectual complexity. However, they also have well-known adverse health effects and are considered to be chronic psychosocial stressors. Indeed, these stressors could increase low-grade inflammation and promote oxidative stress associated with accelerated telomere shortening. Both low-grade inflammation and shorter telomeres have been associated with a cognitive decline. This study aimed to evaluate the total, direct, and indirect effects of work-related psychosocial factors on global cognitive function overall and by sex, through telomere length and an inflammatory index. A random sample of 2219 participants followed over 17 years was included in this study, with blood samples and data with cognitive function drawn from a longitudinal study of 9188 white-collar workers (51% female). Work-related psychosocial factors were evaluated according to the Demand–Control–Support and the Effort–Reward Imbalance (ERI) models. Global cognitive function was evaluated with the validated Montreal Cognitive Assessment (MoCA). Telomere length and inflammatory biomarkers were measured using standardised protocols. The direct and indirect effects were estimated using a novel mediation analysis method developed for multiple correlated mediators. Associations were observed between passive work or low job control, and shorter telomeres among females, and between low social support at work, ERI or iso-strain, and a higher inflammatory index among males. An association was observed with higher cognitive performance for longer telomeres, but not for the inflammatory index. Passive work overall, and low reward were associated with lower cognitive performance in males; whereas, high psychological demand in both males and females and high job strain in females were associated with a higher cognitive performance. However, none of these associations were mediated by telomere length or the inflammatory index. This study suggests that some work-related psychosocial factors could be associated with shorter telomeres and low-grade inflammation, but these associations do not explain the relationship between work-related psychosocial factors and global cognitive function. A better understanding of the biological pathways, by which these factors affect cognitive function, could guide future preventive strategies to maintain cognitive function and promote healthy aging.

1. Introduction

Dementia is emerging as a public health priority due to the aging of the population [1]. In 2020, the Lancet Commission on Dementia Prevention, Intervention, and Care reiterated its position on the relevance of a life-course approach for the identification of modifiable risk factors, targeting those contributing favorably to cognitive reserve [2]. A cognitive reserve refers to the neurobiological maintenance and adaptability of the brain, enabling preservation of cognitive function in everyday activities, despite the presence of brain pathology [3]. Modifiable factors at midlife, such as occupational complexity, frequent social contact, and intellectual and social activities, have been suggested to benefit cognitive reserve [2,3].
The role of work-related psychosocial factors in affecting cognitive function has generated increasing interest in recent years. It has been suggested that these factors could help build cognitive reserve when work involves high intellectual complexity, decision making, creativity, skill discretion and development, and high social interaction with coworkers and supervisors. Systematic reviews and meta-analyses have reported associations between high complexity at work and better cognitive performance [4,5,6], and low risk of dementia [5,6]. However, work-related psychosocial factors that are perceived as stressful may have detrimental health effects [7,8,9]. Chronic psychosocial stressors at work could increase the synthesis of inflammatory biomarkers, including C-reactive protein (CRP) and interleukin-6 (IL-6), which, in turn, could induce low-grade inflammation [10]. This low-grade inflammation promotes the oxidative stress that is involved in the DNA degradation process and telomere shortening [11]. Telomeres are protective and stabilizing structures, made up of repeated non-coding DNA sequences, located at the end of chromosomes [12]. Telomere shortening is part of the natural cellular life cycle, but an increased rate of shortening is associated with premature cellular aging [11]. Both low-grade inflammation and shorter telomeres have been associated with a cognitive decline [13,14,15,16,17,18,19,20] and dementia [21,22,23,24] in longitudinal studies, but these findings have not consistently been replicated [25,26,27,28]. Untangling biological mechanisms by which work-related psychosocial factors affect cognitive function is important to distinguish those that have the potential to increase cognitive reserve from those that could precipitate cognitive decline.
Work-related psychosocial factors have been conceptualized by two recognised theoretical models. According to the Demand–Control–Support (DCS) model [29,30], exposure to high psychological demands, combined with low job control, constitutes high job strain, increasing physiological and psychological stress responses, whereas exposure to low psychological demands, combined with high job control, constitutes a low job strain [29]. Exposure to high psychological demands, combined with high job control, defines active work, which provides high developmental and intellectual opportunities, whereas exposure to low psychological demands, combined with low job control, defines passive work [29]. Exposure to low social support at work from colleagues and supervisors, the third component of this model, can amplify the adverse effect of high job strain on health [30]. Based on the Effort–Reward Imbalance (ERI) model [31], exposure to an imbalance between high efforts spent at work and low economic, social, or organisational rewards obtained in return, is associated with a stress condition that has detrimental health effects. Exposures to high job strain and to ERI have both been associated with an increased risk of poorer health outcomes, such as hypertension [32], cardiovascular diseases [33,34], diabetes [35], and mental health problems [7,8,9].
Longitudinal studies provide evidence linking passive work [36,37,38] and low job control [36,37,38,39,40] to worse global cognitive function. The association of high job strain with lower global cognitive function has been documented [36,39], although inconsistently [37,38,40]. Some longitudinal studies have reported associations between work-related psychosocial factors from the DCS or ERI models and higher concentrations of inflammatory biomarkers [41,42,43]. We previously reported an association between exposure to high job strain, combined with low social support or exposure to ERI, and an inflammatory index, especially among males [44]. To our knowledge, no previous longitudinal study has examined the association of these work-related psychosocial factors with telomere length. Additionally, the mediating effects of telomere length or inflammatory biomarkers have not been examined in previous studies, regarding the relationship between work-related psychosocial factors and cognitive function. Furthermore, sex differences have not been thoroughly examined, even though several sex differences have been reported with respect to exposure to work-related psychosocial factors [45], inflammatory biomarkers [46], telomere length [47], cognitive function [48], decline over time [49], and the prevalence of dementia [50].
This study aimed to evaluate the interplay between work-related psychosocial exposures, global cognitive function, telomere length and inflammatory biomarkers, overall and by sex. Specifically, this study examined the total, direct, and indirect effects of work-related psychosocial factors on global cognitive function through telomere length and an inflammatory index, combining CRP and IL-6 within the PROspective Quebec (PROQ) study on work and health [51].

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Study Design and Data Collection

The PROQ study is a longitudinal occupational cohort study, designed to evaluate the effects of work-related psychosocial factors on cardiovascular and mental health outcomes. The study population at baseline included 9188 white-collar workers (initial participation: 75%; 51% females), recruited in 19 public and semi-public organizations from Quebec City, Canada, 1991–1993 (T1). Two follow-ups were carried out after 8 (T2, 1999–2000) and 24 years (T3, 2015–2018). Methodological details of the study have been described elsewhere [51]. In brief, at each measurement time point, data collection included a self-reported questionnaire, a face-to-face interview, and anthropometric and clinical evaluations. At T3, two new components were added to the main study: one on mental health and the other on biomarkers. Before recontacting participants for T3, one third of all participants at T1 were randomly selected for the study of biomarkers, with the exclusion of 207 participants who had either died between T1 and T2 (n = 117), did not want to be recontacted for the follow-up (n = 77) or were not traceable at T3 (n = 13) (Figure 1). When participants were recontacted at T3, most of them were retired (77%). To ensure sufficient statistical power for the planned analysis, a second random selection was therefore performed, including 50% of participants still working at T3. Finally, 3411 participants were selected for the study of biomarkers. Among those, 959 were not included in the present analysis, because they either died between T2 and T3 (n = 233), were lost to follow-up (n = 82), refused to participate in any part of the T3 data collection (n = 321), declined to participate in the face-to-face interview including the cognitive assessment but granted access to medical data only (n = 218), or completed the self-reported questionnaire only (n = 334). Finally, 4 participants were excluded because of missing data on cognitive function, leaving 2219 participants for the present analysis. All participants, or their respondents, provided written informed consent. The CHU de Québec-Université Laval Research Ethics Board reviewed and approved this study.

2.2. Work-Related Psychosocial Factors

For this study, we used work-related psychosocial factors measured at T2 for baseline exposure, because data from both models were available. Unlike the DCS model, the ERI model was not yet published at T1. Psychological demands (9 items), job control (9 items), and social support at work (11 items) were measured using the validated French adaptation of the Karasek questionnaire [29,30,52,53,54]. Reward was measured using 9 of the 11 items from the validated French adaptation of the Siegrist ERI questionnaire [31,55,56]. Each item was measured on a four-point Likert scale, and scores were calculated for each factor by summing the items. The scores for psychological demands and job control were dichotomised according to the median of a representative sample of Quebec workers [57], and four categories were created: high job strain (high demands and low control), low job strain (low demands and high control), active work (high demands and high control), and passive work (low demands and low control) [29]. To evaluate exposure to high job strain, the reference group comprised the three other categories, and the same procedure was used to evaluate the exposure to passive work. The score of social support at work was dichotomized at the median of the study sample at T2. Exposure to iso-strain was defined by the exposure to high job strain, combined with the exposure to lower-than-median social support at work. The ERI ratio was created by dividing the score of psychological demands, as a proxy of the Siegrist effort scale, by the score of reward. The psychometric qualities of this ERI version have been demonstrated [58]. Participants with a ratio greater than 1 were categorized as exposed to ERI.

2.3. Cognitive Function

Global cognitive function was evaluated with the validated Montreal Cognitive Assessment (MoCA) [59], during a standardised face-to-face interview at T3. The MoCA is a 30-point screening test for mild cognitive impairment or cognitive dysfunction, evaluating visuospatial abilities, executive function, short-term memory recall, attention, concentration, working memory, language and orientation in time and space. The sensitivity, specificity, and positive and negative predictive values of MoCA are about 90% compared to a clinical diagnosis of mild cognitive impairment made by a neurologist [59]. The screening performance for mild cognitive impairment of MoCA is superior to that of the Mini-Mental State Examination and other tests for global cognitive function, justifying its use in cognitively healthy adults [59,60,61].

2.4. Blood Samples

Non-fasting blood samples were taken at T3 by a trained nurse, following a standardised protocol. Venous blood samples were centrifuged within 24 to 72 h, and aliquots of the serum, plasma and buffy coats were kept frozen at minus 80 °C until measurement.

2.5. Telomere Length

Telomere length was measured as described by Ennour-Idrissi et al. [62], with some modifications. DNA was extracted from leucocytes using the Gentra PureGene Cell Kit (QIAGEN Inc., Canada), according to the manufacturer’s protocol. DNA quality and quantity were assessed using the NanoDrop12000c spectrophotometer (Thermo Scientific, Fisher Scientific Canada). Mean relative telomere length was measured using a quantitative polymerase chain reaction (qPCR) method, first described by Cawthon [63], with slight modifications. In short, both the genetic material of interest (telomere) and the single copy gene human beta-globin (Hbg) were amplified in quadruplicates, and the mean value was calculated. A negative control (no DNA template), reference DNA sample for normalisation between the experiments, and two cell line samples (one low-passage for longer telomeres and one high-passage for shorter telomeres) were run in each batch. Standard curves for telomere and Hbg amplifications were done using this same reference DNA sample: the efficiency was 99% and 91%, respectively. The mean cycle threshold (Ct) values were calculated using the three closest values of quadruplicate samples, with exclusion of the fourth value when it fell outside two standard deviations (SD) from the mean [64]. The intra-assay coefficient of variation (CV) of the mean was 1.45% and 1.78%, and the inter-assay CV was 2.80% and 2.04% for telomere and Hbg, respectively. The relative T/S ratio was calculated using this formula: relative T/S ratio = 2 − (mean telomere − mean Hbg) of sample − (mean telomere − mean Hbg) of the reference DNA [65]. All assays were performed blinded to the participants’ characteristics and clinical data. The T/S ratio was logarithmically transformed (base 2) for interpretation purposes, such that an increase of one unit of log2 (ratio T/S) can be interpreted as a two-fold longer telomere.

2.6. Inflammatory Biomarkers

Serum CRP concentrations were obtained by a highly sensitive immunoturbidimetric assay on an automated Modular Roche Diagnostics platform (System ExP Modular Hitachi). The coefficient of variation (CV) for CRP was <5%. Serum concentrations of IL-6 were obtained by ELISA (enzyme-linked immunosorbent assay), according to the maker’s protocol (R&D Systems, Minneapolis, USA), and the fluorescent signal was measured with an automated fluorimeter (Fluoros-Kan, LabSystems). Each sample was analysed in duplicate, and the inter- and intra-batch CVs were 12.7% and 5.3%, respectively. CRP and IL-6 concentrations were log-transformed, because their distributions were right skewed. An inflammatory index was created by summing standardised log-concentrations of CRP and IL-6 [44]. A positive score indicated higher inflammation.

2.7. Covariates

Based on the literature, covariates that might confound or modify the evaluated associations, were identified. Covariates collected at T1 were selected to respect the temporal sequence of events and minimize overadjustment, except for formal education, for which T3 data were used, as this variable was assessed in the numbers of years of schooling during a face-to-face interview. Age, sex, smoking status (never smoked regularly, former regular, current occasional or current regular smokers), alcohol intake (number of drinks per week), physical activity (frequency of 30 min leisure-time physical activity per week), diabetes, cardiovascular diseases (heart diseases, stroke, angina), and social support outside of work (number of confidants, number of helpers, relationship with spouse, and relationship with children) were evaluated using a self-report questionnaire [57]. Height, weight, hip and waist circumferences were measured in-person, using standardised protocols. Body mass index (BMI) (kg/m2) and waist-to-hip ratio were calculated and used as continuous variables. Blood pressure was measured following recognised guidelines [66,67]. Hypertension was defined by either systolic blood pressure >140 mmHg, diastolic blood pressure >90 mmHg [66,67], report of diagnosed hypertension, or use of antihypertensive medication.

2.8. Statistical Analysis

Associations between each work-related psychosocial factor and telomere length or the inflammatory index, and between telomere length or the inflammatory index and cognitive performance were evaluated using linear regression models with a robust variance estimator, using the generalised estimating equations (GEE) method. The potential effect modification of the inflammatory index in the relationship between telomere length and cognitive performance was tested with an interaction term. The exploration of a potential quadratic interaction in this relationship was tested by adding the square of the inflammatory index and an interaction term between the square of the inflammatory index and telomere length. All models were fitted globally and stratified by sex, while adjusting for the covariates previously described. The same procedure was used to estimate the total effect of each work-related psychosocial factor with global cognitive function, i.e., the total effect of these factors on cognitive performance, including direct and indirect effects through any potential mediator.
The Vansteelandt and Daniel method for multiple mediators was used to estimate direct, indirect and residual indirect effects [68]. This method accounts for multiple mediators, even when they are correlated with each other and when the structure of this correlation is unknown. The direct effect represents the association between each work-related psychosocial factor and cognitive performance that is not mediated by the telomere length or the inflammatory index. The indirect effect represents an association between each work-related factor and cognitive performance that is mediated by the telomere length or the inflammatory index, each considered separately. The residual indirect effect represents the association between each work-related factor and cognitive performance that is mediated through the interrelation between the telomere length and the inflammatory index, no matter the structure of this interrelation. This method was chosen given a priori evidence of a strong interrelation between the two mediators [11] that is generally a violation of the default assumptions for standard mediation analysis methods [68]. Standard errors for these estimated direct and indirect effects were obtained by bootstrap with 199 replications.
Multiple imputation (MI), using chained equations [69], was conducted for: (1) missing data in T1 variables among 3618 participants (3411 participants randomly selected for the study of biomarkers, plus 207 participants not included in the random selection); (2) missing data in T2 variables among the 3180 participants that were selected for the study of biomarkers and were either present in T2 data collection (n = 3087), or not present in T2, but present in the in-person data collection at T3 with cognitive data (n = 93); and (3) missing data in T3 variables among the 2219 participants included in the present study. Sixty imputations were computed, because 60% of the study population had at least one missing datum on a variable [69]. Inverse probability of censoring weights (IPCWs) were computed to correct for the differences in the characteristics between included participants and those lost to follow-up between each measurement time. IPCWs were calculated using predicted values obtained from logistic regressions of the probability of being censored between T1 and T2, and between T2 and T3, according to exposure and specific covariates at T1, and at T1 and T2, respectively [70]. Weights were recomputed for each bootstrap sample. Results from each imputed dataset were combined, producing average estimated effects and Wald-type confidence intervals. For more detailed information about the mediation analysis, MI and IPCW methods, their inclusion in the mediation analysis and the predictors used in each model, see Supplementary Methods for statistical analyses.
The procedure to estimate direct and indirect effects was implemented in R version 4.1.3. All other analyses were performed using the SAS software, version 9.4.

3. Results

3.1. Characteristics of the Study Population

Among the 8981 eligible participants for follow-up (Figure 1), those randomly selected for the study of biomarkers (n = 3411) were similar to those not selected (n = 5570) (Supplementary Materials Table S1). Among the participants randomly selected (n = 3411), those included in the present analysis (n = 2219; 51.1% females) were more likely to have a university degree and were less likely to be office workers, regular smokers, or to present with a diagnosis of diabetes or hypertension compared to those not included (n = 1192 (959 + 233 deceased), Figure 1 and Supplementary Materials Table S2).
The mean follow-up time from T2 to T3 was 16.8 (standard deviation: 1.4) years and the mean age of participants at T2 (baseline for exposure) was 46.5 (7.9) years (Table 1). Compared to males, females were slightly younger, less educated and more often exposed to passive work, high job strain, low job control and iso-strain. The exposures to high psychological demands, low social support at work and ERI were similar for males and females. At T3, the mean values for education and the MoCA score were 15.3 (2.9) years completed and 25.6 (2.6), respectively.

3.2. Longitudinal Associations of Work-Related Psychosocial Factors with Telomere Length and Inflammatory Index

Associations were observed between exposure to passive work or low job control and shorter telomeres (β = −0.04, 95% CI −0.08 to −0.00 and β = −0.04, −0.07 to 0.00, respectively, Table 2). These same associations were stronger in females (β = −0.08, 95% CI −0.13 to −0.03 and β = −0.08, −0.13 to −0.02, respectively). No statistically significant associations between other work-related psychosocial factors and telomere length were observed overall or in females, and no statistically significant associations were observed in males. Regarding the inflammatory index, statistically significant associations were observed between low social support at work, ERI or iso-strain, and a higher inflammatory index among males only (β = 0.17, 95% CI 0.03 to 0.31, β = 0.14, 0.00 to 0.29, and β = 0.32, 0.10 to 0.55, respectively). No statistically significant associations with other work-related psychosocial factors and the inflammatory index were observed in males, and no statistically significant associations were observed overall or in females.

3.3. Cross-Sectional (T3) Associations of Telomere Length and Inflammatory Index with Cognitive Function

An association was observed between longer telomeres and higher cognitive performance (β = 0.26, 95% CI −0.00 to 0.52, p = 0.0527, Table 3). This association was stronger in males (β = 0.50, 95% CI 0.15 to 0.86). No statistically significant associations were observed between the inflammatory index and cognitive function, overall or by sex. Exploration of the effect modification of the inflammatory index on the association between telomeres and cognitive function showed a quadratic interaction and a U-shaped modifying association (Supplementary Materials Figure S1). The association between telomere length and cognitive performance was positive for participants with a low and high inflammatory index, but null or negative for middle values of the inflammatory index.

3.4. Mediating Effects of Telomere Length and Inflammatory Index

Overall, total and direct associations were observed between passive work and a lower cognitive performance (βtotal = −0.30, 95% CI −0.55 to −0.06, and βdirect = −0.29, −0.54 to −0.04, Table 4), and between high psychological demands and higher cognitive performance (βtotal = 0.23, 95% CI 0.03 to 0.44, and βdirect = 0.23, 0.02 to 0.43). In stratified analyses, these associations were driven by male participants (βtotal = −0.40, 95% CI −0.78 to −0.02, and βdirect = −0.40, 0.78 to −0.03 for passive work; βtotal = 0.38, 95% CI 0.09 to 0.67, and βdirect = 0.39, 0.09 to 0.68 for high demands). Total and direct associations were also observed between high job strain and higher cognitive performance in females (βtotal and direct = 0.34, 95% CI 0.01 to 0.68), and between low reward and lower cognitive performance in males (βtotal = −0.35, 95% CI −0.69 to −0.01 and βdirect = −0.32, −0.67 to 0.03). None of these associations were mediated by telomere length or the inflammatory index. No statistically significant total effect or direct effect were observed with low job control, low social support at work, ERI, or iso-strain. No statistically significant indirect or residual effects were detected overall, or by sex. Figure 2 illustrates an example of the total, direct, indirect, and residual effects through telomere length and inflammatory index, between passive work and cognitive function.

4. Discussion

4.1. Interpretation of Results

In this 17-year longitudinal study of more than 2000 white-collar workers, it was found that passive work in the study sample and low reward in males were associated with a poorer cognitive performance, whereas high psychological demands in the study sample and high job strain in females were associated with a better cognitive performance. However, none of these associations were mediated by telomere length or the inflammatory index. To our knowledge, this is the first study evaluating the indirect effects of work-related psychosocial factors on global cognitive function through biological mechanisms, including telomere length and low-grade inflammation. This is also the first longitudinal study evaluating the association between work-related psychosocial factors and telomere length.
Only two studies [41,71] had previously evaluated the possible mediating effects of inflammatory biomarkers on the relationship between work-related psychosocial factors and other health outcomes associated with cognitive function [2,72]. The first study was conducted among a subsample of 2101 white-collar workers from the Whitehall II study, aged 50 years on average at the time of exposure measurement, and followed for 10 years [41]. This study reported a weak indirect effect for the association between low social support at work and diabetes among females though IL-6 plasma concentration. The second was a cross-sectional study conducted among a small sample of 204 young male adult workers from veterinary, agricultural, textile or poultry industries in Jordan [71]. This study found that the association between exposure to ERI and the metabolic syndrome was mediated by CRP serum concentrations.
The fact that we found no indirect effects through these biomarkers suggests that other biological pathways may be involved in the association between work-related psychosocial factors and cognitive function. The associations found with exposure to passive work and poorer cognitive performance, or with exposure to high psychological demand and better cognitive performance, are aligned with the cognitive reserve theory [73]. A higher level of education, as well as activities with greater intellectual complexity, or employment that requires more complex skills, may promote the development and the efficiency of the brain, enhancing cognitive reserve and protecting against premature cognitive decline [74]. Passive work is characterized by work with a low cognitive stimulation, in terms of low psychological demands, repetitive work, and low skill utilization and development (i.e., low job control), and is thus less favorable for the preservation of cognitive reserve. A systematic review with meta-analysis reported that exposure to high complexity at work was associated with a lower risk of dementia compared to low complexity at work, exposure to passive work or job strain was associated with a faster cognitive decline compared to active work, and that exposure to high psychological demands was associated with a slower cognitive decline compared to low psychological demands [6]. Moreover, longitudinal studies that have evaluated the association between exposure to passive or active work and cognitive function or dementia, found similar associations as those observed in the present study. In these studies, associations were observed with exposure to passive work and lower global cognitive function [36], lower performance on neuropsychological tests [75,76], or higher risk of dementia [77,78]. Two longitudinal studies also found protective associations between active work and global cognitive function [37] or dementia [79]. Negative findings were reported in two longitudinal studies for the association between passive work and global cognitive function [39] or annual decline of neuropsychological scores [80]. For the most part, these results support the cognitive reserve theory as a plausible explanation for the effect of work-related psychosocial factors on long-term cognitive function.
Although mediation effects were not supported in the current study, associations were observed between specific work-related psychosocial factors and shorter telomeres, especially among females, along with a higher inflammatory index, especially among males, suggesting that these factors can increase systemic low-grade inflammation and oxidation. Only two previous cross-sectional studies have examined the association between these factors and telomere length and their results are difficult to compare to ours, because neither examined potential sex differences [81,82]. The first study was conducted among a sample of 435 workers, aged 61 years on average, from the Multiethnic Study of Atherosclerosis (MESA) study [81]. No association was found between exposure to high job strain, active work, passive work, high psychological demands or low job control evaluated (with a validated questionnaire and a job exposure matrix) and telomere length measured with the T/S ratio. The second study was conducted among a sample of 141 employees working in geriatric care units in Germany, aged 44 years on average [82]. No association was found between exposure to high psychological demands, low job control or low social support at work (evaluated with a validated questionnaire) and telomere length (expressed as relative length compared to albumin DNA). In our study, no association was found between high job strain, high psychological demands, and low social support at work, and telomere length. However, exposure to passive work or low job control was associated with shorter telomeres, especially among females.
The current findings suggest that some associations between exposures and outcomes varies according to biological sex. Females tend to be more exposed to psychosocial stressors at work [45] and have longer telomeres [47]. Moreover, sex differences have been suggested in the effect of chronic stress on the inflammatory and oxidative response to these exposures [46]. In another sample from the MESA study conducted among 1029 participants, sex differences were observed in the association between chronic stress exposure and telomere shortening, using the T/S ratio [83]. Similar to our findings here, the association between greater chronic stress and telomere shortening was stronger among females than males [83]. These results suggested that some differences between males and females could be present, but the body of knowledge is still insufficient to draw solid conclusions.
This study is the first to observe a U-shaped modifying effect of inflammatory biomarkers in the association between telomere length and global cognitive function. One previous longitudinal study conducted among a sample of 497 young adult residents of Jerusalem evaluated the modifying effect of an inflammatory index, combining CRP, fibrinogen and a number of leukocytes on the association between telomere shortening over 5 to 10 years, and global cognitive function evaluated with a neurocognitive test battery [84]. The authors reported an association between faster telomere shortening and poorer cognitive function, but the association was not mediated by inflammation. Nevertheless, considering that telomere shortening and low-grade inflammation are biologically interrelated [11], this effect modification needs to be explored in future studies.

4.2. Strengths and Limitations

This study has several strengths, including a large sample size, with similar number of males and females for the evaluation of sex differences, and a longitudinal design. Work-related psychosocial factor exposure was measured with validated instruments and well before the assessment of cognitive function and biomarkers. Rigorous and innovative statistical methods were employed to evaluate the indirect effects of telomeres and inflammation in the presence of correlation and interaction between these mediators, and to correct for the potential selection bias. Moreover, several potential confounders were measured before exposure assessment and were controlled for in statistical models, which minimized the potential of overadjustment by mediators.
This study also has limitations. First, telomere length, inflammatory biomarkers and global cognitive function were all measured at the end of follow-up. Thus, causal inference about the associations observed between these variables cannot be made. Variation in telomere length and in inflammatory biomarkers can either be a cause or a consequence of the variation in global cognitive function. However, as several previous longitudinal studies have observed associations between inflammatory biomarkers or telomere length and cognitive function [13,14,15,16,17,18,19,20] or dementia [21,22,23,24], a causal hypothesis is supported by a priori evidence. Second, cognitive function was not evaluated at the beginning of the study, thus it was not possible to control for cognitive performance at baseline and change in cognition over time could not be determined. However, the fact that participants were relatively young, with only 4% aged 65 years or older, and that 90% were still working at T2, does not favor the presence of cognitive impairment at T2, although it cannot be completely ruled out. Third, participants may have changed jobs and working conditions during their career over the 17-year follow-up. We do not have this information and misclassification of the exposure over time is possible. However, due to the good working conditions generally offered to white-collar workers, our study population tended to maintain their occupational position over time [44]. Nevertheless, the possibility that exposed participants may have been more likely to quit or change their job cannot be excluded. This would result in a potential selection bias, that generally tends to underestimate the associations. Fourth, the healthy worker effect could be present, as healthier workers and those less exposed to work-related psychosocial factors tend to stay longer in the labour force [85,86], and if so, could lead to an underestimation of the observed associations [85,86,87]. While statistical analyses partially controlled for the healthy worker effect during the course of the follow-up, using IPCW for death and loss to follow-up, underestimation remains possible [87]. Fifth, multiple testing may have increased the risk of false discovery, although all comparisons were determined a priori and were scientifically justified. Finally, the study population was composed of white-collar workers and mostly Caucasians, and thus may not be representative of the entire workforce population. Caution should be exercised in generalizing results. However, participants held a wide variety of jobs, such as office workers, technicians, professionals, and managers, ensuring exposure diversity, while controlling for the physicochemical exposures present in blue-collar workers. White-collar workers constitute the largest segment of the Canadian workforce, a fact which contributes to increasing the scope of the results.

5. Conclusions

This study suggests that some work-related psychosocial factors could be associated with shorter telomeres and low-grade inflammation, but these associations do not explain the relationship between work-related psychosocial factors and global cognitive function. Further research on the biological pathways by which these factors affect cognitive function could help identify which specific work-related psychosocial factors with positive effects could be targeted for enhancement, with the potential of increasing cognitive reserve. Similarly, factors with negative effects could be targeted for reduction, in order to prevent systemic low-grade inflammation, oxidation, and cellular aging. Interventions already exist to reduce occupational exposures. Work-related psychosocial factors, along with other modifiable lifestyle risk factors, including physical activity, diet, and vascular health, could be part of future prevention strategies aimed at maintaining cognitive function and promoting healthy aging.

Supplementary Materials

The following supporting information can be downloaded at: https://www.mdpi.com/article/10.3390/ijerph20064929/s1, Figure S1: Association between telomere length and cognitive function according to inflammatory index; Table S1: Characteristics at T1 of participants randomly selected for blood sample (n = 3411) and of those not selected (n = 5570); Table S2: Comparison of the characteristicsa between included and censored participants at T1b.

Author Contributions

C.B. and D.L. were responsible of the conceptualization, investigation, resources, project administration, and the funding acquisition. C.S.D. further contributed to the conceptualization, formal analysis, interpretation the data, and wrote the original draft of the manuscript as part of her doctoral thesis under the supervision of D.L. and C.B. D.T. supervised the formal data analysis and P.-H.C. contributed to the statistical analysis of the mediator effect. All authors made substantial contributions to the conceptualization, investigation, and interpretation of data. All authors reviewed and edited the draft of the article and gave the final approval. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This work was supported by the Canadian Institutes of Health Research (Grant numbers 201403MOP-325244-BCA-CFBA-52569 to DL and CB, and 201810GSD-422016-DRB-CFBA-280487 to CSD) and by the Fonds de Recherche du Québec—Santé (Grant numbers 35500 to CSD and 265385 to DT). Funders were not involved in the study design, in the collection, analysis and interpretation of data, in the writing of the report, and in the decision to submit the article for publication.

Institutional Review Board Statement

The study was conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki, and approved by the Ethics Committee of CHU de Québec-Université Laval (#2015-2300, 2015-05-27) for studies involving humans.

Informed Consent Statement

Informed consent was obtained from all subjects involved in the study.

Data Availability Statement

The data underlying this article cannot be shared publicly because of privacy concerns relative to the individuals who participated in the study. However, data will be shared following a justified request to the corresponding author, conditional on permission from the CHU de Québec-Université Laval Research Ethics Board.

Acknowledgments

The authors wish to thank Manon Levesque for the coordination of the second follow-up of this large-scale cohort study, Annick Michaud for her implication in the telomere length measurement, Nathalie Bernard for her implication in the IL-6 concentrations measurement, and all study participants and participating workplaces.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

  1. Alzheimer’s Disease International. From Plan to Impact IV: Progress towards Targets of the WHO Global Action Plan on Dementia; Alzheimer’s Disease International: London, UK, 2021. [Google Scholar]
  2. Livingston, G.; Huntley, J.; Sommerlad, A.; Ames, D.; Ballard, C.; Banerjee, S.; Brayne, C.; Burns, A.; Cohen-Mansfield, J.; Cooper, C.; et al. Dementia prevention, intervention, and care: 2020 report of the Lancet Commission. Lancet 2020, 396, 413–446. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  3. Stern, Y.; Arenaza-Urquijo, E.M.; Bartrés-Faz, D.; Belleville, S.; Cantilon, M.; Chetelat, G.; Ewers, M.; Franzmeier, N.; Kempermann, G.; Kremen, W.S.; et al. Whitepaper: Defining and investigating cognitive reserve, brain reserve, and brain maintenance. Alzheimer’s Dement. J. Alzheimer’s Assoc. 2020, 16, 1305–1311. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  4. Nexo, M.A.; Meng, A.; Borg, V. Can psychosocial work conditions protect against age-related cognitive decline? Results from a systematic review. Occup. Environ. Med. 2016, 73, 487–496. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  5. Then, F.S.; Luck, T.; Luppa, M.; Thinschmidt, M.; Deckert, S.; Nieuwenhuijsen, K.; Seidler, A.; Riedel-Heller, S.G. Systematic review of the effect of the psychosocial working environment on cognition and dementia. Occup. Environ. Med. 2014, 71, 358–365. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  6. Huang, L.Y.; Hu, H.Y.; Wang, Z.T.; Ma, Y.H.; Dong, Q.; Tan, L.; Yu, J.T. Association of Occupational Factors and Dementia or Cognitive Impairment: A Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis. J. Alzheimers Dis. 2020, 78, 217–227. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  7. Rugulies, R.; Aust, B.; Madsen, I.E. Effort-reward imbalance at work and risk of depressive disorders. A systematic review and meta-analysis of prospective cohort studies. Scand. J. Work Environ. Health 2017, 43, 294–306. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  8. Duchaine, C.S.; Aubé, K.; Gilbert-Ouimet, M.; Vézina, M.; Ndjaboué, R.; Massamba, V.; Talbot, D.; Lavigne-Robichaud, M.; Trudel, X.; Pena-Gralle, A.B.; et al. Psychosocial Stressors at Work and the Risk of Sickness Absence Due to a Diagnosed Mental Disorder: A Systematic Review and Meta-analysis. JAMA Psychiatry 2020, 77, 842–851. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  9. Madsen, I.E.H.; Nyberg, S.T.; Magnusson Hanson, L.L.; Ferrie, J.E.; Ahola, K.; Alfredsson, L.; Batty, G.D.; Bjorner, J.B.; Borritz, M.; Burr, H.; et al. Job strain as a risk factor for clinical depression: Systematic review and meta-analysis with additional individual participant data. Psychol. Med. 2017, 47, 1342–1356. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  10. Straub, R.H.; Cutolo, M. Psychoneuroimmunology-developments in stress research. Wien. Med. Wochenschr. 2018, 168, 76–84. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  11. Zhang, J.; Rane, G.; Dai, X.; Shanmugam, M.K.; Arfuso, F.; Samy, R.P.; Lai, M.K.; Kappei, D.; Kumar, A.P.; Sethi, G. Ageing and the telomere connection: An intimate relationship with inflammation. Ageing Res. Rev. 2016, 25, 55–69. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Blackburn, E.H. Telomere states and cell fates. Nature 2000, 408, 53–56. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  13. Devore, E.E.; Prescott, J.; De Vivo, I.; Grodstein, F. Relative telomere length and cognitive decline in the Nurses’ Health Study. Neurosci. Lett. 2011, 492, 15–18. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  14. Yaffe, K.; Lindquist, K.; Kluse, M.; Cawthon, R.; Harris, T.; Hsueh, W.C.; Simonsick, E.M.; Kuller, L.; Li, R.; Ayonayon, H.N.; et al. Telomere length and cognitive function in community-dwelling elders: Findings from the Health ABC Study. Neurobiol. Aging 2011, 32, 2055–2060. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  15. Pudas, S.; Josefsson, M.; Nordin Adolfsson, A.; Landfors, M.; Kauppi, K.; Veng-Taasti, L.M.; Hultdin, M.; Adolfsson, R.; Degerman, S. Short leukocyte telomeres, but not telomere attrition rates, predict memory decline in the 20-year longitudinal Betula study. J. Gerontol. A Biol. Sci. Med. Sci. 2020, 76, 955–963. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  16. West, N.A.; Kullo, I.J.; Morris, M.C.; Mosley, T.H. Sex-specific associations of inflammation markers with cognitive decline. Exp. Gerontol. 2020, 138, 110986. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  17. Lindbergh, C.A.; Casaletto, K.B.; Staffaroni, A.M.; Elahi, F.; Walters, S.M.; You, M.; Neuhaus, J.; Rivera Contreras, W.; Wang, P.; Karydas, A.; et al. Systemic Tumor Necrosis Factor-Alpha Trajectories Relate to Brain Health in Typically Aging Older Adults. J. Gerontol. A Biol. Sci. Med. Sci. 2020, 75, 1558–1565. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. Walker, K.A.; Gottesman, R.F.; Wu, A.; Knopman, D.S.; Gross, A.L.; Mosley, T.H., Jr.; Selvin, E.; Windham, B.G. Systemic inflammation during midlife and cognitive change over 20 years: The ARIC Study. Neurology 2019, 92, e1256–e1267. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
  19. Zheng, F.; Xie, W. High-sensitivity C-reactive protein and cognitive decline: The English Longitudinal Study of Ageing. Psychol. Med. 2018, 48, 1381–1389. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Fiocco, A.J.; Krieger, L.; D’Amico, D.; Parrott, M.D.; Laurin, D.; Gaudreau, P.; Greenwood, C.; Ferland, G. A systematic review of existing peripheral biomarkers of cognitive aging: Is there enough evidence for biomarker proxies in behavioral modification interventions? An initiative in association with the nutrition, exercise and lifestyle team of the Canadian Consortium on Neurodegeneration in Aging. Ageing Res. Rev. 2019, 52, 72–119. [Google Scholar]
  21. Kuo, H.K.; Yen, C.J.; Chang, C.H.; Kuo, C.K.; Chen, J.H.; Sorond, F. Relation of C-reactive protein to stroke, cognitive disorders, and depression in the general population: Systematic review and meta-analysis. Lancet Neurol. 2005, 4, 371–380. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Koyama, A.; O’Brien, J.; Weuve, J.; Blacker, D.; Metti, A.L.; Yaffe, K. The role of peripheral inflammatory markers in dementia and Alzheimer’s disease: A meta-analysis. J. Gerontol. A Biol. Sci. Med. Sci. 2013, 68, 433–440. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  23. Dziedzic, T. Systemic inflammatory markers and risk of dementia. Am. J. Alzheimers Dis. Other Demen. 2006, 21, 258–262. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  24. Fuchs, T.; Trollor, J.N.; Crawford, J.; Brown, D.A.; Baune, B.T.; Samaras, K.; Campbell, L.; Breit, S.N.; Brodaty, H.; Sachdev, P.; et al. Macrophage inhibitory cytokine-1 is associated with cognitive impairment and predicts cognitive decline–the Sydney Memory and Aging Study. Aging Cell 2013, 12, 882–889. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  25. Zhan, Y.; Clements, M.S.; Roberts, R.O.; Vassilaki, M.; Druliner, B.R.; Boardman, L.A.; Petersen, R.C.; Reynolds, C.A.; Pedersen, N.L.; Hagg, S. Association of telomere length with general cognitive trajectories: A meta-analysis of four prospective cohort studies. Neurobiol. Aging 2018, 69, 111–116. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  26. Colicino, E.; Wilson, A.; Frisardi, M.C.; Prada, D.; Power, M.C.; Hoxha, M.; Dioni, L.; Spiro, A.; Vokonas, P.S.; Weisskopf, M.G.; et al. Telomere Length, Long-Term Black Carbon Exposure, and Cognitive Function in a Cohort of Older Men: The VA Normative Aging Study. Environ. Health Perspect. 2017, 125, 76–81. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  27. Harris, S.E.; Marioni, R.E.; Martin-Ruiz, C.; Pattie, A.; Gow, A.J.; Cox, S.R.; Corley, J.; von Zglinicki, T.; Starr, J.M.; Deary, I.J. Longitudinal telomere length shortening and cognitive and physical decline in later life: The Lothian Birth Cohorts 1936 and 1921. Mech. Ageing Dev. 2016, 154, 43–48. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Legdeur, N.; Heymans, M.W.; Comijs, H.C.; Huisman, M.; Maier, A.B.; Visser, P.J. Age dependency of risk factors for cognitive decline. BMC Geriatr. 2018, 18, 187. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  29. Karasek, R. Job-demands, job decision latitude, and mental strain: Implications for job redesign. Adm. Sci. Q. 1979, 24, 285–308. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Johnson, J.V. Theoretical developments in psychosocial work environment research. Int. J. Health Serv. 1989, 19, 457–458. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  31. Siegrist, J. Adverse health effects of high-effort/low-reward conditions. J. Occup. Health Psychol. 1996, 1, 27–41. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  32. Gilbert-Ouimet, M.; Trudel, X.; Brisson, C.; Milot, A.; Vézina, M. Adverse effects of psychosocial work factors on blood pressure: Systematic review of studies on demand-control-support and effort-reward imbalance models. Scand. J. Work Environ. Health 2014, 40, 109–132. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  33. Sara, J.D.; Prasad, M.; Eleid, M.F.; Zhang, M.; Widmer, R.J.; Lerman, A. Association Between Work-Related Stress and Coronary Heart Disease: A Review of Prospective Studies Through the Job Strain, Effort-Reward Balance, and Organizational Justice Models. J. Am. Heart Assoc. 2018, 7, e008073. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  34. Huang, Y.; Xu, S.; Hua, J.; Zhu, D.; Liu, C.; Hu, Y.; Liu, T.; Xu, D. Association between job strain and risk of incident stroke: A meta-analysis. Neurology 2015, 85, 1648–1654. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  35. Pena-Gralle, A.P.B.; Talbot, D.; Duchaine, C.S.; Lavigne-Robichaud, M.; Trudel, X.; Aubé, K.; Gralle, M.; Gilbert-Ouimet, M.; Milot, A.; Brisson, C. Job strain and effort-reward imbalance as risk factors for type 2 diabetes mellitus: A systematic review and meta-analysis of prospective studies. Scand. J. Work Environ. Health 2022, 48, 5–20. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  36. Pan, K.Y.; Xu, W.; Mangialasche, F.; Dekhtyar, S.; Fratiglioni, L.; Wang, H.X. Working Life Psychosocial Conditions in Relation to Late-Life Cognitive Decline: A Population-Based Cohort Study. J. Alzheimers Dis. 2019, 67, 315–325. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  37. Andel, R.; Crowe, M.; Kareholt, I.; Wastesson, J.; Parker, M.G. Indicators of job strain at midlife and cognitive functioning in advanced old age. J. Gerontol. B Psychol. Sci. Soc. Sci. 2011, 66, 287–291. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  38. Duchaine, C.S.; Brisson, C.; Talbot, D.; Gilbert-Ouimet, M.; Trudel, X.; Vézina, M.; Milot, A.; Diorio, C.; Ndjaboué, R.; Giguère, Y.; et al. Cumulative exposure to psychosocial stressors at work and global cognitive function: The PROspective Quebec Study on Work and Health. Occup. Environ. Med. 2021, 78, 884–892. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  39. Dong, L.; Eaton, W.W.; Spira, A.P.; Agnew, J.; Surkan, P.J.; Mojtabai, R. Job strain and cognitive change: The Baltimore Epidemiologic Catchment Area follow-up study. Occup. Environ. Med. 2018, 75, 856–862. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  40. Nilsen, C.; Andel, R.; Fors, S.; Meinow, B.; Darin Mattsson, A.; Kareholt, I. Associations between work-related stress in late midlife, educational attainment, and serious health problems in old age: A longitudinal study with over 20 years of follow-up. BMC Public Health 2014, 14, 878. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  41. Magnusson Hanson, L.L.; Virtanen, M.; Rod, N.H.; Steptoe, A.; Head, J.; David Batty, G.; Kivimaki, M.; Westerlund, H. Does inflammation provide a link between psychosocial work characteristics and diabetes? Analysis of the role of interleukin-6 and C-reactive protein in the Whitehall II cohort study. Brain Behav. Immun. 2019, 78, 153–160. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  42. Shirom, A.; Toker, S.; Berliner, S.; Shapira, I. The Job Demand-Control-Support Model and stress-related low-grade inflammatory responses among healthy employees: A longitudinal study. Work Stress 2008, 22, 138–152. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  43. Coronado, J.I.C.; Chandola, T.; Steptoe, A. Allostatic Load and Effort-Reward Imbalance: Associations over the Working-Career. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2018, 15, 191. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  44. Duchaine, C.S.; Brisson, C.; Talbot, D.; Gilbert-Ouimet, M.; Trudel, X.; Vézina, M.; Milot, A.; Diorio, C.; Ndjaboué, R.; Giguère, Y.; et al. Psychosocial stressors at work and inflammatory biomarkers: PROspective Quebec Study on Work and Health. Psychoneuroendocrinology 2021, 133, 105400. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  45. Hoven, H.; Siegrist, J. Work characteristics, socioeconomic position and health: A systematic review of mediation and moderation effects in prospective studies. Occup. Environ. Med. 2013, 70, 663–669. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  46. Bekhbat, M.; Neigh, G.N. Sex differences in the neuro-immune consequences of stress: Focus on depression and anxiety. Brain Behav. Immun. 2018, 67, 1–12. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  47. Gardner, M.; Bann, D.; Wiley, L.; Cooper, R.; Hardy, R.; Nitsch, D.; Martin-Ruiz, C.; Shiels, P.; Sayer, A.A.; Barbieri, M.; et al. Gender and telomere length: Systematic review and meta-analysis. Exp. Gerontol. 2014, 51, 15–27. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  48. Li, R.; Singh, M. Sex differences in cognitive impairment and Alzheimer’s disease. Front. Neuroendocrinol. 2014, 35, 385–403. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  49. Levine, D.A.; Gross, A.L.; Briceño, E.M.; Tilton, N.; Giordani, B.J.; Sussman, J.B.; Hayward, R.A.; Burke, J.F.; Hingtgen, S.; Elkind, M.S.V.; et al. Sex Differences in Cognitive Decline Among US Adults. JAMA Netw. Open 2021, 4, e210169. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  50. Alzheimer’s Association. 2022 Alzheimer’s disease facts and figures. Alzheimer’s Dement. J. Alzheimer’s Assoc. 2022, 18, 700–789. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  51. Trudel, X.; Gilbert-Ouimet, M.; Milot, A.; Duchaine, C.S.; Vézina, M.; Laurin, D.; Sultan-Taieb, H.; Brisson, C. Cohort Profile: The PROspective Quebec (PROQ) Study on Work and Health. Int. J. Epidemiol. 2018, 47, 693–693i. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  52. Brisson, C.; Blanchette, C.; Guimont, C.; Dion, J.; Moisan, J.; Vézina, M. Reliability and Validity of the French Version of the 18-item Karasek Job Content Questionnaire. Work Stress 1998, 12, 322–336. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  53. Karasek, R.; Brisson, C.; Kawakami, N.; Houtman, I.; Bongers, P.; Amick, B. The Job Content Questionnaire (JCQ): An instrument for internationally comparative assessments of psychosocial job characteristics. J. Occup. Health Psychol. 1998, 3, 322–355. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  54. Niedhammer, I. Psychometric properties of the French version of the Karasek Job Content Questionnaire: A study of the scales of decision latitude, psychological demands, social support, and physical demands in the GAZEL cohort. Int. Arch. Occup. Environ. Health 2002, 75, 129–144. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  55. Niedhammer, I.; Siegrist, J.; Landre, M.F.; Goldberg, M.; Leclerc, A. Psychometric properties of the French version of the Effort-Reward Imbalance model. Rev. Epidemiol. Sante Pub. 2000, 48, 419–437. [Google Scholar]
  56. Siegrist, J.; Starke, D.; Chandola, T.; Godin, I.; Marmot, M.; Niedhammer, I.; Peter, R. The measurement of effort-reward imbalance at work: European comparisons. Soc. Sci. Med. 2004, 58, 1483–1499. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  57. Santé Québec. Et votre coeur, ça va? In Rapport de L’enquête Québécoise sur la Santé Cardiovasculaire; Gouvernement du Québec: Québec, QC, Canada, 1990. [Google Scholar]
  58. Aboa-Éboulé, C.; Brisson, C.; Blanchette, C.; Maunsell, E.; Bourbonnais, R.; Abdous, B.; Vézina, M.; Milot, A.; Dagenais, G.R. Effort-reward imbalance at work and psychological distress: A validation study of post-myocardial infarction patients. Psychosom Med. 2011, 73, 448–455. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  59. Nasreddine, Z.S.; Phillips, N.A.; Bedirian, V.; Charbonneau, S.; Whitehead, V.; Collin, I.; Cummings, J.L.; Chertkow, H. The Montreal Cognitive Assessment, MoCA: A brief screening tool for mild cognitive impairment. J. Am. Geriatr. Soc. 2005, 53, 695–699. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  60. Dong, Y.; Lee, W.Y.; Basri, N.A.; Collinson, S.L.; Merchant, R.A.; Venketasubramanian, N.; Chen, C.L. The Montreal Cognitive Assessment is superior to the Mini-Mental State Examination in detecting patients at higher risk of dementia. Int. Psychogeriatr. 2012, 24, 1749–1755. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  61. Pinto, T.C.C.; Machado, L.; Bulgacov, T.M.; Rodrigues-Junior, A.L.; Costa, M.L.G.; Ximenes, R.C.C.; Sougey, E.B. Is the Montreal Cognitive Assessment (MoCA) screening superior to the Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE) in the detection of mild cognitive impairment (MCI) and Alzheimer’s Disease (AD) in the elderly? Int. Psychogeriatr. 2018, 31, 491–504. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  62. Ennour-Idrissi, K.; Tetu, B.; Maunsell, E.; Poirier, B.; Montoni, A.; Rochette, P.J.; Diorio, C. Association of Telomere Length with Breast Cancer Prognostic Factors. PLoS ONE 2016, 11, e0161903. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  63. Cawthon, R.M. Telomere length measurement by a novel monochrome multiplex quantitative PCR method. Nucleic Acids Res. 2009, 37, e21. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  64. Yang, I.V.; Chen, E.; Hasseman, J.P.; Liang, W.; Frank, B.C.; Wang, S.; Sharov, V.; Saeed, A.I.; White, J.; Li, J.; et al. Within the fold: Assessing differential expression measures and reproducibility in microarray assays. Genome Biol. 2002, 3, research0062. [Google Scholar] [PubMed]
  65. Livak, K.J.; Schmittgen, T.D. Analysis of relative gene expression data using real-time quantitative PCR and the 2(-Delta Delta C(T)) Method. Methods 2001, 25, 402–408. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  66. The Joint National Committee on Prevention Detection Evaluation and Treatment of High Blood Pressure. The Sixth Report of the Joint National Committee on Prevention, Detection, Evaluation, and Treatment of High Blood Pressure. Arch. Intern. Med. 1997, 157, 2413–2446.
  67. Strugo, V.; Glew, F.J.; Davis, J.; Opie, L.H. Update: Recommendations for human blood pressure determination by sphygmomanometers. Hypertension 1990, 16, 594. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  68. Vansteelandt, S.; Daniel, R.M. Interventional Effects for Mediation Analysis with Multiple Mediators. Epidemiology 2017, 28, 258–265. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  69. White, I.R.; Royston, P.; Wood, A.M. Multiple imputation using chained equations: Issues and guidance for practice. Stat. Med. 2011, 30, 377–399. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  70. Hernán, M.A.; Robins, J.M. Causal Inference: What If. 2020. Available online: https://cdn1.sph.harvard.edu/wp-content/uploads/sites/1268/2020/02/ci_hernanrobins_21feb20.pdf (accessed on 12 October 2022).
  71. Almadi, T.; Cathers, I.; Chow, C.M. Associations among work-related stress, cortisol, inflammation, and metabolic syndrome. Psychophysiology 2013, 50, 821–830. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  72. Misiak, B.; Leszek, J.; Kiejna, A. Metabolic syndrome, mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimer’s disease—The emerging role of systemic low-grade inflammation and adiposity. Brain Res. Bull. 2012, 89, 144–149. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  73. Stern, Y. What is cognitive reserve? Theory and research application of the reserve concept. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 2002, 8, 448–460. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  74. Stern, Y. Cognitive reserve in ageing and Alzheimer’s disease. Lancet Neurol. 2012, 11, 1006–1012. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  75. Zhuo, L.B.; Pei, J.J.; Yan, Z.; Yao, W.; Hao, C.F.; Wang, H.X. Working life job strain status and cognitive aging in Europe: A 12-year follow-up study. J. Affect. Disord. 2021, 295, 1177–1183. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  76. Sabbath, E.L.; Andel, R.; Zins, M.; Goldberg, M.; Berr, C. Domains of cognitive function in early old age: Which ones are predicted by pre-retirement psychosocial work characteristics? Occup. Environ. Med. 2016, 73, 640–647. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  77. Pan, K.Y.; Xu, W.; Mangialasche, F.; Grande, G.; Fratiglioni, L.; Wang, H.X. The role of Apolipoprotein E epsilon4 in the association between psychosocial working conditions and dementia. Aging 2020, 12, 3730–3746. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  78. Wang, H.X.; Wahlberg, M.; Karp, A.; Winblad, B.; Fratiglioni, L. Psychosocial stress at work is associated with increased dementia risk in late life. Alzheimer’s Dement. J. Alzheimer’s Assoc. 2012, 8, 114–120. [Google Scholar]
  79. Kivimäki, M.; Walker, K.A.; Pentti, J.; Nyberg, S.T.; Mars, N.; Vahtera, J.; Suominen, S.B.; Lallukka, T.; Rahkonen, O.; Pietiläinen, O.; et al. Cognitive stimulation in the workplace, plasma proteins, and risk of dementia: Three analyses of population cohort studies. BMJ 2021, 374, n1804. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  80. Agbenyikey, W.; Karasek, R.; Cifuentes, M.; Wolf, P.A.; Seshadri, S.; Taylor, J.A.; Beiser, A.S.; Au, R. Job strain and cognitive decline: A prospective study of the framingham offspring cohort. Int. J. Occup. Environ. Med. 2015, 6, 79–94. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  81. Fujishiro, K.; Diez-Roux, A.V.; Landsbergis, P.A.; Jenny, N.S.; Seeman, T. Current employment status, occupational category, occupational hazard exposure and job stress in relation to telomere length: The Multiethnic Study of Atherosclerosis (MESA). Occup. Environ. Med. 2013, 70, 552–560. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  82. Chmelar, C.; Jorres, R.A.; Kronseder, A.; Muller, A.; Nowak, D.; Weigl, M. Associations Between Age, Psychosocial Work Conditions, Occupational Well-Being, and Telomere Length in Geriatric Care Professionals: A Mixed-Methods Study. J. Occup. Environ. Med. 2017, 59, 949–955. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  83. Meier, H.C.S.; Hussein, M.; Needham, B.; Barber, S.; Lin, J.; Seeman, T.; Diez Roux, A. Cellular response to chronic psychosocial stress: Ten-year longitudinal changes in telomere length in the Multi-Ethnic Study of Atherosclerosis. Psychoneuroendocrinology 2019, 107, 70–81. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  84. Cohen-Manheim, I.; Doniger, G.M.; Sinnreich, R.; Simon, E.S.; Pinchas, R.; Aviv, A.; Kark, J.D. Increased attrition of leukocyte telomere length in young adults is associated with poorer cognitive function in midlife. Eur. J. Epidemiol. 2016, 31, 147–157. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  85. Massamba, V.K.; Talbot, D.; Milot, A.; Pearce, N.; Trudel, X.; Brisson, C. Assessment of the healthy worker survivor effect in the relationship between psychosocial work-related factors and hypertension. Occup. Environ. Med. 2019, 76, 414–421. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  86. Pearce, N.; Checkoway, H.; Kriebel, D. Bias in occupational epidemiology studies. Occup. Environ. Med. 2007, 64, 562–568. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  87. Buckley, J.P.; Keil, A.P.; McGrath, L.J.; Edwards, J.K. Evolving methods for inference in the presence of healthy worker survivor bias. Epidemiology 2015, 26, 204–212. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
Figure 1. Flow chart of the study population.
Figure 1. Flow chart of the study population.
Ijerph 20 04929 g001
Figure 2. Illustration of the total, direct, and indirect associations through telomere length and inflammatory index, between exposure to passive work and global cognitive function. M0: Associations between exposure and mediators, or between mediators and global cognitive function. All associations were adjusted for age, sex, education, BMI, smoking, alcohol, physical activity, diabetes, hypertension, cardiovascular diseases, number of confidants, number of helpers, relationship with spouse, and relationship with children. Direct associations were additionally adjusted for inflammatory index, telomere length, inflammatory index squared, and interaction terms between telomere length and inflammatory index, and between telomere length and inflammatory index squared. Missing data on covariates, exposure, and outcomes were imputed with multiple imputation, and data were weighted for lost to follow-up between T1 and T2, and between T2 and T3, with an inverse probability of censoring weighting. * p < 0.05.
Figure 2. Illustration of the total, direct, and indirect associations through telomere length and inflammatory index, between exposure to passive work and global cognitive function. M0: Associations between exposure and mediators, or between mediators and global cognitive function. All associations were adjusted for age, sex, education, BMI, smoking, alcohol, physical activity, diabetes, hypertension, cardiovascular diseases, number of confidants, number of helpers, relationship with spouse, and relationship with children. Direct associations were additionally adjusted for inflammatory index, telomere length, inflammatory index squared, and interaction terms between telomere length and inflammatory index, and between telomere length and inflammatory index squared. Missing data on covariates, exposure, and outcomes were imputed with multiple imputation, and data were weighted for lost to follow-up between T1 and T2, and between T2 and T3, with an inverse probability of censoring weighting. * p < 0.05.
Ijerph 20 04929 g002
Table 1. Baseline characteristics a of the study sample b, overall and by sex.
Table 1. Baseline characteristics a of the study sample b, overall and by sex.
Overall c
N = 2219
Mean ± SD or n (%)
Females
N = 1133 (51%)
Mean ± SD or n (%)
Males
N = 1086 (49%)
Mean ± SD or n (%)
Age (y)
At T1 (1991–1993)38.9 ± 7.937.4 ± 7.440.4 ± 8.0
At T2 (1999–2001)46.5 ± 7.945.0 ± 7.448.0 ± 8.1
At T3 (2015–2018)63.2 ± 7.461.8 ± 7.064.7 ± 7.6
Education at T3 (y)15.3 ± 2.914.3 ± 2.716.3 ± 2.7
Smoking status
Never smoked regularly1012 (46.0)546 (48.5)466 (43.4)
Former regular smoker735 (33.4)330 (29.3)405 (37.7)
Current occasional smoker d101 (4.6)55 (4.9)46 (4.3)
Current regular smoker d351 (16.0)195 (17.3)156 (14.5)
Alcohol intake (drinks/week)3.5 ± 4.52.3 ± 2.94.7 ± 5.4
Physical activity (times/month)5.3 ± 4.34.7 ± 4.26.0 ± 4.4
Body mass index (kg/m2)24.5 ± 3.823.5 ± 3.925.5 ± 3.5
Waist-to-hip ratio0.84 ± 0.10.76 ± 0.070.92 ± 0.05
Diabetes (yes)34 (1.5)22 (1.9)12 (1.1)
Hypertension (yes)347 (15.6)120 (10.6)227 (20.9)
Cardiovascular diseases (yes)52 (2.3)26 (2.3)26 (2.4)
Confidants (number)2.1 ± 1.52.3 ± 1.41.9 ± 1.5
Helpers (number)3.0 ± 1.63.1 ± 1.52.9 ± 1.7
Relationship with spouse
Satisfying1370 (64.8)647 (59.6)723 (70.3)
Unsatisfying215 (10.2)112 (10.3)103 (10.0)
No spouse528 (25.0)326 (30.1)202 (19.7)
Relationship with children
Satisfying1311 (61.1)611 (55.4)310 (29.7)
Unsatisfying64 (3.0)30 (2.7)700 (67.1)
No children771 (35.9)461 (41.8)34 (3.3)
Passive work at T2722 (34.4)423 (40.0)299 (28.7)
High job strain at T2396 (18.9)239 (22.6)157 (15.1)
High psychological demands at T2991 (46.9)482 (45.2)509 (48.5)
Low job control at T21120 (53.3)664 (62.7)456 (43.7)
Low social support at work at T21152 (55.2)584 (55.6)568 (54.8)
Effort–reward imbalance at T2497 (24.3)255 (24.7)242 (23.8)
Low reward at T2954 (46.6)491 (47.5)463 (45.6)
Iso-strain at T2275 (13.3)156 (15.0)119 (11.5)
CRP concentration at T3, mg/L, median, interquartile range1.15 (0.43;2.65)1.34 (0.48;3.20)0.99 (0.41;2.15)
IL-6 concentration at T3, pg/mL, median, interquartile range2.05 (1.38;3.20)2.01 (1.35;3.18)2.09 (1.41;3.2)
Telomere length at T3, ratio T/S median, interquartile range0.75 (0.63;0.89)0.76 (0.64;0.92)0.74 (0.61;0.86)
Inflammatory index, mean, min, max0.00 (−4.87;5.22)0.03 (−4.87;5.22)−0.03 (−2.95;5.02)
MoCA score (out of 30) at T325.6 ± 2.625.8 ± 2.625.4 ± 2.5
a Measured at T1, unless otherwise specified. b Participants selected for blood sample, who participated at T3 and had cognitive data. c Less than 5% of missing data on any variable, except for work-related psychosocial factors (6–8%). d Occasional smoker refers to reporting smoking, but not everyday; regular smoker refers to reporting smoking everyday.
Table 2. Longitudinal associations of work-related psychosocial factors (T2) with telomere length and inflammatory index (T3), overall and by sex.
Table 2. Longitudinal associations of work-related psychosocial factors (T2) with telomere length and inflammatory index (T3), overall and by sex.
Telomere Length a
β (95% CI) p-Value
OverallFemalesMales
Passive work−0.04 (−0.08;−0.00) p = 0.0263−0.08 (−0.13;−0.03) p = 0.00260.01 (−0.05;0.06) p = 0.8237
High job strain0.01 (−0.04;0.05) p = 0.78390.01 (−0.05;0.07) p = 0.72180.00 (−0.07;0.07) p = 0.9921
High psychological demand0.02 (−0.02;0.05) p = 0.37700.04 (−0.01;0.09) p = 0.1103−0.01 (−0.06;0.04) p = 0.6403
Low job control−0.04 (−0.07;0.00) p = 0.0533−0.08 (−0.13;−0.02) p = 0.00510.01 (−0.04;0.05) p = 0.8351
Low social support−0.01 (−0.04;0.03 p = 0.7626)−0.01 (−0.06;0.04) p = 0.7497−0.00 (−0.05;0.04) p = 0.8895
Effort–reward imbalance0.02 (−0.01;0.06) p = 0.22320.03 (−0.02;0.08) p = 0.29870.01 (−0.04;0.06) p = 0.6262
Low reward−0.03 (−0.06;0.01) p = 0.1166−0.04 (−0.09;0.01) p = 0.1021−0.02 (−0.07;0.03) p = 0.4544
Iso-strain0.00 (−0.05;0.05) p = 0.9624−0.07 (−0.07;0.06) p = 0.93250.00 (−0.08;0.08) p = 0.9393
Inflammatory index a
β (95% CI) p-value
Passive work−0.01 (−0.11;0.10) p = 0.88090.04 (−0.11;0.19) p = 0.6134−0.07 (−0.22;0.08) p = 0.3633
High job strain0.07 (−0.06;0.20) p = 0.25880.02 (−0.15;0.19) p = 0.78980.18 (−0.02;0.38) p = 0.0759
High psychological demand−0.01 (−0.11;0.09) p = 0.9126−0.03 (−0.18;0.11) p = 0.66100.04 (−0.09;0.18) p = 0.5506
Low job control0.04 (−0.06;0.14) p = 0.42750.06 (−0.09;0.21) p = 0.43800.04 (−0.10;0.17) p = 0.6049
Low social support0.07 (−0.03;0.017) p = 0.1579−0.01 (−0.15;0.14) p = 0.92630.17 (0.03;0.31) p = 0.0142
Effort–reward imbalance0.06 (−0.04;0.16) p = 0.24820.00 (−0.15;0.15) p = 0.99390.14 (0.00;0.29) p = 0.0487
Low reward0.07 (−0.03;0.17) p = 0.19620.02 (−0.12;0.16) p = 0.74190.13 (−0.01;0.27) p = 0.0603
Iso-strain0.10 (−0.05;0.25) p = 0.1749−0.03 (−0.23;0.17) p = 0.74350.32 (0.10;0.55) p = 0.0038
a Adjusted for sex (except for stratified analyses), age, years of education, BMI, smoking, alcohol, physical activity, diabetes, hypertension, cardiovascular diseases, number of confidants, number of helpers, relationship with spouse, and relationship with children. Missing data on covariates, exposure, and outcomes were imputed with multiple imputation, and data were weighted for loss to follow-up between T1 and T2, and between T2 and T3, with an inverse probability of censoring weighting. β = beta coefficient, CI = confidence interval, BMI = body mass index.
Table 3. Cross-sectional associations of telomere length and inflammatory index with cognitive function at T3, overall and by sex.
Table 3. Cross-sectional associations of telomere length and inflammatory index with cognitive function at T3, overall and by sex.
Overall
β (95% CI) p-Value
Females
β (95% CI) p-Value
Males
β (95% CI) p-Value
Telomere length a0.26 (-0.00;0.52) p = 0.05270.10 (−0.27;0.47) p = 0.59230.50 (0.15;0.86) p = 0.0058
Inflammatory index a−0.02 (-0.11;0.08) p = 0.74250.07 (−0.06;0.20) p = 0.2740−0.12 (−0.25;0.01) p = 0.0763
a Adjusted for sex (except for stratified analyses), age, years of education, BMI, smoking, alcohol, physical activity, diabetes, hypertension, cardiovascular diseases, number of confidants, number of helpers, relationship with spouse, and relationship with children. When including interaction terms between telomere length and inflammatory index in quartiles, the p-values for the interaction between telomere length and inflammatory index were overall = 0.0283, females = 0.0050 and males = 0.2003. Missing data on covariates, exposure, and outcomes were imputed with multiple imputation, and data were weighted for loss to follow-up between T1 and T2, and between T2 and T3, with an inverse probability of censoring weighting. β = beta coefficient, CI = confidence interval, BMI = body mass index.
Table 4. Association between work-related psychosocial factors at T2 and global cognitive function at T3: total, direct, and indirect a associations through telomere length and inflammatory index, overall and by sex.
Table 4. Association between work-related psychosocial factors at T2 and global cognitive function at T3: total, direct, and indirect a associations through telomere length and inflammatory index, overall and by sex.
OVERALLTotal b
β (95% CI)
Direct c
β (95% CI)
Indirect
Telomere length b
β (95% CI)
Indirect
Inflammatory index b
β (95% CI)
Passive work−0.30 (−0.55;−0.06) p = 0.0163−0.29 (−0.54;−0.04) p = 0.0208−0.01 (−0.03;0.01) p = 0.2697−0.00 (−0.01;0.01) p = 0.9928
High job strain0.24 (−0.03;0.50) p = 0.07640.23 (−0.04;0.49) p = 0.09110.00 (−0.01;0.02) p = 0.81710.00 (−0.01;0.02) p = 0.9236
High psychological demand0.23 (0.03;0.44) p = 0.02800.23 (0.02;0.43) p = 0.03480.00 (−0.01;0.02) p = 0.5107−0.00 (−0.01;0.01) p = 0.9837
Low job control−0.13 (−0.36;0.10) p = 0.2571−0.13 (−0.36;0.10) p = 0.2726−0.01 (−0.02;0.01) p = 0.24220.00 (−0.01;0.02) p = 0.9497
Low social support−0.15 (−0.37;0.08) p = 0.2051−0.16 (−0.38;0.07) p = 0.1739−0.00 (−0.01;0.01) p = 0.78740.00 (−0.01;0.01) p = 0.8831
Effort–reward imbalance0.12 (−0.10;0.34) p = 0.29040.11 (−0.11;0.34) p = 0.32470.01 (−0.01;0.02) p = 0.39690.00 (−0.01;0.01) p = 0.9483
Low reward−0.21 (−0.44;0.02) p = 0.0708−0.20 (−0.43;0.03) p = 0.0828−0.01 (−0.02;0.01) p = 0.29450.00 (−0.01;0.01) p = 0.8886
Iso-strain0.19 (−0.12;0.51) p = 0.23430.19 (−0.13;0.50) p = 0.25290.00 (−0.01;0.02) p = 0.97050.00 (−0.02;0.02) p = 0.8834
FEMALES
Passive work−0.22 (−0.54;0.10) p = 0.1833−0.21 (−0.54;0.11) p = 0.1943−0.01 (−0.06;0.04) p = 0.69260.00 (−0.02;0.03) p = 0.8117
High job strain0.34 (0.01;0.68) p = 0.04490.34 (0.01;0.68) p = 0.04560.00 (−0.02;0.02) p = 0.89290.00 (−0.03;0.03) p = 0.8929
High psychological demand0.11 (−0.19;0.40) p = 0.47630.10 (−0.20;0.39) p = 0.51000.01 (−0.02;0.04) p = 0.6855−0.00 (−0.03;0.02) p = 0.8380
Low job control0.04 (−0.30;0.36) p = 0.82590.04 (−0.29;0.37) p = 0.8079−0.01 (−0.05;0.03) p = 0.55010.00 (−0.02;0.03) p = 0.7316
Low social support−0.02 (−0.33;0.29) p = 0.8968−0.03 (−0.35;0.28) p = 0.8378−0.00 (−0.02;0.01) p = 0.8465−0.00 (−0.03;0.03) p = 0.9883
Effort–reward imbalance0.05 (−0.25;0.35) p = 0.75350.04 (−0.26;0.35) p = 0.78790.00 (−0.02;0.02) p = 0.68730.00 (−0.02;0.02) p = 0.9920
Low reward−0.09 (−0.39;0.21) p = 0.5472−0.10 (−0.40;0.21) p = 0.5367−0.01 (−0.16;0.14) p = 0.93030.00 (−0.01;0.01) p = 0.9788
Iso-strain0.30 (−0.10;0.71) p = 0.13990.31 (−0.10;0.72) p = 0.1395−0.00 (−0.02;0.02) p = 0.9927−0.00 (−0.15;0.14) p = 0.9927
MALES
Passive work−0.40 (−0.78;−0.02) p = 0.0368−0.40 (−0.78;−0.03) p = 0.03660.00 (−0.02;0.03) p = 0.81280.01 (−0.02;0.03) p = 0.5592
High job strain0.11 (−0.29;0.51) p = 0.58830.12 (−0.29;0.52) p = 0.56300.00 (−0.03;0.03) p = 0.9985−0.02 (−0.06;0.03) p = 0.4314
High psychological demand0.38 (0.09;0.67) p = 0.01040.39 (0.09;0.68) p = 0.0095−0.00 (−0.03;0.02) p = 0.7092−0.00 (−0.02;0.01) p = 0.6562
Low job control−0.28 (−0.61;0.05) p = 0.0981−0.27 (−0.60;0.05) p = 0.10200.00 (−0.02;0.03) p = 0.8299−0.00 (−0.02;0.01) p = 0.7254
Low social support−0.23 (−0.55;0.09) p = 0.1661−0.22 (−0.55;0.10) p = 0.95600.00 (−0.02;0.02) p = 0.9560−0.01 (−0.05;0.02) p = 0.4338
Effort–reward imbalance0.17 (−0.14;0.49) p = 0.27730.18 (−0.13;0.50) p = 0.25610.01 (−0.02;0.03) p = 0.6217−0.01 (−0.04;0.02) p = 0.3853
Low reward−0.35 (−0.69;−0.01) p = 0.0459−0.32 (−0.67;0.03) p = 0.0702−0.01 (−0.03;0.02) p = 0.6116−0.01 (−0.04;0.02) p = 0.4543
Iso-strain0.06 (−0.42;0.53) p = 0.81420.08 (−0.41;0.57) p = 0.74910.00 (−0.04;0.04) p = 0.8857−0.03 (−0.10;0.04) p = 0.3536
a All residual indirect associations showed β = 0.00 or −0.00 with 95% CI or −0.01 to 0.01 at most, and p-values from 0.7791 to 0.9978. b Adjusted for sex (except for stratified analyses), age, education, BMI, smoking, alcohol, physical activity, diabetes, hypertension, cardiovascular disease, number of confidants, number of helpers, relationship with spouse, and relationship with children. c Additionally adjusted for inflammatory index, telomere length, inflammatory index squared, and interaction terms between telomere length and inflammatory index, and between telomere length and inflammatory index squared. Missing data on covariates, exposures, and outcomes were imputed with multiple imputation, and data were weighted for lost to follow-up between T1 and T2, and between T2 and T3, with an inverse probability of censoring weighting. Β = beta coefficient, CI = confidence interval, BMI = body mass index.
Disclaimer/Publisher’s Note: The statements, opinions and data contained in all publications are solely those of the individual author(s) and contributor(s) and not of MDPI and/or the editor(s). MDPI and/or the editor(s) disclaim responsibility for any injury to people or property resulting from any ideas, methods, instructions or products referred to in the content.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Duchaine, C.S.; Brisson, C.; Diorio, C.; Talbot, D.; Maunsell, E.; Carmichael, P.-H.; Giguère, Y.; Gilbert-Ouimet, M.; Trudel, X.; Ndjaboué, R.; et al. Work-Related Psychosocial Factors and Global Cognitive Function: Are Telomere Length and Low-Grade Inflammation Potential Mediators of This Association? Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2023, 20, 4929. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph20064929

AMA Style

Duchaine CS, Brisson C, Diorio C, Talbot D, Maunsell E, Carmichael P-H, Giguère Y, Gilbert-Ouimet M, Trudel X, Ndjaboué R, et al. Work-Related Psychosocial Factors and Global Cognitive Function: Are Telomere Length and Low-Grade Inflammation Potential Mediators of This Association? International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health. 2023; 20(6):4929. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph20064929

Chicago/Turabian Style

Duchaine, Caroline S., Chantal Brisson, Caroline Diorio, Denis Talbot, Elizabeth Maunsell, Pierre-Hugues Carmichael, Yves Giguère, Mahée Gilbert-Ouimet, Xavier Trudel, Ruth Ndjaboué, and et al. 2023. "Work-Related Psychosocial Factors and Global Cognitive Function: Are Telomere Length and Low-Grade Inflammation Potential Mediators of This Association?" International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health 20, no. 6: 4929. https://doi.org/10.3390/ijerph20064929

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop