Next Article in Journal
Consumer Choices in the Bread Market: The Importance of Fiber in Consumer Decisions
Next Article in Special Issue
Maternal Factors Associated with Levels of Fatty Acids, Specifically n-3 PUFA during Pregnancy: ECLIPSES Study
Previous Article in Journal
Low Molecular Weight Barley β-Glucan Affects Glucose and Lipid Metabolism by Prebiotic Effects
Previous Article in Special Issue
Effect of Vitamin D Status during Pregnancy on Infant Neurodevelopment: The ECLIPSES Study
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Long-Chain Polyunsaturated Fatty Acids, Homocysteine at Birth and Fatty Acid Desaturase Gene Cluster Polymorphisms Are Associated with Children’s Processing Speed up to Age 9 Years

by
Cristina Campoy
1,2,3,4,5,*,
Hatim Azaryah
1,2,4,
Francisco J. Torres-Espínola
1,2,5,
Cristina Martínez-Zaldívar
1,2,
José Antonio García-Santos
1,2,4,
Hans Demmelmair
6,
Gudrun Haile
6,
Peter Rzehak
6,
Berthold Koletzko
6,
Eszter Györei
7,
Tamas Décsi
7,
María del Carmen Ramírez-Tortosa
8,
Eva Reischl
9,
Anne M. Molloy
10,
Juan de Dios Luna
11 and
Miguel Pérez-García
12,13
1
EURISTIKOS Excellence Centre for Paediatric Research, Biomedical Research Centre, University of Granada, 180016 Granada, Spain
2
Department of Paediatrics, School of Medicine, University of Granada, Avda, Investigación 11, 180016 Granada, Spain
3
Spanish Network of Biomedical Research in Epidemiology and Public Health (CIBERESP), Granada’s Node, Institute of Health Carlos III, 28029 Madrid, Spain
4
Instituto de Investigación Biosanitaria de Granada (Ibs-GRANADA), Health Sciences Technological Park, 18012 Granada, Spain
5
Instituto de Neurociencias “Doctor Olóriz”, Health Sciences Technological Park, 18012 Granada, Spain
6
Ludwig-Maximilians-Universität München, Department of Paediatrics, Paediatrics, Dr. von Hauner Children’s Hospital, University of Munich Hospitals, 80337 Munich, Germany
7
Department of Paediatrics, University of Pécs, 7623 Pécs, József Attila u. 7, 7623 Pécs, Hungary
8
Department of Biochemistry and Molecular Biology II, Institute of Nutrition and Food Technology “José Mataix Verdú”, Biomedical Research Center, University of Granada, 18100 Granada, Spain
9
Helmholtz Zentrum Munchen, Research Unit of Molecular Epidemiology, D-85764 Neuherberg, Germany
10
School of Medicine, Trinity College, 152–160 Pearse Street, D02 Dublin 2, Ireland
11
Department of Biostatistics, School of Medicine, University of Granada, 18016 Granada, Spain
12
Mind, Brain and Behaviour International Research Centre (CIMCYC), University of Granada, 18011 Granada, Spain
13
Spanish Network of Biomedical Research Centre on Mental Health (CIBERSAM), Granada’s Node, Institute of Health Carlos III, 28029 Madrid, Spain
*
Author to whom correspondence should be addressed.
Nutrients 2021, 13(1), 131; https://doi.org/10.3390/nu13010131
Submission received: 29 October 2020 / Revised: 18 December 2020 / Accepted: 29 December 2020 / Published: 31 December 2020
(This article belongs to the Special Issue Effect of Maternal Nutrition on Cognitive Function of Children)

Abstract

:
Both pre- and early postnatal supplementation with docosahexaenoic acid (DHA), arachidonic acid (AA) and folate have been related to neural development, but their long-term effects on later neural function remain unclear. We evaluated the long-term effects of maternal prenatal supplementation with fish-oil (FO), 5-methyltetrahydrofolate (5-MTHF), placebo or FO + 5-MTHF, as well as the role of fatty acid desaturase (FADS) gene cluster polymorphisms, on their offspring’s processing speed at later school age. This study was conducted in NUHEAL children at 7.5 (n = 143) and 9 years of age (n = 127). Processing speed tasks were assessed using Symbol Digit Modalities Test (SDMT), Children Color Trails Test (CCTT) and Stroop Color and Word Test (SCWT). Long-chain polyunsaturated fatty acids, folate and total homocysteine (tHcy) levels were determined at delivery from maternal and cord blood samples. FADS and methylenetetrahydrofolate reductase (MTHFR) 677 C > T genetic polymorphisms were analyzed. Mixed models (linear and logistic) were performed. There were significant differences in processing speed performance among children at different ages (p < 0.001). The type of prenatal supplementation had no effect on processing speed in children up to 9 years. Secondary exploratory analyses indicated that children born to mothers with higher AA/DHA ratio at delivery (p < 0.001) and heterozygotes for FADS1 rs174556 (p < 0.05) showed better performance in processing speed at 9 years. Negative associations between processing speed scores and maternal tHcy levels at delivery were found. Our findings suggest speed processing development in children up to 9 years could be related to maternal factors, including AA/DHA and tHcy levels, and their genetic background, mainly FADS polymorphism. These considerations support that maternal prenatal supplementation should be quantitatively adequate and individualized to obtain better brain development and mental performance in the offspring.

1. Introduction

The speed of information processing is essential for higher order cognitive functions, including memory or executive functions [1]. Processing speed can be defined as the time required to move information from one neuron to the next [2], or how quickly a person can perform the mental operations needed to complete a task [3]. This cognitive process is highly related to intact myelination, which is important for the integration of information across spatially distributed neural networks. Moreover, the association between white matter integrity and processing speed in cognitive tasks has been consistently established [4,5].
Long-chain polyunsaturated fatty acids (LC-PUFAs) and folic acid play an important role in brain development, particularly during fetal and early postnatal life [6,7]. Interestingly, their effects on neurodevelopment depend on timing of occurrence and brain needs for particular nutrients at that time [8]. For instance, essential fatty acids (FA) deficiencies during the first year of life lead to severe impairments in synapse formation and myelination [9], which may have negative effects on processing speed tasks later in life [10].
LC-PUFAs, particularly docosahexaenoic acid (DHA, 22:6 n − 3) and arachidonic acid (AA, 20:4 n − 6) are incorporated into the brain in relatively large amounts during the pre- and postnatal growth spurt [11,12,13]. However, due to a limited capacity in the fetus and neonate for PUFAs elongation and desaturation, tissue deposition of DHA and AA strongly depends on the pre-formed LC-PUFAs supply via the placenta and postnatal diet [14]. The availability of different PUFAs is also dependent on genetic polymorphisms in the fatty acid desaturase (FADS) gene cluster [15]. There is evidence that dietary LC-PUFAs supply in early life may modulate information processing [12,16], cognitive and visual development [17,18], as well as early mental and motor skills development [6,19]. Recently, higher maternal DHA status has been also related to better performance in language and short-term memory in the offspring [20]. Moreover, early availability of n − 6 PUFAs, mainly AA, during pre- and postnatal periods has been positively associated to cognitive performance and mental function in later childhood [21,22,23].
Folate intake before and during pregnancy is also essential for normal brain development, differentiation and cognitive performance [24,25,26,27]. Maternal folate deficiency causes structural brain abnormalities during fetal development and poor childhood cognitive ability [26], while maternal folate supplementation in pregnancy improves neurological development and may reduce the prevalence of autism spectrum disorders in their offspring [24,28].
Having in mind that LC-PUFAs and folate play key roles in synaptogenesis, synapse maturation and myelination, dietary intake of both nutrients might have effects on processing speed [2]. Long-term effects of nutritional interventions on processing speed have been classically evaluated using perceptual speed tasks [29], but further studies evaluating specific neuropsychological domains are still missing [6,30]. Therefore, our main objective was to evaluate the potential role of LC-PUFAs and total homocysteine (functional maker of folate status) at delivery, as well as maternal FADS polymorphisms, on children´s processing speed at 7.5 and 9 years.

2. Materials and Methods

2.1. Study Design and Subjects

This is a follow-up study of the NUHEAL (Nutraceuticals for a Healthier Life) trial, registered at www.ClinicalTrials.gov, Identifier NCT01180933. Detailed study design, subject recruitment, and population characteristics have been described elsewhere [31,32]. Briefly, NUHEAL project is a multicenter, randomized, double-blind, placebo-controlled trial in healthy pregnant women from Munich (Germany), Pécs (Hungary) and Granada (Spain). Children from Hungary were not included in the present analysis due to high number of missing values. Pregnant women were assigned by blockwise randomization to receive either a modified fish-oil (FO) preparation [500 mg DHA + 150 mg eicosapentaenoic acid (EPA)/day], 5-methyl-tetrahydrofolate (5-MTHF) (400 µg/day), a combination of both supplements (FO + 5-MTHF), or placebo, from gestational week 20 until delivery. Detailed information on sociodemographic data and course of pregnancy together with maternal blood samples were collected at 20 and 30 weeks of pregnancy and at delivery; additionally, cord blood samples were also obtained.
Within NUTRIMENTHE EU Project (grant agreement no. 212652) framework, of 270 women participating in NUHEAL study until giving birth, 152 mothers agreed to participate in the follow-up for their offspring at 7.5 and 9 years of age. Processing speed tasks were entirely performed in 143 NUHEAL children at 7.5 years (FO, n = 38; 5-MTHF, n = 29; FO + 5-MTHF, n = 36; placebo n = 40) and 127 NUHEAL children at 9 years (FO, n = 34; 5-MTHF, n = 28; FO + 5-MTHF = 28; placebo, n = 37) (Figure 1).
The follow-up study protocols were approved by the Ethical Committees from all centers involved in the study. Written informed consent was obtained from parents of all participating children at study entry and at each follow-up.

2.2. Neuropsychological Assessment

NUTRIMENTHE Neuropsychological Battery (NNB) was used to evaluate the whole spectrum of neuropsychological functioning in children aged 7.5 and 9 years, including processing speed tasks such as Symbol Digit Modalities Test (SDMT), Children Color Trails Test (part 1) (CCTT-1) and Stroop Color and Word Test (SCWT) [33].
The Symbol Digit Modalities Test (SDMT) was used to assess information-processing speed and attention [34]. This test requires individuals to write the correct number under the corresponding symbol according to a key code specified on the top of the page, which links different meaningless geometric symbols with numbers 1 through 9. The participant is given 90 s to complete the task. The number of correctly identified symbols (hits) is recorded as a score, interpreting higher scores to be an indicator of better child processing speed.
The Children Color Trails Test (CCTT) is an individually administered neuropsychological instrument which consists of two parts used to evaluate sustained visual attention, sequencing, psychomotor speed (part-1), and cognitive flexibility (part-2). This test requires the connection of one set of encircled numbers (1–25) in ascending order. Current study used only the part 1 of this test, which even numbers are printed in a yellow background while odd numbers are printed in a pink background. The final score is the time (in seconds) taken to complete part-1 of the CCTT [35], so that shortest time in this test is related to better processing speed of participant.
Finally, Golden’s version of the Stroop Color and Word Test (SCWT) was used to evaluate selective attention, cognitive inhibition and information processing speed [36]. There are three components to this task. First, participants are asked to read aloud color words (blue, green and red) printed in black ink. Second, the child is asked to say the colors of “XXXX” printed in blue, green or red. Finally, the child is asked to name the ink color of color words (blue, green or red) printed in incongruent colors as quickly and accurately as possible in 45 s time. As consequence, this test produces three direct scores: the word-reading score, the color-naming score, and the color-word score, respectively [37]. The increase in time taken to perform the color-word test compared with the word-reading and color-naming tests is called color-word interference effect or Stroop effect, which is considered as the main dependent variable for SCWT test [38]. Both word-reading and color-naming scores (hits) are related to processing speed of congruent semantic information (high scores represent better processing speed), but color-word score is associated to attention development and, for this reason, it has not been taken into account in the current study.

2.3. Fatty Acid Analyses in Maternal and Umbilical Cord Plasma Phospholipids

Procedures of analysis for FA determinations in plasma phospholipids have been described in detail elsewhere [31,32]. Briefly, blood was centrifuged at 3500× g for 10 min at room temperature within 2 h. Plasma was thereafter removed and stored at −80 °C. Lipid extraction from plasma was performed according to the method of Kolarovic and Fournier [39]. Analysis of FA methyl esters from plasma phospholipids was performed by high-resolution capillary gas-liquid chromatography. Conditions during the analysis and standards used were described elsewhere [40]. Results were expressed as weight percentages (wt %) of all quantified FA.

2.4. Folate Analysis

Analysis of plasma folate was carried out by microbiological assay using a chloramphenicol resistant strain of Lactobacillus casei, as previously described [41]. Inter and intra-assay coefficients of variation were below of 11%.

2.5. Total Homocysteine

Total homocysteine (tHcy) concentrations were assayed by fluorescence polarized immunoassay on the IMx autoanalyser [42]. Sample preparation and chromatographic conditions were performance as described previously [43]. The fluorescence intensities were measured with excitation at 385 nm and emission at 515 nm.

2.6. SNP Selection and Genotyping

Genotyping of single-nucleotide polymorphisms (SNPs) from the FADS gene cluster was performed with the iPLEX method (Sequenom, San Diego, CA, USA) by means of matrix-assisted laser desorption ionization-time of flight mass spectrometry method (MALDI-TOF MS, Mass Array, Sequenom), according to the manufacturer´s instructions, as previously described [44]. Standard genotyping quality control included 10% duplicate and negative samples. Genotyping discordance rate was below 0.3%. SNP selection for following analyses is showed in Supplemental Methods (Supplemental Figure S1 and Supplemental Tables S1–S3).

2.7. MTHFR 677 C/T Polymorphism

Genomic DNA was prepared from maternal and umbilical cord blood samples obtained at delivery. DNA samples were genotyped for the methylenetetrahydrofolate reductase (MTHFR) 677 C/T variant by polymerase chain reaction (PCR), restriction enzyme digestion, and DNA fragment separation by electrophoresis, as described previously [42]. MTHFR 677 C > T was selected for its high clinical relevance in humans due to the association of TT genotype with high plasma homocysteine concentrations [45].

2.8. Statistical Analysis

Using standard approaches, statistical power for the current study was calculated, setting α value as 0.05 and β value as 0.2. Different intervention groups (FO, 5-MTHF and FO + 5-MTHF) were grouped and compared with placebo group for processing speed tasks, including SDMT, CCTT and SCWT, in children at 7.5 years old. A statistical power of 80% was obtained for selected tasks in aforementioned population (Supplemental Table S4). “PowerEQTL v0.1.3” (R software) was also used to calculate the statistical power for our genetic study; except for FADS2 rs174570 and FADS3 rs2727271, a statistical power of 90% was obtained for analyzed SNPs (Supplemental Figure S2 and Supplemental Table S5).
A descriptive analysis of quantitative variables was performed using summary measures (mean, standard deviation (SD), standard error of the mean, centiles, median, interquartile or amplitude ranges), meanwhile frequency distribution was used for qualitative variables. Comparisons among different groups of treatment (FO, 5-MTHF, both or placebo) were made using one-way ANOVA for continuous variables or χ2 test for categorical variables. In order to verify the underlying hypothesis of one-way ANOVA (variance’s homogeneity and normality), Box-Cox transformation was computed when considered necessary. When one-way ANOVA resulted significant, Bonferroni test post-hoc was applied.
Variables of processing speed obtained from SDMT, CCTT-1 and SCWT tests were considered as dependent variables. Considering the hierarchical structure resulted from processing speed evaluation in children at 7.5 and 9 years, mixed models for repeated measures were performed considering as fixed effects a set of potential confounding variables, such as study group, time point, country of origin, maternal age, hematocrit levels, parity, gravidity risk maternal education level, maternal smoking, maternal BMI, mode of delivery, gestational age, child’s sex and mother’s biochemical and molecular parameters at delivery (AA/DHA ratio, FADS1 rs174556 polymorphism, total homocysteine, MTHFR C677T polymorphisms, plasma folate). Interactions between study group and time point were also studied. The random effects were the subjects nested ID to estimate the intra variance. Logistic regression mixed model was used when dependent variables were dichotomized (below or above percentile 25, 50 and 75), in which case the measure of the effect was the suitable odds ratio with corresponding confidence interval (Supplemental Tables S6 and S7). All potential confounders were added at once and selected those which significance p < 0.05.
All statistical analyses were performed using the statistical package STATA 12.1 (Stata Corp, College Station, TX, USA). p values < 0.05 were considered as statistically significant.

3. Results

3.1. Background and Baseline Characteristics of the NUHEAL Study Participants

The baseline characteristics of the mothers whose children were evaluated at 7.5 and 9 years of age are shown in Table 1. No difference between prenatal supplementation groups were observed in those descriptive variables analyzed prior or during pregnancy, including country of origin, maternal age, BMI at 20 and 30 weeks of pregnancy, mode of delivery, hematocrit at 30 weeks of pregnancy, parity, smoking during pregnancy, gravidity risk at 20 weeks of pregnancy, high maternal education, family status or gestational age at delivery. Moreover, we analyzed maternal biochemical parameters at delivery. As expected, plasma levels of folate were significantly higher in those mothers who received 5-MTHF or FO + 5-MTHF supplementation during pregnancy (p < 0.001). However, type of prenatal supplementation had no effect on maternal AA/DHA ratio and tHcy levels.

3.2. Processing Speed Task of the NUHEAL Children at 7.5 and 9 Years Old

As shown in Table 2, no significant statistical differences were found among the type of prenatal supplementation in all analyzed processing speed tasks at 7.5 years, except for CCTT-1 test. In fact, children born to mothers who were supplemented with 5-MTHF during pregnancy showed a decrease in the timing to solve CCTT-1 (p = 0.017). Moreover, there were no differences between type of prenatal supplementation and processing speed tasks at 9 years old. In general, we observed that children aged 9 years had better processing speed than those aged 7.5 years in terms of higher scores and less time to solve CCTT-1 test.

3.3. Prenatal Predictors of Processing Speed in Children up to 9 Years

3.3.1. Symbol Digit Modalities Test (SDMT) Hits

Type of prenatal supplementation had no long-term effects on SDMT hits in children at 7.5 and 9 years, (Table 3). However, we observed an age-dependent increase in SDMT hits; in fact, children at 9 years showed an increase of 10.72 points in the mean of hits [(95% CI: 7.98–13.47); p < 0.001] compared to children at 7.5 years. Moreover, each unit of increase in maternal AA/DHA ratio at delivery predicted an increase of 8.80 points in the mean of SDMT hits [(95% CI: 3.61–13.99); p = 0.001]. Conversely, higher maternal weight gain between 20 and 30 weeks of pregnancy (dBMI) [(95% CI: −1.25–−0.02); p = 0.043] and maternal tHcy [(95% CI: −1.02–−0.002); p = 0.049] were associated with a decrease of 0.63 and 0.52 points in the mean of SDMT hits, respectively.
Further logistic regression analysis, characterizing those children aged 9 years having hits above P75, showed that maternal AA/DHA ratio at delivery was the best predictor to obtain higher number of SMDT hits [OR: 30.46 (95% CI: 3.68–252.0); p = 0.002] (Table 4). This probability is also increased in those children whose mothers had a high educational level (p = 0.013) or were heterozygote for FADS1 rs174556 (p = 0.014). However, maternal tHcy at delivery reduced the odds of placing children aged 9 years above the P75, with a probability of 0.71 to obtain less hits per each unit (μmol/L) of increase of tHcy [OR: 0.71 (95% CI: 0.56–0.89); p = 0.003].

3.3.2. Children Color Trails Test (CCTT−1)

No statistically significant differences in elapsed time for CCTT-1 at 7.5 and 9 years were found among groups of prenatal supplementation after adjustment for selected confounders. Independently of prenatal supplementation, children aged 9 years showed a decrease in the timing spent to solve the task compared to their results at the previous examination at age 7.5 years (p < 0.001) (Table 3).
After considering the interaction between age and prenatal supplementation, we observed that decrease in time elapsed was higher in those children whose mothers were supplemented with FO during pregnancy (p = 0.0001), 5-MTHF (p = 0.0113) or FO + 5-MTHF (p = 0.038) (Figure 2), but not in the placebo group.
As shown in Table 3, other selected cofounders had also a significant association on the time required to complete CCTT-1 test. In fact, vacuum delivery determined an increase in the time elapsed of 24.77 s for the task (p = 0.010) compared to those children whose mothers had an uncomplicated spontaneous delivery. Moreover, smoking during pregnancy had also a negative effect on the child’s CCTT-1 hits, increasing the time of solving this task in 23.43 s (p = 0.001). Finally, we observed that maternal AA/DHA ratio at delivery was a significant factor determining less timing to solve the CCTT-1 task of their offspring up to 9 years (−27.87 s, p = 0.019). Positive association between maternal AA/DHA ratio and CCTT-1 was also determined after logistic regression analysis, and characterizing those children aged 9 years with elapsed time of solving the CCTT-1 below the P25 [OR: 18.53 (95% CI: 2.13–160.9); p = 0.008] (Table 4). Shorter solving times were also observed in those children born to mothers who were heterozygote for FADS1 rs174556 [OR: 2.75 (95% CI: 1.02–7.39; p = 0.045].

3.3.3. Stroop Color and Word Test (SCWT)

We analyzed the influence of prenatal supplementation and other selected cofounders on the information processing speed using Stroop Color and Word test and its obtained scores: Word-Reading (WRST) and Color-Naming (CNST).
Similarly to initial descriptive evaluation, further adjusted analysis for selected cofounders did not show statistically significant differences between different types of prenatal supplementation in the number of WRST hits obtained by children at 7.5 and 9 years (Table 3). However, independently of maternal supplementation during pregnancy, children at 9 years showed an increase of 15.37 points in the number of hits for WRST (p < 0.001) respect to children at 7.5 years. In relation to other cofounders considered in the model, boys showed 4.93 less hits than girls solving the task (p = 0.019). Moreover, we also observed that each increment of one unit (µmol/L) of maternal tHcy at delivery predicted that their offspring had 1.09 less hits by average solving the task (p = 0.025). Further logistic regression analysis, characterizing those children aged 9 years which hits of solving the WRST test below the P50, determined that maternal tHcy at delivery had a negative influence on this test [OR: 0.69 (95% CI: 0.51–0.93); p = 0.015] (Table 4).
When analyzing the effects of both prenatal supplementation and child´s age on CNST, similar results to those reported above were found. In fact, cofounder adjustment analysis did not show statistical differences between different groups of prenatal supplementation in the number of hits obtained by NUHEAL children at 7.5 and 9 years. Again, there was an increase of CNST hits at 9 years compared to 7.5 years (p < 0.001), but this effect was independent of type of supplementation during pregnancy (Table 3). Interestingly, we observed that each increment of one unit of AA/DHA ratio in maternal blood at delivery determined that their offspring had 6.55 more hits by average solving the CNST (p = 0.034) (Table 3). After logistic regression analysis, we observed that maternal age positively influenced the likelihood for children aged 9 years to be above the P75 to solve CNST test (OR: 1.20 (95% CI: 1.01–1.42); p = 0.039), but maternal tHcy level at delivery reduced this probability (OR: 0.51 (95% CI: 0.32–0.81); p = 0.005) (Table 4).

4. Discussion

This study was performed to analyze the long-term effects of prenatal supplementation, as well as maternal FADS and MTHFR genetic polymorphisms, on processing speed in healthy school-age children. Our results suggest that neither FO nor folate prenatal supplementation predicted high processing speed scores at school-age children. However, our secondary exploratory analyses seem to indicate that maternal AA/DHA ratio and FADS1 rs174556 SNPs, were positively associated with later processing speed in their offspring up to 9 years, particularly in SDMT and CCTT-1 tests, while tHcy concentrations in maternal plasma at delivery showed a negatively effect on child processing speed, according to results obtained in SDMT, WRST and CNST tests.
Prenatal folic acid supplementation has been related to better neurodevelopment in offspring, in terms of reducing the risk of behavioral problems [46], language delay [47], inattention [24], hyperactivity and peer problems [4,48,49]. However, their potential effects on cognitive and mental performance during development are inconsistent, partly due to the very limited number of studies published [50]. Folic acid acts as methyl donor in the metabolic conversion of homocysteine (Hcy) to methionine [51]. As consequence, poor folate status, by itself or in combination with a poor status of other B-vitamins, attenuates this metabolic pathway, which subsequently increases total Hcy levels. There is evidence that higher maternal Hcy (≥8.3 µmol/L) may not only negatively influence placental development, birth weight and pregnancy outcomes [52], but is also related to cytotoxic- and oxidative stress-dependent endothelial cell impairment and apoptosis of placental trophoblast [53,54]. Our results suggest a negative relation between maternal tHcy and the child´s later cognitive function. Children born to mothers with high tHcy levels during pregnancy showed a decrease in SDMT and SCWT hits, as well as lower likelihood to be in the upper percentiles of the WRST and CNST. Because tHcy concentrations are a functional indicator of folate status, folate supplementation before and during pregnancy may enhance neurodevelopment, while early supplementation also reduces the risk of congenital malformations [55].
Fetal blood levels of LC-PUFAs, including DHA and AA levels, are closely related to maternal LC-PUFAs status during pregnancy [32], and play a major role for an optimal brain development [20,56]. DHA is related to synaptogenesis, nerve growth factor expression and neuronal differentiation [57]. AA is involved in several synaptic signaling pathways [58], synthesis of eicosanoids, prostaglandins and leukotrienes [11,12], growth-related early gene expression and cell growth [59,60]. However, controversial results have been reported regarding long-term effects of n − 3 or n − 6 LC-PUFAs supplementation during pregnancy or lactation on the child´s later neurodevelopment [6,17,31,61,62,63]. As a consequence, there is growing interest to analyze the long-term effects of AA/DHA ratio on child neurodevelopment, which reflects both its endogenous synthesis and exogenous supply. Moreover, because both FAs compete for the same enzymatic pathways to convert them into biologically active eicosanoids, AA/DHA ratio is strongly influenced by the prevalence of genetic predisposition for FADS and elongase genes [57]. Higher DHA/AA ratio and higher DHA concentrations in cord blood have been considered beneficial for infant visual, cognitive and motor development in Arctic Inuit exposed to high intakes of seafood and n − 3 LC-PUFAs [64]. In our study, we did not find clear associations between type of prenatal supplementation and processing speed development, except for beneficial effects of prenatal FO supplementation on CCTT-1 elapsed time at 9 years. However, a higher maternal AA/DHA ratio seems to be a positive and long-term modulator of processing speed (mainly on SDMT, CCTT-1 and CNST) in the offspring, indicating the importance not only of DHA but also its equilibrium with AA. Interestingly, at 9 years, children whose mothers were heterozygotes for FADS1 rs174556 performed better the processing speed tasks respect to those born to mothers with homozygous major alleles. Our findings are also consistent with a role of PUFAs in myelination and white matter integrity, as shown in animal studies [65,66,67]. In this regard, DHA may increase processing speed by changing the physical-chemical and structural properties of membrane [68]. Moreover, Peters et al. [69] not only demonstrated that erythrocyte membrane PUFAs concentrations in young adults seem to be robustly related to white matter integrity, but also showed that these associations were mostly related to AA levels. Since a connection between white matter integrity and processing speed in cognitive tasks has been established, our results seem to show positive and strong long-term effects of perinatal LC-PUFAs, in terms of adequate AA/DHA ratio and FADS1 polymorphism, on cognitive development, suggesting an increase of white matter volume and better integrity.
The major strength of the present study is the long-term follow-up, from pregnancy to age 9 years, allowing us to obtain evaluation of long-term effects of prenatal supplementation with FO, 5-MTHF or FO + 5-MTHF on the child’s cognitive abilities. Moreover, the NUHEAL study was conducted in three different countries (Spain and Germany) with distinct eating habits. Given that country of origin has been accounted for as a confounder, our data show that long-term effects observed on processing speed are independent of the women’s diet. In this regard, we highlight the influence of the cultural level of the mothers on the processing speed of their children, which increases the need to take into account the different socio-environmental factors during early life that may influence on later speed processing capacities.
Our results have some limitations. The number of children who belong to each study group was homogeneous but relatively low. However, after combining the data from children born to mothers who were supplemented during pregnancy (n = 103), the effects of supplement combinations were significant with respect to children whose mothers received placebo. Secondly, the effects of FO, 5-MTHF or FO + 5-MTHF supplementation were not evaluated at different time-points of administration. Moreover, our study has been conducted only in a selected age range (7.5–9 years). Thus, future studies will be necessary to evaluate whether our findings can be extended to other times of administration or at different ages during development. Moreover, conversion of homocysteine to methionine is largely based on both folate and vitamin B12 levels, which act as substrate and cofactor, respectively. However, the status of vitamin B12 level in our study is largely unknown. Finally, neuropsychological tests used to evaluate processing speed were administered by different technicians in each country, although all of them received a common training to reduce the examiner and cultural influences on the results.
Findings obtained in this study should be interpreted with caution. NUHEAL women belonging to FO and FO + 5-MTHF groups received fish oil preparation, alone or in combination with folate, at a dose of 500 mg DHA + 150 mg EPA from week 20 until delivery, which are higher than current international recommendations (200–300 mg of DHA/day) [12,70]. In addition, study participants followed their usual eating patterns, including PUFAs rich food. Therefore, it is of utmost importance to determine the timing, necessary duration and dosage of DHA + EPA supplementation (in equilibrium to AA) during pregnancy, for obtaining the best cognitive development in the offspring. Interestingly, the mixed supplementation including FO and 5-MTHF had no effect on processing speed up to age 9 years. Thus, we propose that maternal supplementation based on folate, DHA and EPA should be individualized, taking into account diet, habits, folate status and maternal FADS1 genetic variant rs174556 G/A, and perhaps not together at the same time during pregnancy.

5. Conclusions

In summary, in our population the maternal AA/DHA ratio at delivery and maternal heterozygosity for the FADS1 genetic variant rs174556 had positive long-term effects on processing speed in the offspring up to 9 years. Processing speed tasks, in terms of less time to solve CCTT-1 task time, was also better in the offspring of mothers who received prenatal FO supplementation. Our results also suggest that the increased maternal tHcy levels predict worse speed processing development at 9 years. These results suggest to devote attention to an adequate maternal LC-PUFAs and folate status during pregnancy.

Supplementary Materials

The following are available online at https://www.mdpi.com/2072-6643/13/1/131/s1, Table S1: Characteristics of the 17 analysed variants in FADS 1/2/3 gene cluster of NUHEAL population; Table S2: Characteristics of the 4 selected variants in FADS 1/2 gene from the initial 17 SNPs analyzed in the NUHEAL population; Table S3: Pairwise linkage disequilibrium analysis in maternal selected FADS SNPs; Table S4: Power calculation obtained for processing speed evaluation in NUHEAL children; Table S5: Power calculation obtained for genetic study in NUHEAL children; Table S6: Pairwise comparison of predictive margins of the mixed effects logistic regression model; Table S7: Pairwise comparison of predictive margins of the mixed effects logistic regression model. Figure S1: Pairwise LD measured in D’ (a) and r2 (b) for selected maternal FADS SNPs; Figure S2: Power estimation, α = 0.05, SNPs = 17 (one-way unbalanced ANOVA).

Author Contributions

Conceptualization, C.C., M.P.-G.; methodology, C.C., J.d.D.L. and M.P.-G.; formal analysis, C.C., H.A., M.d.C.R.-T., A.M.M., J.d.D.L.; investigation, F.J.T.-E., C.M.-Z., G.H., E.G.; writing—original draft preparation, C.C.; writing—review and editing, C.C., J.A.G.-S., H.D., E.R., P.R., M.P.-G.; supervision, C.C., T.D., B.K.; project administration, C.C., T.D., B.K.; conceptualization of the NUHEAL trial and funding acquisition, C.C., T.D., B.K., C.C., B.K. and T.D. were coordinators of the NUHEAL study at Granada, Germany and Hungary, respectively. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.

Funding

This work was supported by the Commission of the European Community’s 7th Framework Program (FP7/2008-2013), Grant agreement no. 212652 (NUTRIMENTHE Project); within the 6th Framework Program, Contract no. 007036 (EARNEST Project); and supported in part by the Commission of the European Community within the 5th Framework Program, Contract no. QLK1-CT-1999-00888 (NUHEAL EU Project). This publication is the work of the authors and does not necessarily reflect the views of the Commission of the European Community. The work of B.K. is supported by the European Commission, H2020 Programmes DYNAHEALTH-633595 und Lifecycle-733206, the European Research Council Advanced Grant META-GROWTH ERC-2012-AdG–no.322605, the Erasmus Plus Programmes Early Nutrition eAcademy Southeast Asia-573651-EPP-1-2016-1-DE-EPPKA2-CBHE-JP and Capacity Building to Improve Early Nutrition and Health in South Africa-598488-EPP-1-2018-1-DE-EPPKA2-CBHE-JP, the EU Interreg Programme Focus in CD-CE111 and the European Joint Programming Initiative Project NutriPROGRAM and EndObesity, the German Ministry of Education and Research, Berlin (Grant Nr. 01 GI 0825), and the German Research Council (Ko912/12-1 and INST 409/224-1 FUGG). BK is the Else Kröner-Senior professor of Paediatrics at LMU financially supported by the Else Kröner-Fresenius-Foundation and LMU University Hospitals. Funders had no role in the design, analysis or writing of this article.

Institutional Review Board Statement

NUTRIMENTHE Project (FP7 KBBE-2007-1) was conducted according to the guidelines of the Declaration of Helsinki for human research studies in 2013, and approved by the Ethics Committee for human research of University of Munich (protocol code 01111), University of Granada (Grant agreement number 212652) and University of Pécs (Grant agreement number 3297).

Informed Consent Statement

Informed consent was obtained from all subjects involved in the study.

Data Availability Statement

Not applicable.

Acknowledgments

The authors wish to acknowledge the parents and children who participated in the study, an also the pediatricians and technicians belonging to NUHEAL team from University of Munich, University of Granada and University of Pécs for their contribution.

Conflicts of Interest

The authors declare no conflict of interest.

Statement

The results of this article have been included in the Doctoral Thesis of Hatim Azaryah in the context of the Epidemiology and Public Health Doctoral Program at the University of Granada.

References

  1. Rabbitt, P.; Scott, M.; Lunn, M.; Thacker, N.; Lowe, C.; Pendleton, N.; Horan, M.; Jackson, A. White matter lesions account for all age-related declines in speed but not in intelligence. Neuropsychology 2007, 21, 363–370. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  2. Cheatham, C.L.; Nerhammer, A.S.; Asserhoj, M.; Michaelsen, K.F.; Lauritzen, L. Fish oil supplementation during lactation: Effects on cognition and behavior at 7 years of age. Lipids 2011, 46, 637–645. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  3. Salthouse, T.A. Aging and measures of processing speed. Biol. Psychol. 2000, 54, 35–54. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  4. Kochunov, P.; Coyle, T.; Lancaster, J.; Robin, D.A.; Hardies, J.; Kochunov, V.; Bartzokis, G.; Stanley, J.; Royall, D.; Schlosser, A.E.; et al. Processing speed is correlated with cerebral health markers in the frontal lobes as quantified by neuroimaging. Neuroimage 2010, 49, 1190–1199. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  5. Penke, L.; Munoz, M.S.; Murray, C.; Gow, A.J.; Hernandez, M.C.; Clayden, J.D.; Starr, J.M.; Wardlaw, J.M.; Bastin, M.E.; Deary, I.J. A general factor of brain white matter integrity predicts information processing speed in healthy older people. J. Neurosci. 2010, 30, 7569–7574. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  6. Campoy, C.; Escolano-Margarit, M.V.; Anjos, T.; Szajewska, H.; Uauy, R. Omega 3 fatty acids on child growth, visual acuity and neurodevelopment. Br. J. Nutr. 2012, 107 (Suppl. 2), S85–S106. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  7. Koletzko, B.; Boey, C.C.; Campoy, C.; Carlson, S.E.; Chang, N.; Guillermo-Tuazon, M.A.; Joshi, S.; Prell, C.; Quak, S.H.; Sjarif, D.R.; et al. Current information and Asian perspectives on long-chain polyunsaturated fatty acids in pregnancy, lactation, and infancy: Systematic review and practice recommendations from an early nutrition academy workshop. Ann. Nutr. Metab. 2014, 65, 49–80. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. Georgieff, M.K. Nutrition and the developing brain: Nutrient priorities and measurement. Am. J. Clin. Nutr. 2007, 85, 614S–620S. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. McNamara, R.K.; Carlson, S.E. Role of omega-3 fatty acids in brain development and function: Potential implications for the pathogenesis and prevention of psychopathology. Prostaglandins Leukot. Essent. Fatty Acids 2006, 75, 329–349. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  10. Chevalier, N.; Kurth, S.; Doucette, M.R.; Wiseheart, M.; Deoni, S.C.; Dean, D.C., III; O’Muircheartaigh, J.; Blackwell, K.A.; Munakata, Y.; LeBourgeois, M.K. Myelination Is Associated with Processing Speed in Early Childhood: Preliminary Insights. PLoS ONE 2015, 10, e0139897. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  11. Delgado-Noguera, M.F.; Calvache, J.A.; Cosp, X.B.; Kotanidou, E.P.; Galli-Tsinopoulou, A. Supplementation with long chain polyunsaturated fatty acids (LCPUFA) to breastfeeding mothers for improving child growth and development. Cochrane. Database. Syst. Rev. 2015. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  12. Koletzko, B.; Cetin, I.; Brenna, J.T. Dietary fat intakes for pregnant and lactating women. Br. J. Nutr. 2007, 98, 873–877. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  13. Simmer, K.; Patole, S.K.; Rao, S.C. Long-chain polyunsaturated fatty acid supplementation in infants born at term. Cochrane. Database. Syst. Rev 2008. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  14. Carlson, S.E. Docosahexaenoic acid supplementation in pregnancy and lactation. Am. J. Clin. Nutr. 2009, 89, 678S–684S. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  15. Lattka, E.; Klopp, N.; Demmelmair, H.; Klingler, M.; Heinrich, J.; Koletzko, B. Genetic variations in polyunsaturated fatty acid metabolism—Implications for child health? Ann. Nutr. Metab. 2012, 60 (Suppl. 3), 8–17. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  16. Simmer, K.; Patole, S.K.; Rao, S.C. Long-chain polyunsaturated fatty acid supplementation in infants born at term. Cochrane Database Syst. Rev. 2011, 12. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. De, G.R.; Roggi, C.; Cena, H. n-3 LC-PUFA supplementation: Effects on infant and maternal outcomes. Eur. J. Nutr. 2014, 53, 1147–1154. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  18. Scholtz, S.A.; Kerling, E.H.; Shaddy, D.J.; Li, S.; Thodosoff, J.M.; Colombo, J.; Carlson, S.E. Docosahexaenoic acid (DHA) supplementation in pregnancy differentially modulates arachidonic acid and DHA status across FADS genotypes in pregnancy. Prostaglandins Leukot. Essent. Fatty Acids 2015, 94, 29–33. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  19. Judge, M.P.; Cong, X.; Harel, O.; Courville, A.B.; Lammi-Keefe, C.J. Maternal consumption of a DHA-containing functional food benefits infant sleep patterning: An early neurodevelopmental measure. Early Hum. Dev. 2012, 88, 531–537. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Mulder, K.A.; Elango, R.; Innis, S.M. Fetal DHA inadequacy and the impact on child neurodevelopment: A follow-up of a randomised trial of maternal DHA supplementation in pregnancy. Br. J. Nutr. 2018, 119, 271–279. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Kohlboeck, G.; Glaser, C.; Tiesler, C.; Demmelmair, H.; Standl, M.; Romanos, M.; Koletzko, B.; Lehmann, I.; Heinrich, J. Effect of fatty acid status in cord blood serum on children’s behavioral difficulties at 10 y of age: Results from the LISAplus Study. Am. J. Clin. Nutr. 2011, 94, 1592–1599. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  22. Koletzko, B.; Carlson, S.E.; van Goudoever, J.B. Should Infant Formula Provide Both Omega-3 DHA and Omega-6 Arachidonic Acid? Ann. Nutr. Metab. 2015, 66, 137–138. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  23. Steer, C.D.; Lattka, E.; Koletzko, B.; Golding, J.; Hibbeln, J.R. Maternal fatty acids in pregnancy, FADS polymorphisms, and child intelligence quotient at 8 y of age. Am. J. Clin. Nutr. 2013, 98, 1575–1582. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  24. Julvez, J.; Fortuny, J.; Mendez, M.; Torrent, M.; Ribas-Fito, N.; Sunyer, J. Maternal use of folic acid supplements during pregnancy and four-year-old neurodevelopment in a population-based birth cohort. Paediatr. Perinat. Epidemiol. 2009, 23, 199–206. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  25. Marques, A.H.; O’Connor, T.G.; Roth, C.; Susser, E.; Bjorke-Monsen, A.L. The influence of maternal prenatal and early childhood nutrition and maternal prenatal stress on offspring immune system development and neurodevelopmental disorders. Front. Neurosci. 2013, 7, 120. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  26. Veena, S.R.; Krishnaveni, G.V.; Srinivasan, K.; Wills, A.K.; Muthayya, S.; Kurpad, A.V.; Yajnik, C.S.; Fall, C.H. Higher maternal plasma folate but not vitamin B-12 concentrations during pregnancy are associated with better cognitive function scores in 9- to 10- year-old children in South India. J. Nutr. 2010, 140, 1014–1022. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  27. Villamor, E.; Rifas-Shiman, S.L.; Gillman, M.W.; Oken, E. Maternal intake of methyl-donor nutrients and child cognition at 3 years of age. Paediatr. Perinat. Epidemiol. 2012, 26, 328–335. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  28. Suren, P.; Roth, C.; Bresnahan, M.; Haugen, M.; Hornig, M.; Hirtz, D.; Lie, K.K.; Lipkin, W.I.; Magnus, P.; Reichborn-Kjennerud, T.; et al. Association between maternal use of folic acid supplements and risk of autism spectrum disorders in children. JAMA 2013, 309, 570–577. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  29. Lu, P.H.; Lee, G.J.; Raven, E.P.; Tingus, K.; Khoo, T.; Thompson, P.M.; Bartzokis, G. Age-related slowing in cognitive processing speed is associated with myelin integrity in a very healthy elderly sample. J. Clin. Exp. Neuropsychol. 2011, 33, 1059–1068. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Carlson, S.E. Early determinants of development: A lipid perspective. Am. J. Clin. Nutr. 2009, 89, 1523S–1529S. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  31. Campoy, C.; Escolano-Margarit, M.V.; Ramos, R.; Parrilla-Roure, M.; Csabi, G.; Beyer, J.; Ramírez-Tortosa, M.C.; Molloy, A.M.; Decsi, T.; Koletzko, B.V. Effects of prenatal fish-oil and 5-methyltetrahydrofolate supplementation on cognitive development of children at 6.5 y of age. Am. J. Clin. Nutr. 2011, 94, 1880S–1888S. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  32. Krauss-Etschmann, S.; Shadid, R.; Campoy, C.; Hoster, E.; Demmelmair, H.; Jiménez, M.; Gil, A.; Rivero, M.; Veszpremi, B.; Decsi, T.; et al. Effects of fish-oil and folate supplementation of pregnant women on maternal and fetal plasma concentrations of docosahexaenoic acid and eicosapentaenoic acid: A European randomized multicenter trial. Am. J. Clin. Nutr. 2007, 85, 1392–1400. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  33. Escribano, J.; Luque, V.; Canals-Sans, J.; Ferre, N.; Koletzko, B.; Grote, V.; Weber, M.; Gruszfeld, D.; Szott, K.; Verduci, E.; et al. Mental performance in 8-year-old children fed reduced protein content formula during the 1st year of life: Safety analysis of a randomised clinical trial. Br. J. Nutr. 2016, 2, 22–30. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  34. Smith, A. Symbol Digit Modalities Test: Manual; Western Psychological Corporation: Los Angeles, CA, USA, 2002. [Google Scholar]
  35. Llorente, A.M.; Voigt, R.G.; Williams, J.; Frailey, J.K.; Satz, P.; D’Elia, L.F. Children’s Color Trails Test 1 & 2: Test-retest reliability and factorial validity. Clin. Neuropsychol. 2009, 23, 645–660. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  36. Van der Elst, W.; Van Boxtel, M.P.; Van Breukelen, G.J.; Jolles, J. The Stroop color-word test: Influence of age, sex, and education; and normative data for a large sample across the adult age range. Assessment 2006, 13, 62–79. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  37. Peña-Casanova, J.; Quiñones-Ubeda, S.; Gramunt-Fombuena, N.; Quintana, M.; Aguilar, M.; Molinuevo, J.L.; Serradell, M.; Robles, A.; Barquero, M.S.; Payno, M.; et al. Spanish Multicenter Normative Studies (NEURONORMA Project): Norms for the Stroop color-word interference test and the Tower of London-Drexel. Arch. Clin. Neuropsychol. 2009, 24, 413–429. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  38. Strauss, E.; Sherman, E.M.S.; Spreen, O. A Compendium of Neuropsychological Tests: Administration, Norms, and Commentary, 2006; Oxford University Press: Oxford, UK, 2018. [Google Scholar]
  39. Kolarovic, L.; Fournier, N.C. A comparison of extraction methods for the isolation of phospholipids from biological sources. Anal. Biochem. 1986, 156, 244–250. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  40. Jakobik, V.; Burus, I.; Decsi, T. Fatty acid composition of erythrocyte membrane lipids in healthy subjects from birth to young adulthood. Eur. J. Pediatr. 2009, 168, 141–147. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  41. Molloy, A.M.; Scott, J.M. Microbiological assay for serum, plasma, and red cell folate using cryopreserved, microtiter plate method. Methods Enzymol. 1997, 281, 43–53. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  42. García-Minguillan, C.J.; Fernández-Ballart, J.D.; Ceruelo, S.; Ríos, L.; Bueno, O.; Berrocal-Zaragoza, M.I.; Molloy, A.M.; Ueland, P.M.; Meyer, K.; Murphy, M.M. Riboflavin status modifies the effects of methylenetetrahydrofolate reductase (MTHFR) and methionine synthase reductase (MTRR) polymorphisms on homocysteine. Genes Nutr. 2014, 9, 435. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  43. Mills, J.L.; McPartlin, J.M.; Kirke, P.N.; Lee, Y.J.; Conley, M.R.; Weir, D.G.; Scott, J.M. Homocysteine metabolism in pregnancies complicated by neural-tube defects. Lancet 1995, 345, 149–151. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  44. González-Casanova, I.; Rzehak, P.; Stein, A.D.; García Feregrino, R.; Rivera Dommarco, J.A.; Barraza-Villarreal, A.; Demmelmair, H.; Romieu, I.; Villalpando, S.; Martorell, R.; et al. Maternal single nucleotide polymorphisms in the fatty acid desaturase 1 and 2 coding regions modify the impact of prenatal supplementation with DHA on birth weight. Am. J. Clin. Nutr. 2016, 103, 1171–1178. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  45. Klerk, M.V.P.; Clarke, R.; Blom, H.J.; Kok, F.J.; Schouten, E.G. MTHFR 677C-->T polymorphism and risk of coronary heart disease: A meta-analysis. JAMA 2002, 288, 2023–2031. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  46. Roza, S.J.; van Batenburg-Eddes, T.; Steegers, E.A.; Jaddoe, V.W.; Mackenbach, J.P.; Hofman, A.; Verhulst, F.C.; Tiemeier, H. Maternal folic acid supplement use in early pregnancy and child behavioural problems: The Generation R Study. Br. J. Nutr. 2010, 103, 445–452. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  47. Roth, C.; Magnus, P.; Schjolberg, S.; Stoltenberg, C.; Suren, P.; McKeague, I.W.; Davey, S.G.; Reichborn-Kjennerud, T.; Susser, E. Folic acid supplements in pregnancy and severe language delay in children. JAMA 2011, 306, 1566–1573. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  48. Del Río, C.; Torres-Sánchez, L.; Chen, J.; Schnaas, L.; Hernández, C.; Osorio, E.; Galván-Portillo, M.; López-Carillo, L. Maternal MTHFR 677C>T genotype and dietary intake of folate and vitamin B(12): Their impact on child neurodevelopment. Nutr. Neurosci. 2009, 12, 13–20. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  49. Schlotz, W.; Jones, A.; Phillips, D.I.; Gale, C.R.; Robinson, S.M.; Godfrey, K.M. Lower maternal folate status in early pregnancy is associated with childhood hyperactivity and peer problems in offspring. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 2010, 51, 594–602. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  50. Chmielewska, A.; Dziechciarz, P.; Gieruszczak-Bialek, D.; Horvath, A.; Piescik-Lech, M.; Ruszczynski, M.; Skorka, A.; Szajewska, H. Effects of prenatal and/or postnatal supplementation with iron, PUFA or folic acid on neurodevelopment: Update. Br. J. Nutr. 2016, 122. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  51. Skorka, A.; Gieruszczak-Bialek, D.; Piescik, M.; Szajewska, H. Effects of prenatal and/or postnatal (maternal and/or child) folic acid supplementation on the mental performance of children. Crit. Rev. Food Sci. Nutr. 2012, 52, 959–964. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  52. Bergen, N.E.; Jaddoe, V.W.; Timmermans, S.; Hofman, A.; Lindemans, J.; Russcher, H.; Raat, H.; Steegers-Theunissen, R.P.; Steegers, E.A. Homocysteine and folate concentrations in early pregnancy and the risk of adverse pregnancy outcomes: The Generation R Study. BJOG 2012, 119, 739–751. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  53. Di, S.N.; Maggiano, N.; Caliandro, D.; Riccardi, P.; Evangelista, A.; Carducci, B.; Caruso, A. Homocysteine induces trophoblast cell death with apoptotic features. Biol. Reprod. 2003, 69, 1129–1134. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  54. Van Mil, N.H.; Oosterbaan, A.M.; Steegers-Theunissen, R.P. Teratogenicity and underlying mechanisms of homocysteine in animal models: A review. Reprod. Toxicol. 2010, 30, 520–531. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  55. Wald, N.J.; Morris, J.K.; Blakemore, C. Public health failure in the prevention of neural tube defects: Time to abandon the tolerable upper intake level of folate. Public Health Rev. 2018, 39, 2. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  56. Escolano-Margarit, M.V.; Campoy, C.; Ramírez-Tortosa, M.C.; Demmelmair, H.; Miranda, M.T.; Gil, A.; Decsi, T.; Koletzko, B.V. Effects of fish oil supplementation on the fatty acid profile in erythrocyte membrane and plasma phospholipids of pregnant women and their offspring: A randomised controlled trial. Br. J. Nutr. 2013, 109, 1647–1656. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  57. Davis-Bruno, K.; Tassinari, M.S. Essential fatty acid supplementation of DHA and ARA and effects on neurodevelopment across animal species: A review of the literature. Birth Defects Res. B Dev. Reprod. Toxicol. 2011, 92, 240–250. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  58. Friesen, R.W.; Innis, S.M. Linoleic acid is associated with lower long-chain n-6 and n-3 fatty acids in red blood cell lipids of Canadian pregnant women. Am. J. Clin. Nutr. 2010, 91, 23–31. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  59. Sellmayer, A.; Danesch, U.; Weber, P.C. Effects of different polyunsaturated fatty acids on growth-related early gene expression and cell growth. Lipids 1996, 31, S37–S40. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  60. Sellmayer, A.; Koletzko, B. Long-chain polyunsaturated fatty acids and eicosanoids in infants-physiological and pathophysiological aspects and open questions. Lipids 1999, 34, 199–205. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  61. Anjos, T.; Altmae, S.; Emmett, P.; Tiemeier, H.; Closa-Monasterolo, R.; Luque, V.; Wiseman, S.; Pérez-García, M.; Lattka, E.; Demmelmair, H.; et al. Nutrition and neurodevelopment in children: Focus on NUTRIMENTHE project. Eur. J. Nutr. 2013, 52, 1825–1842. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  62. Dunstan, J.A.; Simmer, K.; Dixon, G.; Prescott, S.L. Cognitive assessment of children at age 2(1/2) years after maternal fish oil supplementation in pregnancy: A randomised controlled trial. Arch. Dis. Child. Fetal Neonatal Ed. 2008, 93, F45–F50. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  63. Makrides, M.; Gibson, R.A.; McPhee, A.J.; Yelland, L.; Quinlivan, J.; Ryan, P. Effect of DHA supplementation during pregnancy on maternal depression and neurodevelopment of young children: A randomized controlled trial. JAMA 2010, 304, 1675–1683. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  64. Jacobson, J.L.; Jacobson, S.W.; Muckle, G.; Kaplan-Estrin, M.; Ayotte, P.; Dewailly, E. Beneficial effects of a polyunsaturated fatty acid on infant development: Evidence from the inuit of arctic Quebec. J. Pediatr. 2008, 152, 356–364. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  65. Figueroa, J.D.; Cordero, K.; Baldeosingh, K.; Torrado, A.I.; Walker, R.L.; Miranda, J.D.; León, M.D. Docosahexaenoic acid pretreatment confers protection and functional improvements after acute spinal cord injury in adult rats. J. Neurotrauma 2012, 29, 551–566. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  66. Haubner, L.; Sullivan, J.; Ashmeade, T.; Saste, M.; Wiener, D.; Carver, J. The effects of maternal dietary docosahexaenoic acid intake on rat pup myelin and the auditory startle response. Dev. Neurosci. 2007, 29, 460–467. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  67. Ward, R.E.; Huang, W.; Curran, O.E.; Priestley, J.V.; Michael-Titus, A.T. Docosahexaenoic acid prevents white matter damage after spinal cord injury. J. Neurotrauma 2010, 27, 1769–1780. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  68. Stillwell, W.; Wassall, S.R. Docosahexaenoic acid: Membrane properties of a unique fatty acid. Chem. Phys. Lipids 2003, 126, 1–27. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  69. Peters, B.D.; Machielsen, M.W.; Hoen, W.P.; Caan, M.W.; Malhotra, A.K.; Szeszko, P.R.; Duran, M.; Olabarriaga, S.D.; de Haan, L. Polyunsaturated fatty acid concentration predicts myelin integrity in early-phase psychosis. Schizophr. Bull. 2013, 39, 830–838. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  70. Koletzko, B.; Lien, E.; Agostoni, C.; Bohles, H.; Campoy, C.; Cetin, I.; Decsi, T.; Dudenhausen, J.W.; Dupont, C.; Forsyth, S.; et al. The roles of long-chain polyunsaturated fatty acids in pregnancy, lactation and infancy: Review of current knowledge and consensus recommendations. J. Perinat. Med. 2008, 36, 5–14. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
Figure 1. Flowchart of NUHEAL participants up to 9 years. FO: fish-oil; 5-MTHF: 5-methyltetrahydrofolate. a 4 participants who did not meet the inclusion criteria: 2 women weighed > 92 kg, 1 of whom used commercial FO preparations and 2 women regularly consumed FO preparations. b 41 participants did not complete the study: noncompliance (n = 2), relocation (n = 1), aversion to or bad taste of the supplement (n = 9), loss of contact (n = 2) and unknown reasons (n = 27). c 133 participants lost to follow up at 7.5 years: relocation (n = 3), loss of contact (n = 74), infants born prematurely (n = 4), congenital left-side anophthalmus (n = 1), craniosynostosis (n = 1), left-side deafness (n = 1), unwillingness to continue (n = 50). d Processing speed tasks were not entirely performed in 16 participants at 9 years.
Figure 1. Flowchart of NUHEAL participants up to 9 years. FO: fish-oil; 5-MTHF: 5-methyltetrahydrofolate. a 4 participants who did not meet the inclusion criteria: 2 women weighed > 92 kg, 1 of whom used commercial FO preparations and 2 women regularly consumed FO preparations. b 41 participants did not complete the study: noncompliance (n = 2), relocation (n = 1), aversion to or bad taste of the supplement (n = 9), loss of contact (n = 2) and unknown reasons (n = 27). c 133 participants lost to follow up at 7.5 years: relocation (n = 3), loss of contact (n = 74), infants born prematurely (n = 4), congenital left-side anophthalmus (n = 1), craniosynostosis (n = 1), left-side deafness (n = 1), unwillingness to continue (n = 50). d Processing speed tasks were not entirely performed in 16 participants at 9 years.
Nutrients 13 00131 g001
Figure 2. Effects of prenatal supplementation on CCTT-1 test in children at 7.5 and 9 years. Level of significance was obtained from one-way ANOVA. * Significant differences between 7.5 years and 9 years were observed in time spent (s) to solve the CCTT-1 test between children whose mothers were supplemented with FO (p = 0.0001), 5-MTHF (p = 0.0113) or both treatments (FO + 5MTHF) (p = 0.038) during pregnancy. 5-MTHF: 5-Methyltetrahydrofolate; CCTT-1: Children´s Color Trails Test; FO: fish-oil.
Figure 2. Effects of prenatal supplementation on CCTT-1 test in children at 7.5 and 9 years. Level of significance was obtained from one-way ANOVA. * Significant differences between 7.5 years and 9 years were observed in time spent (s) to solve the CCTT-1 test between children whose mothers were supplemented with FO (p = 0.0001), 5-MTHF (p = 0.0113) or both treatments (FO + 5MTHF) (p = 0.038) during pregnancy. 5-MTHF: 5-Methyltetrahydrofolate; CCTT-1: Children´s Color Trails Test; FO: fish-oil.
Nutrients 13 00131 g002
Table 1. General characteristics of the studied population.
Table 1. General characteristics of the studied population.
FO
(n = 38)
5-MTHF
(n = 29)
FO + 5-MTHF
(n = 36)
Placebo
(n = 40)
p
Maternal age (years)28.81 ± 5.2530.70 ± 5.6829.56 ± 4.3230.71 ± 3.900.231
BMI (kg/m2)
20 weeks26.03 ± 3.6224.92 ± 2.4525.28 ± 2.7724.74 ± 2.280.221
30 weeks28.52 ± 3.9126.87 ± 2.4327.23 ± 2.8626.91 ± 2.290.511
Country of origin 0.102
Spain28(73.7%)18(62%)26(72.2%)25(62.5%)
Germany10(24.3%)11(38%)10(27.8%)15(37.5%)
Hematocrit (%) at 30 weeks33.85 ± 3.7332.61 ± 5.1433.37 ± 2.5833.13 ± 2.750.662
Parity [n (%)] 0.521
023 (60.5%)23 (79.3%)28 (93.3%)28 (70%)
≥13 (7.9%)3 (12.5%)2 (5.5%)4 (10%)
Mode of Delivery 0.220
Spontaneous22(57.9%)15(51.7%)21(58.3%)18(45%)
Forceps2(5.2%)6(17.2%)2(5.5%)2(5%)
Vacuum0002(5%)
Cesarean section4(10.5%)2(6.9%)3(8.3%)6(15%)
Smoking in pregnancy
[Yes = n (%)]
7 (18.4%)5 (17.2%)7 (19.4%)4 (10%)0.251
Gravidity risk at 20 weeks [n (%)] 0.652
No risk factors6 (15.8%)6 (20.7%)8 (22.2%)11 (27.5%)
≥1 risk factors18 (47.4%)18 (62.1%)22 (61.1%)21 (52.5%)
High Maternal education [n (%)]8 (21.05%)8 (27.58%)14 (38.89%)10 (25%)0.332
Family Status Pregnancy [n (%)] 0.124
Single8 (21.1%)8 (27.4%)1 (2.7%)10 (25%)
Partnership23 (60.5%)23 (44.8%)29 (80.6%)29 (72.5%)
Gestational Age (weeks)38.90 ± 1.5138.75 ± 1.6238.73 ± 2.0639.43 ± 1.430.622
AA/DHA 10.90 ± 0.381.13 ± 0.540.97 ± 0.421.14 ± 0.630.098
Plasma Folate (µg/L) 16.17 ± 4.3312.10 ± 5.5513.46 ± 5.786.06 ± 0.82<0.001
tHcy (µmol/L) 17.08 ± 2.836.29 ± 2.816.93 ± 3.096.78 ± 2.390.952
Data are presented as n (%) for categorical data, and mean ± SDs for parametrically distributed data.1: Values obtained from mothers at delivery. p: level of significance from one-way ANOVA for continuous variables or χ2 test for categorical variables. Bold: p value < 0.05. Noted that there are missing values for some descriptive variables. 5-MTHF: 5-methyltetrahydrofolate; AA: arachidonic acid; DHA: docosahexaenoic acid; FO: fish-oil; tHcy: total homocysteine. n = number of cases.
Table 2. Descriptive analysis of processing speed evaluation in the NUHEAL children at 7.5 and 9 years.
Table 2. Descriptive analysis of processing speed evaluation in the NUHEAL children at 7.5 and 9 years.
FO5-MTHFFO + 5-MTHFPlacebop
Children at 7.5 YearsnMean ± SDnMean ± SDnMean ± SDnMean ± SD
SDMT—hits3623.56 ± 7.622822.61 ± 6.873422.38 ± 5.274124.66 ± 6.380.204
CCTT-1 (sc)36100.44 ± 39.272887.85 ± 30.053496.56 ± 34.484199.88 ± 50.770.017
STROOP Test—hits-1 (Word-reading)3759.49 ± 17.722861.82 ± 11.663457.68 ± 16.344159.34 ± 16.410.155
STROOP Test—hits-2 (Color-naming)3738.97 ± 7.772842.14 ± 7.263442.18 ± 9.364141.95 ± 8.410.523
Children at 9 years
SDMT—hits3331.42 ± 6.662630.00 ± 7.162830.25 ± 7.733733.76 ± 8.600.500
CCTT-1 (sc)3357.60 ± 17.132867.99 ± 23.162873.61 ± 24.733766.58 ± 25.990.112
STROOP Test—hits-1 (Word-Reading)3470.53 ± 12.692871.54 ± 10.292872.32 ± 8.713773.62 ± 9.650.187
STROOP Test—hits-2 (Color-Naming)3448.26 ± 7.582849.32 ± 9.492850.61 ± 8.573751.41 ± 7.940.627
p: level of significance obtained from one-way ANOVA test; Bold: p < 0.05. 5-MTHF: 5-methyltetrahydrofolate; CCTT-1: Children Color Trails Test; FO: fish-oil; SDMT: Symbol Digit Modalities Test; n = number of cases.
Table 3. Effect of selected cofounders on the processing speed outcomes in the NUHEAL children. Only significant associations are shown.
Table 3. Effect of selected cofounders on the processing speed outcomes in the NUHEAL children. Only significant associations are shown.
CategoriesbCI (95%)p
LCLUCL
Symbol Digit Modalities Test (SDMT) (Hits)
Time point7.5 years0---
9 years10.727.9813.47<0.001
dBMI (kg/m2) −0.63−1.25−0.020.043
AA/DHAratio 1,2 8.803.6113.990.001
tHcy (µmol/L) 1 −0.52−1.02−0.0020.049
Children Color Trails Test (CCTT−1) (sc)
Time point7.5 years0---
9 years−34.12−46.92−21.10<0.001
SmokingNo0---
Yes23.439.4337.440.001
Mode of deliverySpontaneous0---
Forceps−5.91−25.0713.240.545
Vacuum24.776.0143.530.010
Cesarean section2.66−9.1514.470.659
AA/DHAratio 1,2 −27.87−51.13−4.610.019
Word-Reading Stroop Test (WRST) (hits)
Time point7.5 years0---
9 years15.3711.2019.55<0.001
SexGirls0---
Boys−4.93−9.06−0.810.019
tHcy (µmol/L) 1 −1.09−2.04−0.140.025
Color-Naming Stroop Test (CNST) (hits)
Time point7.5 years
9 years10.006.8013.21<0.001
AA/DHA ratio 1,2 6.550.5012.600.034
1: values obtained from mothers at delivery; 2: fatty acids measured as % of total fatty acids. b: Regression coefficient; CI: Confidence Interval; LCL: lower confidence limit; UCL; upper confidence limit; p: level of significance obtained from mixed model analysis; dBMI: difference of body mass index between 20 and 30 weeks of pregnancy; AA: Arachidonic acid; DHA: Docosahexaenoic acid; tHcy: total homocysteine. Bold: p value < 0.05.
Table 4. Logistic regression analysis after dichotomizing children aged 9 years of age below or above P75, P50 or P25 for Symbol Digit Modalities Test (SDMT), Children Color Trails Test (CCTT-1) and STROOP Test. Only significant associations are shown.
Table 4. Logistic regression analysis after dichotomizing children aged 9 years of age below or above P75, P50 or P25 for Symbol Digit Modalities Test (SDMT), Children Color Trails Test (CCTT-1) and STROOP Test. Only significant associations are shown.
Maternal
AA/DHA
FADS1 rs174556Maternal
tHcy
Maternal High EducationMaternal
Age
12
SDMT—hits P75
Contrast
95% CI
p-value
30.46
(3.68, 252.0)
0.002
3.31
(1.28, 8.56)
0.014
1.43
(0.31, 6.55)
0.64
0.71
(0.56, 0.89)
0.003
3.63
(1.32, 9.98)
0.013
1.04
(0.94, 1.14)
0.446
CCTT-1 (sc) P25
Contrast
95% CI
p-value
18.53
(2.13, 160.9)
0.008
2.75
(1.02, 7.39)
0.045
4.95
(0.91, 27.07)
0.065
1.03
(0.85, 1.26)
0.733
2.03
(0.71, 5.77)
0.184
1.03
(0.93, 1.14)
0.587
STROOP Test—hits 1
(Word-reading) P50
Contrast
95% CI
p-value
3.73
(0.31, 44.28)
0.297
0.63
(0.19, 2.03)
0.439
1.67
(0.22, 12.85)
0.624
0.69
(0.51, 0.93)
0.015
1.47
(0.44, 4.85)
0.529
0.95
(0.84, 1.08)
0.440
STROOP Test—hits 2
(Color-naming) P75
Contrast
95% CI
p-value
21.70
(0.88, 534.97)
0.06
0.28
(0.07, 1.16)
0.08
0.67
(0.07, 6.48)
0.733
0.51
(0.32, 0.81)
0.005
0.69
(0.17, 2.76)
0.602
1.20
(1.01, 1.42)
0.039
p: level of significance obtained from logistic regression mixed model; Bold: p < 0.05. 1 = Heterozygous, 2 = Homozygous minor; AA: arachidonic acid; CCTT-1: Children Color Trails Test; DHA: Docosahexaenoic acid; FADS: Fatty Acid Desaturase; SDMT: Symbol Digit Modalities Test; tHcy: total homocysteine.
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Campoy, C.; Azaryah, H.; Torres-Espínola, F.J.; Martínez-Zaldívar, C.; García-Santos, J.A.; Demmelmair, H.; Haile, G.; Rzehak, P.; Koletzko, B.; Györei, E.; et al. Long-Chain Polyunsaturated Fatty Acids, Homocysteine at Birth and Fatty Acid Desaturase Gene Cluster Polymorphisms Are Associated with Children’s Processing Speed up to Age 9 Years. Nutrients 2021, 13, 131. https://doi.org/10.3390/nu13010131

AMA Style

Campoy C, Azaryah H, Torres-Espínola FJ, Martínez-Zaldívar C, García-Santos JA, Demmelmair H, Haile G, Rzehak P, Koletzko B, Györei E, et al. Long-Chain Polyunsaturated Fatty Acids, Homocysteine at Birth and Fatty Acid Desaturase Gene Cluster Polymorphisms Are Associated with Children’s Processing Speed up to Age 9 Years. Nutrients. 2021; 13(1):131. https://doi.org/10.3390/nu13010131

Chicago/Turabian Style

Campoy, Cristina, Hatim Azaryah, Francisco J. Torres-Espínola, Cristina Martínez-Zaldívar, José Antonio García-Santos, Hans Demmelmair, Gudrun Haile, Peter Rzehak, Berthold Koletzko, Eszter Györei, and et al. 2021. "Long-Chain Polyunsaturated Fatty Acids, Homocysteine at Birth and Fatty Acid Desaturase Gene Cluster Polymorphisms Are Associated with Children’s Processing Speed up to Age 9 Years" Nutrients 13, no. 1: 131. https://doi.org/10.3390/nu13010131

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop