Next Article in Journal
COVID-19 Pandemic-Induced Disruptions and Implications for National Food Security and Farm Incomes: Farm-Level Evidence from Indian Punjab
Previous Article in Journal
The Role of GARCH Effect on the Prediction of Air Pollution
 
 
Font Type:
Arial Georgia Verdana
Font Size:
Aa Aa Aa
Line Spacing:
Column Width:
Background:
Article

Sensory and Emotional Components in Tourist Memories of Wildlife Encounters: Intense, Detailed, and Long-Lasting Recollections of Individual Incidents

International Chair in Ecotourism Research, Griffith University, Gold Coast 4222, Australia
Sustainability 2022, 14(8), 4460; https://doi.org/10.3390/su14084460
Submission received: 20 March 2022 / Revised: 1 April 2022 / Accepted: 6 April 2022 / Published: 8 April 2022

Abstract

:
To quantify the role of senses and emotions in creating memorable tourism experiences, we need measurement frameworks that match how memories are created. This study examines that process through directed-content qualitative analysis of tourist encounters with wildlife. Data are derived from: interviews with 20 experienced wildlife tourism experts in 12 countries; 3000 social media posts on tourism enterprise and wildlife encounter websites; and participant observations and records of 168 memorable encounters involving >100 wildlife species, >850 tourists, and ~10,000 h in total, ranging over five decades. Across all data sources, senses and emotions differed between tourist interests and personalities, wildlife species and behaviours, and encounter circumstances. All senses were reported, with the most frequent being sight, followed by sound and smell, and, rarely, touch or taste. Descriptions were fine-grained and complex. The emotions reported were awe, joy, wonder, delight, thrill, amazement, envy, aww (cute-emotion), surprise, elation, satisfaction, interest, boredom, disappointment, sadness, embarrassment, concern, pity, distress, disgust, anxiety, shock, alarm, fear, and panic. Some experiences generated powerful recalls persisting for decades. Short-term, intense, and finely detailed senses and emotions defined experiences, created memories, and determined satisfaction, wellbeing, and subsequent outcomes. More accurate methods are needed to measure and characterise senses, emotions, and memories in tourism experience.

1. Introduction

Senses and emotions are influential components of tourism experiences [1,2,3] and wellbeing [4,5,6], but they are difficult to quantify. Recent approaches rely on broadly defined sensescapes [7,8] and emoscapes [9], which compress the complexity of real-life experiences into simplified numerical scales. The wildlife tourism sector [10] provides a natural experiment to test these approaches. Experience value and satisfaction depend on wildlife sightings, but sightings are not guaranteed [11,12,13,14]. Therefore, we can compile data from sightings and encounters that did occur and proved memorable, and examine how tourists recall their sensory and emotional components. This is relevant to the psychological analysis of tourist experience and the experiential marketing of tourism products.

2. Theoretical Framework

The theoretical framework adopted here is summarised in Figure 1. The tourism destination, attraction, and activity create sensory experiences; these, coupled with individual tourist personalities, create emotional experiences, which form memories, contributing to long-term wellbeing. This framework is a distillation from multiple previous models. It includes place and activity components from nature tourism research [15,16], sensory, emotional, and personality components from marketing research [17], memory components from research on memorable tourism experiences [18], and well-being components from research on tourism and wellbeing [19]. It treats short-term well-being perceptions within emotional components, and long-term well-being outcomes as derived from memories of experiences. It considers self-perceived qualitative well-being, rather than externally quantified mental health. By incorporating existing knowledge in a simple testable structure, Figure 1 complies with the criteria for strong psychological theories [20,21,22,23,24].
Place or destination, and personality and psychology, are longstanding aspects of tourism research [25] that need not be repeated here. Recent additions show specifically that destination wildlife diversity can contribute to wellbeing [26,27,28,29]. Senses have been analysed extensively in tourism contexts [7,8,30,31,32,33,34,35]. These analyses include sight [36,37], sound [38,39,40,41,42,43,44,45,46,47,48,49], smell [50,51,52], taste [53,54,55,56,57], and touch and temperature [58,59]. Emotions also play a central role in tourism experiences [60,61,62]. There are numerous theories of human emotions [63,64,65,66,67], including neurophysiological [68,69], psychological [70], evolutionary [71], behavioural [72,73], social [74], cultural [75], and linguistic [76]. Emotions may be associated with either immediate or recollected sensory impressions [77,78]. Emotions reported in tourism include awe [79,80,81], fear, thrill, and triumph [82,83], as well as joy, amazement, sadness, concern, and empathy [84]. In wildlife tourism, emotions have been reported during encounters with whales [85], pandas [86], wolves [87], bears [88], and penguins [89]. None of these analysed the longevity of emotions post-encounter [10,90].
Memorable experiences are key components of tourism [18]. Memories are strongest for critical incidents associated with powerful emotions [77,78,91,92,93]. Powerful emotions can be experienced in less than a second but recalled lifelong [82,94,95,96,97]. The well-being effects of tourism are widely studied [4,5,19,25,98,99]. In particular, they include significant relief and recovery from stress, both via commercial tourism products [16,100,101,102] and independent outdoor recreation [103,104,105], especially in parks and nature [106,107,108,109,110,111]. The well-being effects of vacation experiences fade out over periods of weeks, months, or years [3,101,112,113,114,115,116,117,118,119,120,121]. The well-being effects of wildlife tourism have been examined specifically [122].

3. Materials & Methods

3.1. Multi-Source Directed-Content Qualitative Analysis

The overall method adopted was a directed-content analysis [123,124], using multiple data sources in parallel, each with different data compression approaches. The focus was on the sensory and emotional factors that contributed most strongly to memorable wildlife tourism experiences. The data sources were interviews with expert practitioners, netnographic analysis of online content, and participant observation and ethnography. Interviews and netnography yield written text content, amenable to standard thematic analysis under an interpretivist paradigm. Participant observation and ethnography rely on spoken communications and unspoken observations from critical incidents, providing data for qualitative meta-analysis of cases [125].

3.2. Expert Interviews and Netnography

The interviews were carried out during 2020–2022, in person or electronically, with 20 experienced wildlife tourists from 12 countries. They were semi-structured, and catalysed by asking interviewees to describe their earliest memorable wildlife encounters. Data included species, circumstances, year, age, senses, emotions, and memories. Each person described 1–3 encounters, ~50 in all. These also yielded data on intensity, wildlife species, worldview effects, and the early age of first memorable encounters. Netnographic data were obtained from public social media accounts of (a) wildlife tourism enterprises worldwide, following an approach adopted by earlier analysts [13], and (b) wildlife-watching, identification, and appreciation groups on social media, which provided additional information on highly memorable encounters. Data-rich posts in comment threads were extracted, ignoring short data-poor text or emoji comments. Searches were stopped after 3000 posts were scanned.

3.3. Participant Observation, Ethnography, and Autoethnography of Encounter Incidents

Participant observation [126], used widely in outdoor tourism research, is an ethnographic method where the researcher is a full member of the group under study, indistinguishable by ethnicity, language, wealth, or behaviour. In participant observation, the researcher (a) earns recognition within the group through extended membership, capability, shared experiences, and mutual obligations past and present, and (b) experiences events and communications as a peer participant within the group. The method includes ethnographic components through observations and communications with other participants, and autoethnographic components through observations of one’s own actions and emotions [82,96], including flashbulb memories [127] of critical incidents [128,129].
This study includes participant observation of wildlife encounters during commercial wildlife tours over several decades (Supplementary Tables S1 and S2). Encounters were recorded in notebooks, photographs, and video, including audio of spoken reactions. These records were used: (a) to document the longevity of individual memories; (b) to trigger encounter memories that can then be recalled and reexperienced mentally [82]; and (c) to confirm emotions experienced by other tourists, as identified at the time from spoken language, voice tone, non-verbal vocalisations, facial expressions, and body language [74]. Tourists were close together, quiet, and still, in a vehicle, boat, or on foot, and often discussed their sensory and emotional experiences of individual wildlife sightings. Observations did not alter any encounter, for either wildlife or tourists. Data, observations, and analyses were replicated across individuals, species, ecosystems, and methodologies.

4. Results

4.1. Interviews

Interviewees reported encounters from Australia, Brazil, Chile, France, India, Kenya, Mexico, Nepal, Norway, USA, Venezuela, and Vietnam. Strong sensory and emotional memories were reported from up to 60 years ago, when the respondents were aged from 4 years old (2 cases) upwards. Several respondents emphasised the level of detail in their memories: “I remember everything as if it were yesterday”. Some reported encounters involving little emotion: “I’m not a very emotional person”. Some reported interest, e.g., for tiger or armadillo, or pleasure at finding a long-sought species, e.g., rare species of monkey or wild cat, or feelings of connectedness to nature. Some reported awe tinged with fear, e.g., of ostrich or grizzly bear at close range, or awe mixed with joy, e.g., for whale sharks. Many reported various levels of excitement, delight, and joy. Emotions were felt not only for larger wildlife, but also for frogs, smaller mammals such as chipmunks, and birds such as blue jays and toucans. One person reported that surfing with dolphins generated an extremely intense level of happiness or bliss, so that they felt blessed by the experience.

4.2. Netnography

Comment threads on the social media accounts of commercial wildlife tourism enterprises used terms such as “breathtaking”, “awesome”, “amazing”, “stunning”, “magnificent”, “splendid”, “beautiful” and “gorgeous”, with longevity indicated by phrases such as “forever engraved on my mind”. One described a hippopotamus as “funny, incredible, and terrifying at the same time”. Another described “the sound of pure nature when a megapod [of common dolphins] is on the move”. Comment threads on broader wildlife-related social media accounts provided many examples of powerful recollections. One person, describing a childhood experience, wrote that a red fox sat so close to her that “I could see the sunlight in each individual hair … red fur seemed to glow from the inside out. I was in awe. Dumbstruck. I’ll never forget it”. [130]. Describing close encounters with moose in Alaska and Norway respectively, individuals wrote that they were “scared” with their “heart pounding” but would remember the encounter “like it was yesterday”, even at age 95. Descriptions included smell, e.g., of skunk, and especially sound: “You can feel the bass notes rumbling in your cells”; “love hearing bighorn sheep butt those horns”; “an otter screaming”. Overall, these posts show that some wildlife encounters have strong sensory and emotional components, can be highly memorable for decades, and contribute to well-being.

4.3. Participant Observation and Ethnography

Participant observations included 168 incidents, involving >850 individual tourists and >100 wildlife species, from ~10,000 h as clients or guides in >100 wildlife tours from 1970–2019, across all continents and oceans. Individual incidents lasted from seconds to minutes, with an aggregate duration of ~10 h, <0.1% of the overall period. Senses and emotions could be recognised in <1 s. Encounters are listed and described in Supplementary Tables S1 and S2. Sensory and emotional components are summarised in Table 1 and Table 2.
Some sensory experiences can be recalled in considerable detail. Sights include colours, patterns, shapes, and movements. Sounds include intensities, frequencies, and recognisable origins. Scents, smells, and tastes differ by type and intensity, and many are identifiable to particular sources. Individuals respond differently to specific smells and tastes, and also to touch, temperature, and humidity. Sensory experiences can be very complex and detailed. An elephant seen at a far distance, for example, generates a very different set of sensory experiences than an elephant encountered on foot, within trunk range, with direct eye contact. Different senses are dominant for different memorable experiences. Some experiences involve the interactions of multiple senses [52,131]. Wildlife tourists react very differently, depending on how they interpret the significance of sensory impressions: e.g., as an immediate threat, or a long-sought goal. People from different cultural backgrounds may react differently to the same sensory experiences [132,133,134]. Many sensory experiences have emotional connotations [62], considered below.
Over 20 identifiable named emotions were recorded. This is several times more than reported previously from wildlife tourism [84], but only about a quarter of the overall set of named emotions [76]. Valences included both positive and negative, and intensities ranged from very low to very high. Several emotions, even of different valence, were sometimes experienced simultaneously. Experiences generating negative emotions are nonetheless valued by many wildlife tourists. Individuals can experience: very powerful positive emotions such as awe, wonder and joy; moderately powerful positive emotions such as thrill, delight, elation, and aww or cute-emotion; and weaker positive emotions such as enjoyment. They can experience: weakly negative emotions such as disappointment or sadness; moderately negative emotions such as pity, embarrassment or concern; and powerful negative emotions such as distress, disgust or fear. Some emotions reflect principally the circumstances of the observers themselves; others, the observers’ attitudes towards the animals under observation. Different tourists at the same wildlife sighting may experience different emotions.

4.4. Memories

Recollections with intense sensory and emotional components can persist for at least five or six decades. This matches the findings from the interviews and netnography. Written records show that there were also many unemotional sightings that no longer exist as memories. Where encounters are still recalled, the memories commonly contain very high levels of detail, sufficient for the individual concerned to mentally re-experience the encounter. This matches an analysis of adventure tourism [82]. As with that study, memories of wildlife encounters may include complete mental replay, including the physiological re-creation of emotions experienced at the time, or they may include the unemotional recollection of past senses and emotions experienced at the time, without re-experiencing them.

5. Discussion

5.1. Principal Findings

Wildlife tourism generates detailed and powerful sensory and emotional experiences that remain memorable for many decades. Such recollections of wildlife encounters have been identified in popular literature for well over a century. One example from over a century ago [135] mentioned hunting tour guides in the Indian Himalayas, describing decades-earlier encounters with ibex, serow, and markhor as “every detail lighted from behind like twigs on tree-tops seen against lightning”. This is the principal finding here. The degree to which past wildlife tourism may contribute to current wellbeing and life satisfaction, and hence to future repeat bookings, depends on fine detail and the intensity of memories from specific encounters, not broad aggregate sensescapes or emoscapes. This matches recent analyses of well-being outcomes from holiday experiences more generally [3,112].
The second principal finding is that experiences can differ considerably between tourists. Different circumstances can generate different responses, and different individuals may have different responses to the same encounters, or similar responses but for different reasons. For example, one may be happy to see a bird because it is beautiful, another because it is rare, and another because it adds to their lifetime checklist. Responses may have cultural components: particular species may be seen as cute or ugly, or lucky or unlucky, and perceptions of animal sentience, pain, and cruelty differ considerably between cultures. Responses also depend on what individual animals are doing. An inactive animal may generate elation if it is long-sought and indifference if it is not, whereas the same species in a more active mode may generate fear, awe, or delight. Wildlife tourism enterprises and guides choreograph encounters to create memorable sensory and emotional experiences, whilst sparing them from shock or panic. In doing so, they also focus on fine detail, aiming to create encounters with individual animals engaged in particular behaviours.

5.2. Research Priorities

First, given the key importance of fine sensory and emotional detail in memorable wildlife tourism experiences, the most immediate research priority is to seek techniques to quantify individual senses and emotions in fine detail. This is needed for sounds [38,39,40,41,42,43,44,45,46,47,48,49,136], scents [50,51,52,137], touch [59], and taste [53,54,55,56,57], as well as sight. It is also needed in analysing emotions, using fine linguistic distinctions rather than broad valence-intensity approaches. Broad “scape” approaches, e.g., as proposed for experiential marketing [7], are too imprecise for the psychological analysis of individual tourist experiences. Second, we can compare factors creating differences between individuals. We can compare the effects on experiences, memories, and well-being, of: animal species and behaviours [138]; circumstances of encounters; and the personality and experiences of individual tourists [139]. Such differences are also likely to prove important in increasingly customised marketing through the digital matching of clients, products, and destinations [16,55,140].
Third, we could quantify the effects of intense and memorable wildlife tourism experiences on mental health parameters that can be converted to health service values [107] and hence to economic valuations, e.g., of the role of wildlife tourism product design and wildlife tourism guide skills in engineering-specific wildlife encounters for clients. Fourth, we could search for an underlying psychological model for these links. As a testable hypothesis, perhaps intense positive memories create a buffer that protects net self-perceived well-being against negative experiences, and individual memories continue to contribute to that buffer as long as they can be recollected, even decades later. This hypothesis applies well beyond tourism, but we could use tourism to analyse it by testing links from short-term responses to medium-term stress recovery and long-term worldview change. If supported, it would add to the general frameworks of human psychology. Recent neurological research has shown that mental resilience is physically expressed in long-lasting brain structures that change as a result of individual experiences [141].

5.3. Practical Implications in Mental Health and Tourism Marketing

The COVID-19 pandemic has greatly extended the incidence of poor mental health worldwide, from ~13% of the populations in developed nations [142] to 40–50% [143,144,145,146]. Poor mental health imposes substantial economic costs [147]. Countries are seeking low-cost measures for mental health recovery, as components of broader social and economic recovery. Where tourism can improve well-being, it has a role to play in promoting mental health. The results outlined here indicate that wildlife tourism, in particular, certainly does have such an effect. Therefore, tourism portfolios, enterprises, and advocates can lobby for the promotion of wildlife tourism as one means of post-pandemic mental health recovery, with associated social and economic benefits [107,148]. Wildlife tourism enterprises, and tourism enterprises more generally [15,98,106,149,150,151,152], can enhance the effects of their products on client satisfaction and well-being through design and guide choreography to create close and memorable encounters with intense sensory and emotional experiences [18,55,153]. As retail applications of virtual reality become increasingly commonplace [140], tourism enterprises and destinations can also improve the effectiveness of experiential marketing through more effective use of sensory and emotional components.

Supplementary Materials

The following supporting information can be downloaded at: https://www.mdpi.com/article/10.3390/su14084460/s1, Table S1: List of Wildlife Encounters; Table S2: Descriptions of Encounters and Emotions Experienced. Reference [154] is cited in Supplementary Materials.

Funding

This research received no external funding.

Institutional Review Board Statement

Collection: the re-use and analysis of data for this research were compliant with University Human Ethics Research requirements and procedures, as well as the National Codes of Conduct for Human Ethics in Research. There were no inducements, interventions, interference, or identification of any observed persons, or behaviour modifications. Individuals are not identifiable by name, role, image, geotag, other digital identifiers, or place/time coordinates.

Informed Consent Statement

Informed consent was obtained from all identifiable subjects involved in the study.

Data Availability Statement

Acknowledgments

The participant observation component included commercial wildlife tours. Some of these were sponsored by the enterprises concerned.

Conflicts of Interest

The author declares no conflict of interest.

References

  1. Prebensen, N.K.; Chen, J.S.; Uysal, M. (Eds.) Creating Experience Value in Tourism, 2nd ed.; CABI: Wallingford, UK, 2018. [Google Scholar]
  2. Stienmetz, J.; Kim, J.J.; Xiang, Z.; Fesenmaier, D.R. Managing the structure of tourism experiences: Foundations for tourism design. J. Destin. Mark. Manag. 2021, 19, 100408. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  3. Strijbosch, W.; Mitas, O.; van Blaricum, T.; Vugts, O.; Govers, C.; Hover, M.; Gelissen, J.; Bastiaansen, M. When the parts of the sum are greater than the whole: Assessing the peak-and-end-theory for a heterogeneous, multi-episodic tourism experience. J. Destin. Mark. Manag. 2021, 20, 100607. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  4. Smith, M.K.; Diekmann, A. Tourism and wellbeing. Ann. Tour. Res. 2017, 66, 1–13. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  5. Uysal, M.; Berbekova, A.; Kim, H. Designing for quality of life. Ann. Tour. Res. 2020, 83, 102944. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  6. Buckley, R.C. Tourism and mental health: Foundations, frameworks and futures. J. Trav. Res. 2022. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  7. Buzova, D.; Sanz-Blas, S.; Cervera-Taulet, A. “Sensing” the destination: Development of the destination sensescape index. Tour. Manag. 2021, 87, 104362. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  8. Kah, J.A.; Shin, H.J.; Lee, S.H. Traveler sensoryscape experiences and the formation of destination identity. Tour. Geogr. 2020. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  9. Shakeela, A.; Weaver, D. Resident reactions to a tourism incident: Mapping a Maldivian emoscape. Ann. Tour. Res. 2012, 39, 1337–1358. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  10. Hosey, G.; Melfi, V. (Eds.) Anthrozoology: Human-Animal Interactions in Domesticated and Wild Animals; University Press: Oxford, UK, 2019. [Google Scholar]
  11. Dybsand, H.N.H. In the absence of a main attraction: Perspectives from polar bear watching tourism participants. Tour. Manag. 2020, 79, 104097. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  12. Buckley, R.C.; Mossaz, A.C. Decision making by specialist luxury travel agents. Tour. Manag. 2016, 55, 133–138. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  13. Buckley, R.C.; Mossaz, A.C. Private conservation funding from wildlife tourism enterprises in sub-Saharan Africa: Conservation marketing beliefs and practices. Biol. Cons. 2018, 218, 57–63. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  14. Buckley, R.C.; Cooper, M.A. Conservation of a threatened mammal species through domestic tourism in Chile. Front. Ecol. Environ. 2019, 17, 557–558. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  15. Buckley, R.; Zhong, L.; Martin, S. Mental health key to tourism infrastructure in China’s new megapark. Tour. Manag. 2021, 82, 104169. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  16. Cooper, M.A.; Buckley, R.C. Tourist mental health drives destination choice, marketing, and matching. J. Travel Res. 2021, 61, 00472875211011548. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  17. Krishna, A. (Ed.) Sensory Marketing; Routledge: London, UK, 2011. [Google Scholar]
  18. Tung, V.W.S.; Ritchie, J.B. Exploring the essence of memorable tourism experiences. Ann. Tour. Res. 2011, 38, 1367–1386. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  19. Lengieza, M.L.; Hunt, C.A.; Swim, J.K. Measuring eudaimonic travel experiences. Ann. Tour. Res. 2019, 74, 195–197. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  20. Borsboom, D.; van der Maas, H.; Dalege, J.; Kievit, R.; Haig, B. Theory construction methodology: A practical framework for theory formation in psychology. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 2021, 16, 756–766. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  21. Eronen, M.I.; Bringmann, L.F. The theory crisis in psychology: How to move forward. Perspect. Psychol. Sci. 2021, 16, 779–788. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  22. Fried, E.I. Theories and models: What they are, what they are for, and what they are about. Psychol. Inq. 2020, 31, 336–344. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  23. Smaldino, P.E. How to build a strong theoretical foundation. Psychol. Inq. 2020, 31, 336–344. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  24. Thagard, P. Conceptual Revolutions; University Press: Princeton, NJ, USA, 2018. [Google Scholar]
  25. Berbekova, A.; Uysal, M.; Assaf, A.G. Toward an assessment of quality-of-life indicators as measures of destination performance. J. Travel Res. 2021, 00472875211026755. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  26. Cameron, R.W.; Brindley, P.; Mears, M.; McEwan, K.; Ferguson, F.; Sheffield, D.; Jorgensen, A.; Riley, J.; Goodrick, J.; Ballard, L.; et al. Where the wild things are! Do urban green spaces with greater avian biodiversity promote more positive emotions in humans? Urban Ecosyst. 2020, 23, 301–317. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  27. Gonçalves, P.; Grilo, F.; Mendes, R.C.; Vierikko, K.; Elands, B.; Marques, T.A.; Santos-Reis, M. What’s biodiversity got to do with it? Perceptions of biodiversity and restorativeness in urban parks. Ecol. Soc. 2021, 26, 25. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  28. Marselle, M.R.; Hartig, T.; Cox, D.T.; de Bell, S.; Knapp, S.; Lindley, S.; Triguero-Mas, M.; Böhning-Gaese, K.; Braubach, M.; Cook, P.A.; et al. Pathways linking biodiversity to human health: A conceptual framework. Environ. Int. 2021, 150, 106420. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  29. Methorst, J.; Rehdanz, K.; Mueller, T.; Hansjürgens, B.; Bonn, A.; Böhning-Gaese, K. The importance of species diversity for human well-being in Europe. Ecol. Econ. 2021, 181, 106917. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  30. Agapito, D. The senses in tourism design: A bibliometric review. Ann. Tour. Res. 2020, 83, 102934. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  31. Kastenholz, E.; Marques, C.P.; Carneiro, M.J. Place attachment through sensory-rich, emotion-generating place experiences in rural tourism. J. Destin. Mark. Manag. 2020, 17, 100455. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  32. Kim, J.; Fesenmaier, D. Measuring human senses and the touristic experience: Methods and applications. In Analytics in Smart Tourism Design; Xiang, Z., Fesenmaier, D., Eds.; Springer: New York, NY, USA, 2017; pp. 47–63. [Google Scholar]
  33. Lv, X.; McCabe, S. Expanding theory of tourists’ destination loyalty: The role of sensory impressions. Tour. Manag. 2020, 77, 104026. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  34. Mehraliyev, F.; Kirilenko, A.P.; Choi, Y. From measurement scale to sentiment scale: Examining the effect of sensory experiences on online review rating behavior. Tour. Manag. 2020, 79, 104096. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  35. Pan, S.; Ryan, C. Tourism sense-making: The role of the senses and travel journalism. J. Travel Tour. Mark. 2009, 26, 625–639. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  36. Llobera, M. Extending GIS-based visual analysis: The concept of visualscapes. Int. J. Geogr. Inf. Sci. 2003, 17, 25–48. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  37. Urry, J.; Larsen, J. The Tourist Gaze 3.0; Sage: London, UK, 2011. [Google Scholar]
  38. Echeverri, A.; Karp, D.S.; Frishkoff, L.O.; Krishnan, J.; Naidoo, R.; Zhao, J.; Zook, J.; Chan, K.M. Avian cultural services peak in tropical wet forests. Conserv. Lett. 2021, 14, e12763. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  39. Ednie, A.; Gale, T.; Beeftink, K.; Adiego, A. Connecting protected area visitor experiences, wellness motivations, and soundscape perceptions in Chilean Patagonia. J. Leis. Res. 2020. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  40. Ferraro, D.M.; Miller, Z.D.; Ferguson, L.A.; Taff, B.D.; Barber, J.R.; Newman, P.; Francis, C.D. The phantom chorus: Birdsong boosts human well-being in protected areas. Proc. R. Soc. B 2020, 287, 20201811. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  41. Fisher, J.C.; Irvine, K.N.; Bicknell, J.E.; Hayes, W.M.; Fernandes, D.; Mistry, J.; Davies, Z.G. Perceived biodiversity, sound, naturalness and safety enhance the restorative quality and wellbeing benefits of green and blue space in a neotropical city. Sci. Total Environ. 2021, 755, 143095. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  42. Francomano, D.; Rodríguez González, M.I.; Valenzuela, A.E.J.; Ma, Z.; Raya Rey, A.N.; Anderson, C.B.; Pijanowski, B.C. Human-nature connection and soundscape perception: Insights from Tierra del Fuego, Argentina. J. Nat. Conserv. 2022, 65, 126110. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  43. Gale, T.; Ednie, A.; Beeftink, K.; Adiego, A. Beyond noise management: Exploring visitors’ perceptions of positive emotional soundscape dimensions. J. Leis. Res. 2021, 52, 129–153. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  44. He, M.; Li, J.; Li, J.; Chen, H. A comparative study on the effect of soundscape and landscape on tourism experience. Int. J. Tour. Res. 2019, 21, 11–22. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  45. Kankhuni, Z.; Ngwira, C. Overland tourists’ natural soundscape perceptions: Influences on experience, satisfaction, and electronic word-of-mouth. Tour. Recreat. Res. 2021. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  46. Liu, A.; Wang, X.; Liu, F.; Yao, C.; Deng, Z. Soundscape and its influence on tourist satisfaction. Serv. Ind. J. 2018, 38, 164–181. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  47. Lu, Y.H.; Zhang, J.; Zhang, H.; Xiao, X.; Liu, P.; Zhuang, M.; Hu, M. Flow in soundscape: The conceptualization of soundscape flow experience and its relationship with soundscape perception and behaviour intention in tourism destinations. Curr. Issues Tour. 2021. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  48. Morrison, C.A.; Auniņš, A.; Benkő, Z.; Brotons, L.; Chodkiewicz, T.; Chylarecki, P.; Escandell, V.; Eskildsen, D.P.; Gamero, A.; Herrando, S.; et al. Bird population declines and species turnover are changing the acoustic properties of spring soundscapes. Nat. Commun. 2021, 12, 6217. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  49. Qiu, M.; Zhang, J.; Zhang, H.; Zheng, C. Is looking always more important than listening in tourist experience? J. Travel Tour. Mark. 2018, 35, 869–881. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  50. Dann, G.; Jacobsen, J. Tourism smellscapes. Tour. Geogr. 2003, 5, 3–25. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  51. Jiang, J.; Zhang, J.; Zhang, H.; Yan, B. Natural soundscapes and tourist loyalty to nature-based tourism destinations: The mediating effect of tourist satisfaction. J. Travel Tour. Mark. 2018, 35, 218–230. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  52. Xiao, J.; Tait, M.; Kang, J. Understanding smellscapes: Sense-making of smell-triggered emotions in place. Emot. Space Soc. 2020, 37, 100710. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  53. Björk, P.; Kauppinen-Räisänen, H. Destination foodscape: A stage for travelers’ food experience. Tour. Manag. 2019, 71, 466–475. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  54. Chen, H.T.; Lin, Y.T. A study of the relationships among sensory experience, emotion, and buying behavior in coffeehouse chains. Serv. Bus. 2018, 12, 551–573. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  55. Goolaup, S.; Nunkoo, R. Reconceptualizing tourists’ extraordinary experiences. J. Travel Res. 2021, 00472875211064632. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  56. Park, E.; Kim, S.; Xu, M. Hunger for learning or tasting? An exploratory study of food tourist motivations visiting food museum restaurants. Tour. Recreat. Res. 2020, 47, 130–144. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  57. Su, X.; Zhang, H. Tea drinking and the tastescapes of wellbeing in tourism. Tour. Geogr. 2020. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  58. Denstadli, J.M.; Jacobsen, J.K.S.; Lohmann, M. Tourist perceptions of summer weather in Scandinavia. Ann. Tour. Res. 2011, 38, 920–940. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  59. Rickard, S.C.; White, M.P. Barefoot walking, nature connectedness and psychological restoration: The importance of stimulating the sense of touch for feeling closer to the natural world. Landsc. Res. 2021, 46, 975–991. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  60. Hosany, S.; Martin, D.; Woodside, A.G. Emotions in tourism: Theoretical designs, measurements, analytics, and interpretations. J. Trav. Res. 2020, 60, 1391–1407. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  61. Volo, S. The experience of emotion: Directions for tourism design. Ann. Tour. Res. 2021, 86, 103097. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  62. Wang, L.; Hou, Y.S.; Chen, Z.X. Are rich and diverse emotions beneficial? The impact of emodiversity on tourists’ experiences. J. Travel Res. 2021, 60, 1085–1103. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  63. Barrett, L.F.; Mesquita, B.; Ochsner, K.N.; Gross, J.J. The experience of emotion. Ann. Rev. Psychol. 2007, 58, 373–403. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  64. Ekman, P. Emotion in the Human Face, 2nd ed.; University Press: Cambridge, UK, 1982. [Google Scholar]
  65. Izard, C.E. Emotion theory and research: Highlights, unanswered questions, and emerging issues. Ann. Rev. Psychol. 2009, 60, 1–25. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  66. Moors, A. Theories of emotion causation: A review. Cogn. Emot. 2009, 23, 625–662. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  67. Xu, X.; Kang, C.; Sword, K.; Guo, T. Are emotions abstract or concrete? Exp. Psychol. 2017, 64, 315–324. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  68. Pessoa, L. Understanding emotion with brain networks. Curr. Opin. Behav. Sci. 2018, 19, 19–25. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  69. Spunt, R.P.; Adolphs, R. The neuroscience of understanding the emotions of others. Neurosci. Lett. 2019, 693, 44–48. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  70. De Castella, K.; Platow, M.J.; Tamir, M.; Gross, J.J. Beliefs about emotion: Implications for avoidance-based emotion regulation and psychological health. Cogn. Emot. 2017, 32, 773–795. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  71. Frijda, N.H. The evolutionary emergence of what we call “emotions”. Cogn. Emot. 2016, 30, 609–620. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  72. Hommel, B.; Moors, A.; Sander, D.; Deonna, J. Emotion meets action: Towards an integration of research and theory. Emot. Rev. 2017, 9, 295–298. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  73. Ridderinkhof, K.R. Emotion in action: A predictive processing perspective and theoretical synthesis. Emot. Rev. 2017, 9, 319–325. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  74. Niedenthal, P.M.; Rychlowska, M.; Wood, A. Feelings and contexts: Socioecological influences on the nonverbal expression of emotion. Curr. Opin. Psychol. 2017, 17, 170–175. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  75. Gendron, M. Revisiting diversity: Cultural variation reveals the constructed nature of emotion perception. Curr. Opin. Psychol. 2017, 17, 145–150. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  76. Johnson-Laird, P.N.; Oatley, K. The language of emotions: An analysis of a semantic field. Cogn. Emot. 1989, 3, 81–123. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  77. Chai, W.J.; Abd Hamid, A.I.; Abdullah, J.M. Working memory from the psychological and neurosciences perspectives: A review. Front. Psychol. 2018, 9, 401. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  78. Van der Stigchel, S. An embodied account of visual working memory. Vis. Cogn. 2020, 28, 414–419. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  79. Coghlan, A.; Buckley, R.; Weaver, D. A framework for analysing awe in tourism experiences. Ann. Tour. Res. 2012, 39, 1710–1714. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  80. Pearce, J.; Strickland-Munro, J.; Moore, S.A. What fosters awe-inspiring experiences in nature-based tourism destinations? J. Sustain. Tour. 2017, 25, 362–378. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  81. Wang, L.; Lyu, J. Inspiring awe through tourism and its consequence. Ann. Tour. Res. 2019, 77, 106–116. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  82. Buckley, R.C. Qualitative analysis of emotions: Fear and thrill. Front. Psychol. 2016, 7, 1187. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  83. Hetland, A.; Kjelstrup, E.; Mittner, M.; Vittersø, J. The thrill of speedy descents: A pilot study on differences in facially expressed online emotions and retrospective measures of emotions during a downhill mountain-bike descent. Front. Psychol. 2019, 10, 566. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  84. Ballantyne, R.; Packer, J.; Sutherland, L.A. Visitors’ memories of wildlife tourism: Implications for the design of powerful interpretive experiences. Tour. Manag. 2011, 32, 770–779. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  85. Higham, J.; Bejder, L.; Williams, R. (Eds.) Whale-Watching: Sustainable Tourism and Ecological Management; University Press: Cambridge, UK, 2014. [Google Scholar]
  86. Cong, L.; Wu, B.; Morrison, A.M.; Shu, H.; Wang, M. Analysis of wildlife tourism experiences with endangered species: An exploratory study of encounters with giant pandas in Chengdu, China. Tour. Manag. 2014, 40, 300–310. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  87. Montag, J.M.; Patterson, M.E.; Freimund, W.A. The wolf viewing experience in the Lamar Valley of Yellowstone National Park. Hum. Dimens. Wildl. 2005, 10, 273–284. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  88. Skibins, J.C.; Sharp, R.L. Binge watching bears: Efficacy of real vs. virtual flagship exposure. J. Ecotourism 2019, 18, 152–164. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  89. Schänzel, H.A.; McIntosh, A.J. An insight into the personal and emotive context of wildlife viewing at the Penguin Place, Otago Peninsula, New Zealand. J. Sustain. Tour. 2000, 8, 36–52. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  90. McIntosh, D.; Wright, P.A. Emotional processing as an important part of the wildlife viewing experience. J. Outdoor Recreat. Tour. 2017, 18, 1–9. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  91. Klinzing, J.G.; Niethard, N.; Born, J. Mechanisms of systems memory consolidation during sleep. Nat. Neurosci. 2019, 22, 1598–1610. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  92. Michaelian, K.; Sutton, J. Memory. In The Stanford Encyclopedia of Philosophy Archive; Zalta, E.N., Ed.; Stanford University: Stanford, CA, USA, 2017; Available online: https://plato.stanford.edu/archives/sum2017/entries/memory (accessed on 20 March 2022).
  93. Zhang, H.; Fell, J.; Axmacher, N. Electrophysiological mechanisms of human memory consolidation. Nat. Commun. 2018, 9, 1–11. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  94. Brymer, E.; Schweitzer, R. Phenomenology and the Extreme Sport Experience; Taylor & Francis: Abingdon, UK, 2017. [Google Scholar]
  95. Iyadurai, L.; Visser, R.M.; Lau-Zhu, A.; Porcheret, K.; Horsch, A.; Holmes, E.A.; James, E.L. Intrusive memories of trauma: A target for research bridging cognitive science and its clinical application. Clin. Psychol. Rev. 2019, 69, 67–82. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  96. Rubin, D.C.; Berntsen, D.; Deffler, S.A.; Brodar, K. Self-narrative focus in autobiographical events: The effect of time, emotion, and individual differences. Mem. Cogn. 2019, 47, 63–75. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  97. Sheldon, S.; Fenerci, C.; Gurguryan, L. A neurocognitive perspective on the forms and functions of autobiographical memory retrieval. Front. Syst. Neurosci. 2019, 13, 4. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  98. Buckley, R.C. Therapeutic mental health effects perceived by outdoor tourists: A large-scale, multi-decade, qualitative analysis. Ann. Tour. Res. 2019, 77, 164–167. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  99. Su, L.; Tang, B.; Nawijn, J. Eudaimonic and hedonic well-being pattern changes: Intensity and activity. Ann. Tour. Res. 2020, 84, 103008. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  100. Chen, C.C.; Petrick, J.F.; Shahvali, M. Tourism experiences as a stress reliever: Examining the effects of tourism recovery experiences on life satisfaction. J. Travel Res. 2016, 55, 150–160. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  101. Chen, C.C.; Huang, W.J.; Petrick, J.F. Holiday recovery experiences, tourism satisfaction and life satisfaction-is there a relationship? Tour. Manag. 2016, 53, 140–147. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  102. Chen, C.C.; Zou, S.; Gao, J. Towards the recovery mechanisms of leisure travel experiences: Does the length of vacation matter? J. Travel Tour. Mark. 2020, 37, 636–648. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  103. Bielinis, E.; Łukowski, A.; Omelan, A.; Boiko, S.; Takayama, N.; Grebner, D.L. The effect of recreation in a snow-covered forest environment on the psychological wellbeing of young adults: Randomized controlled study. Forests 2019, 10, 827. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  104. Buckley, R.C.; Westaway, D. Mental health rescue effects of women’s outdoor tourism: A role in COVID-19 recovery. Ann. Tour. Res. 2020, 85, 103041. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  105. Svensson, M.; Brundin, L.; Erhardt, S.; Madaj, Z.; Hållmarker, U.; James, S.; Deierborg, T. Long distance ski racing is associated with lower long-term incidence of depression in a population based, large-scale study. Psychiatry Res. 2019, 281, 112546. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  106. Buckley, R.C. Nature tourism and mental health: Parks, happiness, and causation. J. Sustain. Tour. 2020, 28, 1409–1424. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  107. Buckley, R.C.; Brough, P.; Hague, L.; Chauvenet, A.; Fleming, C.; Roche, E.; Sofija, E.; Harris, N. Economic value of protected areas via visitor mental health. Nat. Commun. 2019, 10, 5005. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  108. Buckley, R.C.; Brough, P. Mental health: Set up long-term cohort studies. Nature 2021, 595, 352. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  109. Tester-Jones, M.; White, M.P.; Elliott, L.R.; Weinstein, N.; Grellier, J.; Economou, T.; Bratman, G.N.; Cleary, A.; Gascon, M.; Korpela, K.M.; et al. Results from an 18-country cross-sectional study examining experiences of nature for people with common mental health disorders. Sci. Rep. 2020, 10, 19408. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  110. Tomasso, L.P.; Yin, J.; Cedeño Laurent, J.G.; Chen, J.T.; Catalano, P.J.; Spengler, J.D. The relationship between nature deprivation and individual wellbeing across urban gradients under COVID-19. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2021, 18, 1511. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  111. White, M.P.; Elliott, L.R.; Grellier, J.; Economou, T.; Bell, S.; Bratman, G.N.; Cirach, M.; Gascon, M.; Lima, M.L.; Lõhmus, M.; et al. Associations between green/blue spaces and mental health across 18 countries. Sci. Rep. 2021, 11, 8903. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  112. Chark, R.; King, B.; Tang, C.M.F. The journey from episodes to evaluations: How travelers arrive at summary evaluations. J. Travel Res. 2022, 61, 265–278. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  113. De Bloom, J.; Geurts, S.A.; Kompier, M.A. Vacation (after-) effects on employee health and well-being, and the role of vacation activities, experiences and sleep. J. Happiness Stud. 2013, 14, 613–633. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  114. Gump, B.B.; Hruska, B.; Pressman, S.D.; Park, A.; Bendinskas, K.G. Vacation’s lingering benefits, but only for those with low stress jobs. Psychol. Health 2021, 36, 895–912. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  115. Kroesen, M.; Handy, S. The influence of holiday-taking on affect and contentment. Ann. Tour. Res. 2014, 45, 89–101. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  116. Mitas, O.; Kroesen, M. Vacations over the years: A cross-lagged panel analysis of tourism experiences and subjective well-being in the Netherlands. J. Happiness Stud. 2020, 21, 2807–2826. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  117. Packer, J. Taking a break: Exploring the restorative benefits of short breaks and vacations. Ann. Tour. Res. Empir. Insights 2021, 2, 100006. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  118. Sonnentag, S.; Cheng, B.H.; Parker, S.L. Recovery from work: Advancing the field toward the future. Annu. Rev. Organ. Psychol. Organ. Behav. 2022, 9, 33–60. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  119. Sousa, C.; Gonçalves, G. Back to work bang! Difficulties, emotions and adjustment strategies when returning to work after a vacation. Int. J. Hum. Resour. Manag. 2021, 32, 2242–2263. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  120. Syrek, C.J.; Weigelt, O.; Kühnel, J.; de Bloom, J. All I want for Christmas is recovery: Changes in employee affective well-being before and after vacation. Work Stress 2018, 32, 313–333. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  121. Yu, J.; Smale, B.; Xiao, H. Examining the change in wellbeing following a holiday. Tour. Manag. 2021, 87, 104367. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  122. Lee, D.J.; Kruger, S.; Whang, M.J.; Uysal, M.; Sirgy, M.J. Validating a customer well-being index related to natural wildlife tourism. Tour. Manag. 2014, 45, 171–180. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  123. Assarroudi, A.; Heshmati Nabavi, F.; Armat, M.R.; Ebadi, A.; Vaismoradi, M. Directed qualitative content analysis: The description and elaboration of its underpinning methods and data analysis process. J. Res. Nurs. 2018, 23, 42–55. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  124. Ebrahimi Belil, F.; Alhani, F.; Ebadi, A.; Kazemnejad, A. Self-efficacy of people with chronic conditions: A qualitative directed content analysis. J. Clin. Med. 2018, 7, 411. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  125. Thiri, M.A.; Villamayor-Tomás, S.; Scheidel, A.; Demaria, F. How social movements contribute to staying within the global carbon budget: Evidence from a qualitative meta-analysis of case studies. Ecol. Econ. 2022, 195, 107356. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  126. Spradley, J. Participant Observation; Holt, Rinehart and Winston: New York, NY, USA, 1980. [Google Scholar]
  127. Hirst, W.; Phelps, E.A. Flashbulb memories. Curr. Dir. Psychol. Sci. 2016, 25, 36–41. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  128. Flanagan, J.C. The critical incident technique. Psychol. Bull. 1954, 51, 327–358. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  129. Loftus, E.F. Eavesdropping on memory. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 2017, 68, 1–18. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  130. Stone, E. Untitled Post. Available online: https://cheezburger.com/16325637/the-wildest-and-funniest-encounters-people-had-with-wild-animals (accessed on 20 March 2022).
  131. Xiong, J.; Hashim, N.H.; Murphy, J. Multisensory image as a component of destination image. Tour. Manag. Perspect. 2015, 14, 34–41. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  132. Duff, H.; Vignoles, V.L.; Becker, M.; Milfont, T.L. Self-construals and environmental values in 55 cultures. J. Environ. Psychol. 2021, 79, 101722. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  133. Lwin, M.O.; Wijaya, M. Do scents evoke the same feelings across cultures? In Sensory Marketing; Krishna, A., Ed.; Routledge: London, UK, 2011; pp. 109–121. [Google Scholar]
  134. Majid, A.; Roberts, S.G.; Cilissen, L.; Emmorey, K.; Nicodemus, B.; O’Grady, L.; Woll, B.; LeLan, B.; de Sousa, H.; Cansler, B.L.; et al. Differential coding of perception in the world’s languages. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA 2018, 115, 11369–11376. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed] [Green Version]
  135. Kipling, R. Kim; Macmillan: London, UK, 1901; p. 297. [Google Scholar]
  136. Smalley, A.J.; White, M.P.; Ripley, R.; Atack, T.X.; Lomas, E.; Sharples, M.; Coates, P.A.; Groom, N.; Grand, A.; Heneberry, A.; et al. Forest 404: Using a BBC drama series to explore the impact of nature’s changing soundscapes on human wellbeing and behavior. Glob. Environ. Chang. 2022, 102497. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  137. Madzharov, A.V. Scents research and its applications in tourism. Ann. Tour. Res. 2021, 93, 103309. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  138. Wyles, K.J.; White, M.P.; Hattam, C.; Pahl, S.; King, H.; Austen, M. Are some natural environments more psychologically beneficial than others? The importance of type and quality on connectedness to nature and psychological restoration. Environ. Behav. 2019, 51, 111–143. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  139. Wang, R.A.H.; Nelson-Coffey, S.K.; Layous, K.; Jacobs Bao, K.; Davis, O.S.; Haworth, C.M. Moderators of wellbeing interventions: Why do some people respond more positively than others? PLoS ONE 2017, 12, e0187601. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  140. Köchling, A. Experiential marketing as a tool to enhance tourists’ pre-travel online destination experiences? A web-based experiment. J. Destin. Mark. Manag. 2021, 22, 100669. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  141. Park, H.R.; Quidé, Y.; Schofield, P.R.; Williams, L.M.; Gatt, J.M. Grey matter covariation and the role of emotion reappraisal in mental wellbeing and resilience after early life stress exposure. Transl. Psychiatry 2022, 12, 85. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  142. Vos, T.; Lim, S.S.; Abbafati, C.; Abbas, K.M.; Abbasi, M.; Abbasifard, M.; Abbasi-Kangevari, M.; Abbastabar, H.; Abd-Allah, F.; Abdelalim, A.; et al. Global burden of 369 diseases and injuries in 204 countries and territories, 1990–2019: A systematic analysis for the Global Burden of Disease Study 2019. Lancet 2020, 396, 1204–1222. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  143. Brülhart, M.; Klotzbücher, V.; Lalive, R.; Reich, S.K. Mental health concerns during the COVID-19 pandemic as revealed by helpline calls. Nature 2021, 600, 121–126. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  144. Burkova, V.N.; Butovskaya, M.L.; Randall, A.K.; Fedenok, J.N.; Ahmadi, K.; Alghraibeh, A.M.; Allami, F.B.M.; Alpaslan, F.S.; Al-Zu’bi, M.A.A.; Biçer, D.F.; et al. Predictors of anxiety in the COVID-19 pandemic from a global perspective: Data from 23 countries. Sustainability 2021, 13, 4017. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  145. Petersen, M.W.; Dantoft, T.M.; Jensen, J.S.; Pedersen, H.F.; Frostholm, L.; Benros, M.E.; Carstensen, T.B.W.; Ørnbøl, E.; Fink, P. The impact of the COVID-19 pandemic on mental and physical health in Denmark—A longitudinal population-based study before and during the first wave. BMC Public Health 2021, 21, 1418. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  146. Santomauro, D.F.; Herrera, A.M.M.; Shadid, J.; Zheng, P.; Ashbaugh, C.; Pigott, D.M.; Abbafati, C.; Adolph, C.; Amlag, J.O.; Aravkin, A.Y.; et al. Global prevalence and burden of depressive and anxiety disorders in 204 countries and territories in 2020 due to the COVID-19 pandemic. Lancet 2021, 398, 1700–1712. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  147. McDaid, D.; Park, A.L.; Wahlbeck, K. The economic case for the prevention of mental illness. Annu. Rev. Public Health 2019, 40, 373–389. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  148. Hakulinen, C.; Elovainio, M.; Arffman, M.; Lumme, S.; Pirkola, S.; Keskimäki, I.; Manderbacka, K.; Böckerman, P. Mental disorders and long-term labour market outcomes: Nationwide cohort study of 2 055 720 individuals. Acta Psychiatr. Scand. 2019, 140, 371–381. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [PubMed]
  149. Marselle, M.R.; Warber, S.L.; Irvine, K.N. Growing resilience through interaction with nature: Can group walks in nature buffer the effects of stressful life events on mental health? Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health 2019, 16, 986. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
  150. Derose, K.P.; Wallace, D.D.; Han, B.; Cohen, D.A. Effects of park-based interventions on health-related outcomes: A systematic review. Prev. Med. 2021, 147, 106528. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  151. Jordan, E.J.; Spencer, D.M.; Prayag, G. Tourism impacts, emotions and stress. Ann. Tour. Res. 2019, 75, 213–226. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  152. Zins, A.H.; Ponocny, I. On the importance of leisure travel for psychosocial wellbeing. Ann. Tour. Res. 2022, 93, 103378. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  153. Kim, Y.; Ribeiro, M.A.; Li, G. Tourism memory, mood repair and behavioural intention. Ann. Tour. Res. 2022, 93, 103369. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef]
  154. Buckley, R.C. Aww: The emotion of perceiving cuteness. Front. Psychol. 2016, 7, 1740. [Google Scholar] [CrossRef] [Green Version]
Figure 1. Theoretical framework: principal linkages.
Figure 1. Theoretical framework: principal linkages.
Sustainability 14 04460 g001
Table 1. Sensory experiences during wildlife tourism encounters.
Table 1. Sensory experiences during wildlife tourism encounters.
SenseExamples from Wildlife Tourism Encounters
SightColour: plum-coloured starling, lion or tiger eye, iridescent mantle of clam or octopus, distinctive colours of parrots or nudibranchs
Shape: identifying features, e.g., of bird, gazelle, or shark species
Movement: sunlight rippling on whale shark skin, wheeling bird flocks and fish schools, undulating manta ray fins, muscles moving on a bear
SoundBirdsong, e.g., Arctic loon, skylark, curlew, Australian magpie; speeding wings, e.g., cockatoos; howls, e.g., wolves, coyotes; trumpeting elephant
SmellCharacteristic scents, e.g., striped possum; disgusting odours, e.g., rotting hippopotamus carcass; powerfully pungent smells, e.g., stifling airborne ammonia in bat colonies; acrid identifying scents, e.g., large carnivores
Touch and temperatureFur or feathers, e.g., possum or goose; wool, e.g., sheep; hide, e.g., rhinoceros; scales, e.g., snake or fish; shells, e.g., crustacea or molluscs; spines, e.g., sea-urchin or echidna; slime, e.g., eel; jelly, e.g., frog eggs or jellyfish; hot or cold bird feet
TasteRarely involved in wildlife tourism, except consumptive hunting, fishing and foraging, e.g., for rock oysters or mopane worms
Table 2. Emotions experienced during wildlife tourism encounters.
Table 2. Emotions experienced during wildlife tourism encounters.
EmotionsExamples from Wildlife Tourism Encounters
AweElephant, rhino, tiger, whales, flocks of birds, schools of fish
WonderExceptional behaviours, e.g., narwhal crossing horns
ThrillWhale breaching near boat, elephant trumpeting close by
JoySurfing with dolphins, virtuoso birdsong, very close views
DelightBird courtship displays, sand-swimming lizards
Amazement, envyAcrobatic bird flight, marine mammal swimming
Aww/cuteGalago, sugar glider, newborn mammals, bird chicks
ElationPygmy seahorse, aardwolf, numbat, rare bird species
InterestObservations of rarely-seen animal behaviours
Sadness, pityAnimal injury or death, abandoned juveniles, e.g., from predation
DistressJuveniles suffering violent predation, parent animals powerless
EmbarrassmentMating behaviours, especially large mammals
DisgustAnimals eating rotting carcasses, nasal mucus, vomit etc
ShockClose views of dismemberment, e.g., during predation
Concern, anxietyCircumstances potentially putting observers at risk
AlarmAnimal behaviours requiring observers’ immediate safety response
Fear, panicActual or threatened attack, e.g., elephant, monkeys, sharks, leopard
Publisher’s Note: MDPI stays neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Share and Cite

MDPI and ACS Style

Buckley, R.C. Sensory and Emotional Components in Tourist Memories of Wildlife Encounters: Intense, Detailed, and Long-Lasting Recollections of Individual Incidents. Sustainability 2022, 14, 4460. https://doi.org/10.3390/su14084460

AMA Style

Buckley RC. Sensory and Emotional Components in Tourist Memories of Wildlife Encounters: Intense, Detailed, and Long-Lasting Recollections of Individual Incidents. Sustainability. 2022; 14(8):4460. https://doi.org/10.3390/su14084460

Chicago/Turabian Style

Buckley, Ralf C. 2022. "Sensory and Emotional Components in Tourist Memories of Wildlife Encounters: Intense, Detailed, and Long-Lasting Recollections of Individual Incidents" Sustainability 14, no. 8: 4460. https://doi.org/10.3390/su14084460

Note that from the first issue of 2016, this journal uses article numbers instead of page numbers. See further details here.

Article Metrics

Back to TopTop